ラコステ(Lacoste)アリーゼ・コルネ USオープン着用モデル テニスドレス

advertisement
Discourse, Diversity and Distinction:
Macromarketing at 30 Years
Special Sub-theme:
“Remember the Titans!”
THE 30
TH
Papers of
ANNUAL MACROMARKETING CONFERENCE
St. Petersburg, Florida, USA
May 28 – May 31, 2005
Program Chair:
Mark Peterson, University of Texas at Arlington
Arrangements Chair:
Ronald Paul Hill, University of South Florida, St. Petersburg
Sponsored by:
The Macromarketing Society, Inc.
The University of South Florida, St. Petersburg
&
The Journal of Macromarketing
i
Editor’s Notes:
Special thanks to the track chairs who have played such an instrumental role in fielding this
year’s program. Their ideas, vision and execution have contributed invaluably to the creation of
a memorable program for the thirtieth anniversary of the Macromarketing Conference.
Track
Australasian Macro Issues
Chair
Ben Wooliscroft
Affiliation
University of Otago, NZ
Competition & Markets
Bill Redmond
Bowling Green State University
Development & Marketing
Tony Pecotich
University of Western Australia
Environment & Marketing
Andrea Prothero
Pierre McDonagh
University College Dublin
Dublin Institute of Technology
Ethics & Distributive Justice
Deirdre Shaw
Glasgow Caledonian University
Faith, Meaning & Value
John Mittelstaedt
Clemson University
Families & Marketing
Jim Gentry
University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Food & Agribusiness
Tim Burkink
University of Nebraska-Kearney
Globalization
Don Rahtz
College of William & Mary
Quality-of-Life
Dong-Jin Lee
Yonsei University, Korea
Teaching Macromarketing
Bill Kilbourne
Clemson University
Technology
Nik Dholakia
University of Rhode Island
Vulnerable Consumers
Ron Hill
University of South Florida,
St. Petersburg
Macromarketing Potpourri
Roger Dickinson
University of Texas at Arlington
Special thanks are also in order for John Mittelstaedt, the treasurer of the Macromarketing Society for
mounting and maintaining this year’s conference website, as well as taking registrations for conference
attendees. Additionally, the College of Business at the University of South Florida, St. Petersburg shared
the talents of both Dean Ron Hill and his assistant Debbie Janes in preparing the arrangements for this
year’s conference.
Remember the Titans!
"If I have seen further [than certain other men] it is by standing upon the
shoulders of giants."
Isaac Newton (1642–1727),
Speaking of his dependency on Galileo’s
and Kepler’s previous work in physics and
astronomy.
ii
This year’s special sub-theme calls macromarketers to reflect upon the founders of
macromarketing scholarship on whose shoulders we now stand. Without those who dared to
consider the interplay of marketing and society, many marketing scholars today would
undoubtedly not see much further than the “bottom line” of business practice. Some of these
titans follow - our deceased titans are presented first.
Johan Arndt, (deceased) – contributed to advanced political economy analysis.
Jim Carman, (deceased) - contributed to externalities analysis and transition economy
development.
Stan Hollander, (deceased) - the father of the history of marketing thought.
Charles Slater, (deceased) - the father of both the macromarketing conferences and JMM,
and influential in Marketing and development.
Roger Dickinson, (continuous player) – encouraged and edited the Review and
Communications section of JMM since 1993. Roger’s scholarship has focused
upon the structure of marketing and the Robinson-Patman Act.
Don Dixon, (continuous up to 2000) – contributed to marketing history.
George Fisk, (continuous player) - first editor of JMM and a guiding innovative thinker.
George contributed work in systems approaches and societal provisioning framing.
Chuck Goeldner, (continuous player) - published the JMM before Sage began publishing JMM
and took a real organizational interest. Supported many early macromarketing
conferences.
Shelby Hunt - helped frame the field in the 1980’s.
Tom Klein, (continuous player) - contributed ethics, religion, and externality thrusts.
Roger Layton, (continuous player) - developed trade flows analysis of the national
accounts, especially in developing situations.
Bob Mittelstaedt, (continuous player) – contributed to better understanding of
interorganizational dynamics including government.
Bob Nason, (continuous player) - JMM editor for ten years and general glue for
macromarketing. Bob’s macromarketing scholarship has focused upon
externalities analysis and framing.
Stan Shapiro, (continuous player) - second editor of JMM and a guiding organizer who
contributed in the environmental/ conserver society area.
iii
Remember the Editors!
The editors of the Journal of Macromarketing have done more to advance macromarketing than
any other group of individuals. The commitment of their talents, and time have resulted in the
journal becoming a very respected outlet for research on the interplay of marketing and society.
In 1998, Koojaroenprsit et al ranked the Journal of Macromarketing as one of the top ten
journals in marketing.
George Fisk
1981 Volume 1 No. 1
1983 Volume 3 No. 2
Stanley J. Shapiro
1984 Volume 4 No. 1
1987 Volume 7 No. 1
Robert W. Nason
1987 Volume 7 No. 2
1997 Volume 17 No. 2
Luis V. Dominguez
1998 Volume 18 No. 1
2000 Volume 20 No. 2
Sanford Grossbart
2001 Volume 21 No. 1
2003 Volume 23 No. 2
Clifford J. Schultz II
2004 Volume 24 No. 1
present
Koojaroenprasit, Narong, Art Weinstein, William C. Johnson and David O. Remington,
1998. “Marketing Journal Rankings Revisited: Research Findings and Academic
Implications, Marketing Education Review”, 8, 1 (Spring), 95-102.
iv
Table of Contents
Editor’s Notes
ii
Table of Contents
v
Program Schedule
ix
Remember the Titans!
Beyond Their Imagination: What the Titans Thought and What is Happening Now
A. Coskun Samli
1
Panel: History of Macromarketing
George Fisk, Tom Klein, Roger Layton,
Robert A. Mittelstaedt, Stanley J. Shapiro
Moderator: M. Joseph Sirgy
2
Food & Agribusiness
Misuse of the Marketing Concept:
The Marketing of Seemingly Healthy Packaged Food
William H. Redmond,
Consumer Attitudes Toward Organic Foods:
A Cross-Cultural Examination of U.S. and Irish Market Segments
Julie Stanton, Renee Hughner, Pierre McDonagh, and Andrea Prothero
3
11
Genetically Modified Food: A Cross-Cultural Investigation
Timothy J. Burkink, Marko Grunhagen, Klervi N. Le Marre, Gary J. Wells, and Carl L. Witte
17
Relationships Among Individual, Institutional, and System Level Public Trust:
Consumer Evaluations of Food Safety
Ahmet Ekici and Linda L. Price
26
Families & Marketing
What Marketing Does to Family
James W. Gentry and Stacey M. Baker
30
Father Involvement: Who's Your Daddy and Why Marketers Care
Kyle A. Huggins
37
The Effect of Household Decision Behavior on Retailing Structure
of the Transitional Country
Vesna Brcic-Stipcevic and Sanda Renko
45
Living on the Edge: Financial Services for the Lower Socio-Economic Strata
Lisa Szykman, Don R. Rahtz, Michael Plater and Greg Goodwin
59
v
Special Session: Do We Really Care?
Consumer Vulnerability: What Do We REALLY Mean?
Stacey M. Baker, James W. Gentry and Terri L. Rittenburg
63
The Good, the Bad, and the Ugly of Media Portrayals of "Model Minorities
Charles R. Taylor, Stacy Landreth, and Hae-Kyong Bang
When Does a Dollar Buy Less than a Dollar's Worth?
Racial Profiling and Marketplace Discrimination
Anne-Marie Harris, Geraldine Henderson, Jerome Williams
If We Can Feed Surfers, Why not the Poor?
The Moral Case for Universal Consumption Adequacy
Ron Hill
Australasian Macro Issues
Australian Industry Responses to Current Ethical Dilemmas –
Fast Cars and Fast Food
Sandra C. Jones
What Does it Take to Get Published in a Top Marketing Journal
from Australasia?
Daniela Rosenstreich and Ben Wooliscroft
64
73
Environmental Consumerism: Recycling Behavior and Its Impact
on Intentions to Purchase Green Products
Clare D’Souza, Mehdi Taghian and Peter Lamb
85
Are Negative Reactions to Sexist Appeals in Alcohol Advertisements
a Function of Feminism or Gender?
Sandra C. Jones
96
Ethics & Distributive Justice
A Dynamic Approach to International Codes of Ethics
Göran Svensson, and Greg Wood
104
Localize Global Codes of Ethics
Göran Svensson, and Greg Wood
112
Consumer's Moral Sensitivity: Building a Scale
Samar Baqer
118
Compulsive Buying and Credit Card Use among Young Adults
Tânia Modesto Veludo-de-Oliveira, Ana Akemi Ikeda and Rubens da Costa Santos
138
vi
Longitudes & Latitudes of Macromarketing
Incentives for Growth, 1005 and 2005
James M. Carman
The Impact of Trade Globalization on Quality of Life
M. Joseph Sirgy, Dong-Jin Lee, Chad Miller, and James E. Littlefield
Publication Patterns in Journals Dedicated to Issues of Marketing and Society:
Benchmarking and Analysis
Michael J. Polonsky and John Mittelstaedt
Panel: Thought and Comment on Future Scenarios for Macromarketing
Mark Peterson, Sandy Grossbart, Bill Kilbourne
Environment & Marketing
Macro-micro Linking Using System Dynamics Modeling:
A Preliminary Examiniation of Eco-Labeling Effects for Farmed Shrimp
Steven P. Arquitt and T. Bettina Cornwell
Globalization: For Better or Worse? The Effects of International Trade
on the Economy, Physical Quality of Life and the Environment
Selima Ben Mrad, Hongwen She and Michael Mullen
149
155
169
181
182
193
Death Saliency and Environmentally Friendly Consumer Behavior
Bertrand Urien
208
'Plant a tree, save the world' – Hobson’s Choice or an Environmental Citizen?
John Connolly and Andrea Prothero
223
Quality-of-Life
Consumer Well-Being, Income, Religious Attitudes and Their Relationship
to Selected Measures of Life Satisfaction Among Professionals
Gerald M. Hampton and Marco Wolf
Introducing the Rasch Model as an Alternative Measurement Paradigm
When Investigating Quality of Life
Alexandra Ganglmair-Wooliscroft and Rob Lawson
Native People and Quality of Life: A Macromarketing Perspective
Alf H. Walle
Opportunities for Marketing Practice and Scholarship
Does Society Consider Marketing to Be a Science2?
Philip Barnes and Ben Wooliscroft
Panel: The Future of Marketing
Robert W. Nason, John O'Shaughnessy, Clifford J. Shultz II and William L. Wilkie
237
245
256
270
286
vii
Competition and Markets
Social Capital and Co-Production in the Marketplace
Susie Pryor and Sandy Grossbart
287
Ants, Brand Loyalty, Variety Seeking, Market Topography,
and Aggregate Adaptive Optimization
Ben Wooliscroft and Tim Molteno
296
Faith, Meaning and Value
Early Twenty-First Century Worldviews and Macromarketing
Mark Peterson and Stephen J. Terry
299
Macromarketing Implications of Religious Fractionalization
and Religious Affiliation
John D. Mittlestaedt and Robert A. Mittelstaedt
309
Development & Marketing
Women and Development in Uganda
Terri L. Rittenburg
317
Relationship Marketing - Competitive Strategy or Destruction of the Market?
Anthony Pecotich and Russel Kingshott
328
Macromarketing and the Concept of Openness: Toward a Research Agenda
Anthony Pecotich and Kenneth Beng Yap
337
National Development, Knowledge Transfer and Marketing Intermediaries in China
Paul D. Ellis, Anthony Pecotich, and Zhuang Guijun
349
Micro to Macro: Innovations
An Application of the Marketing Management Process to the Socio-political System
Hiroshi Kosaka
356
Strategic Factors Affecting the Success Rate of Online Grocers:
Is the US Finally Ready for Online Grocery Shopping?
Rosa Lemel
361
Everett Rogers and the Influence of Diffusion Research in the Developing World
Marilyn Liebrenz-Himes
365
Micro to Macro: Transition Economies
‘Balkan Bricolage’: Consumption Symbolism in Socialist and Post-Socialist Croatia
Katherine C. Sredl and Natasa Renko
366
The Emerging Consumer Society of Southeast China and
the Growing Divide in China's Transforming Economy
Shawn Strange
373
Interdependent Selves and Retail Decision Making with Companions:
The Case of Chinese Mall Patrons
Jay Wu
385
viii
30th Annual Macromarketing Conference 2005
Program Schedule May 28 – May 31, 2005
Hilton St. Petersburg, Florida
"Discourse, Diversity and Distinction: Macromarketing at 30 Years"
Special Sub-theme:
“Remember the Titans!”
Saturday, May 28th, 2005:
Arrival and Early Bird Reception
3:00 pm - 6:00 pm
6:00 pm - 9:00 pm
Registration I
Registration II
Harborview
Bayboro
7:00 pm – 9:00 pm
Early Bird Reception
Bayboro
ix
Sunday, May 29th, 2005:
8:00 am – 8:30 am
8:00 am – 8:30 am
8:30 am – 10:00 am
Full Conference Day
Registration III
Continental Breakfast
Harborview
Harborview
Plenary Session
Harborview
Mark Peterson
Remember the Titans!
1. Ron Hill
Welcome and Conference Overview
2. A. Coskun Samli,
"Beyond Their Imagination: What the Titans Thought and What is Happening Now"
3. George Fisk, Tom Klein, Roger Layton, Robert A. Mittelstaedt, Stanley J. Shapiro
Moderator: M. Joseph Sirgy
Panel: History of Macromarketing
10:00 am – 10:30 am
Break
10:30 am – 12:00 noon
Concurrent Sessions
Tim Burkink
Food & Agribusiness
Harborview
1. William H. Redmond,
“Misuse of the Marketing Concept: The Marketing of Seemingly Healthy Packaged Food”
2. Julie Stanton, Renee Hughner, Pierre McDonagh, and Andrea Prothero,
“Consumer Attitudes Toward Organic Foods: A Cross-Cultural Examination of U.S.
and Irish Market Segments”
3. Timothy J. Burkink, Marko Grunhagen, Klervi N. Le Marre, Gary J. Wells, and Carl L. Witte,
“Genetically Modified Food: A Cross-Cultural Investigation”
4. Ahmet Ekici and Linda L. Price,
“Relationships Among Individual, Institutional, and System Level Public Trust:
Consumer Evaluations of Food Safety
Jim Gentry
Families & Marketing
1. James W. Gentry and Stacey M. Baker,
"What Marketing Does to Family"
Williams
2. Kyle A. Huggins,
"Father Involvement: Who's Your Daddy and Why Marketers Care"
3. Vesna Brcic-Stipcevic and Sanda Renko,
"The Effect of Household Decision Behavior on Retailing Structure of the Transitional
Country"
4. Lisa Szykman, Don R. Rahtz, Michael Plater and Greg Goodwin,
"Living on the Edge: Financial Services for the Lower Socio-Economic Strata"
12:00 noon – 1:30 pm
x
Lunch
Bayboro
Sunday, May 29th, 2005: (continued)
1:30 pm – 3:00 pm
Plenary Session
Harborview
Ron Hill and Debra Stephens
Do We Really Care?
1. Stacey M. Baker, James W. Gentry and Terri L. Rittenburg,
"Consumer Vulnerability: What Do We REALLY Mean?
2. Charles R. Taylor, Stacy Landreth, and Hae-Kyong Bang,
"The Good, the Bad, and the Ugly of Media Portrayals of "Model Minorities"
3. Anne-Marie Harris, Geraldine Henderson, Jerome Williams,
"When Does a Dollar Buy Less than a Dollar's Worth? Racial Profiling and Marketplace
Discrimination"
4. Ron Hill,
"If We Can Feed Surfers, Why not the Poor? The Moral Case for Universal Consumption
Adequacy"
3:00 pm – 3:30 pm
Break
3:30 pm – 5:00 pm
Concurrent Sessions
Ben Wooliscroft
Australasian Macro Issues
Harborview
1. Sandra C. Jones
“Australian Industry Responses to Current Ethical Dilemmas - Fast Cars and Fast Food”
2. Daniela Rosenstreich and Ben Wooliscroft
“What Does it Take to Get Published in a Top Marketing Journal from Australasia?”
3. Clare D’Souza, Mehdi Taghian and Peter Lamb
“Environmental Consumerism: Recycling Behaviour and Its Impact on Intentions to
Purchase Green Products”
4. Sandra C. Jones
“Are Negative Reactions to Sexist Appeals in Alcohol Advertisements a Function
of Feminism or Gender?”
Mary Carsky
Ethics & Distributive Justice
1. Göran Svensson, and Greg Wood
"A Dynamic Approach to International Codes of Ethics"
Williams
2. Göran Svensson, and Greg Wood
"Localize Global Codes of Ethics"
3. Samar Baqer
"Consumer's Moral Sensitivity: Building a Scale"
4. Tânia Modesto Veludo-de-Oliveira, Ana Akemi Ikeda and Rubens da Costa Santos,
"Compulsive Buying and Credit Card Use among Young Adults"
6:00 pm
6:30 pm – 9:00 pm
7:30 pm – 8:30 pm
9:30 pm
Depart Hilton lobby
Reception at Salvador Dalí Museum
Docent-led tour of Dalí Museum
Return to Hilton St. Petersburg
xi
Monday, May 30th, 2005:
8:00 am – 8:30 am
8:30 am – 10:00 am
Full Conference Day
Continental Breakfast
Harborview
Plenary Session
Harborview
Ahmet Ekici
Longitudes & Latitudes
of Macromarketing
1. James M. Carman (presented by Roger Layton),
"Incentives for Growth, 1005 and 2005"
2. M. Joseph Sirgy, Dong-Jin Lee, Chad Miller, and James E. Littlefield,
"The Impact of Trade Globalization on Quality of Life"
3. Michael J. Polonsky and John Mittelstaedt ,
"Publication Patterns in Journals Dedicated to Issues of Marketing and Society:
Benchmarking and Analysis”
4. Mark Peterson, Sandy Grossbart, Bill Kilbourne
Panel: Thought and Comment on Future Scenarios for Macromarketing
10:00 am – 10:30 am
Break
10:30 am – 12:00 noon
Concurrent Sessions
Andrea Prothero
Environment & Marketing
Harborview
1. Steven P. Arquitt and T. Bettina Cornwell,
"Macro-micro Linking Using System Dynamics Modeling: A Preliminary Examination
of Eco-Labeling Effects for Farmed Shrimp”
2. Selima Ben Mrad, Hongwen She and Michael Mullen,
"Globalization: For Better or Worse? The Effects of International Trade on the Economy,
Physical Quality of Life and the Environment”
3. Bertrand Urien,
"Death Saliency and Environmentally Friendly Consumer Behavior"
4. John Connolly and Andrea Prothero,
"'Plant a tree, save the world' – Hobson’s Choice or an Environmental Citizen?"
Dong-Jin Lee
Quality-of-Life
Williams
1. Gerald M. Hampton and Marco Wolf,
"Consumer Well-Being, Income, Religious Attitudes and Their Relationship to Selected
Measures of Life Satisfaction Among Professionals"
2. Alexandra Ganglmair-Wooliscroft and Rob Lawson,
"Introducing the Rasch Model as an Alternative Measurement Paradigm When
Investigating Quality of Life”
3. Alf H. Walle,
"Native People and Quality of Life: A Macromarketing Perspective"
xii
Monday, May 30th, 2005: (Continued)
12:00 noon – 1:30 pm
Lunch
1:30 pm – 3:00 pm
Plenary Session
Roger Dickinson
Opportunities for Marketing Practice
and Scholarship
Bayboro
Harborview
1. Philip Barnes and Ben Wooliscroft,
"Does Society Consider Marketing to Be a Science2?"
2. Robert W. Nason, John O'Shaughnessy, Clifford J. Shultz II and William L. Wilkie,
Panel: The Future of Marketing
3:00 pm – 3:30 pm
Break
3:30 pm – 4:30 pm
Concurrent Sessions
Bill Redmond
Competition and Markets
1. Susie Pryor and Sandy Grossbart,
"Social Capital and Co-Production in the Marketplace"
Harborview
2. Ben Wooliscroft and Tim Molteno
"Ants, Brand Loyalty, Variety Seeking, Market Topography, and Aggregate Adaptive
Optimization”
John Mittelstaedt
Faith, Meaning and Value
1. Mark Peterson and Stephen J. Terry,
"Early Twenty-First Century Worldviews and Macromarketing"
Williams
2. John D. Mittlestaedt and Robert A. Mittelstaedt,
"Macromarketing Implications of Religious Fractionalization and Religious Affiliation"
6:00 pm
6:30 pm – 9:00 pm
Depart Hilton lobby
Reception at Studio @ 620
The Studio @ 620 is a new creative venue in downtown
St. Petersburg that focuses on multicultural events
exploring history, visual arts, crafts, music, poetry,
theater, film, dance and dialogue.
7:30 pm – 8:30 pm
Presentation by Herb Snitzer –
jazz musician photographer
Herb Snitzer is a former photographic journalist for LIFE,
LOOK, TIME, FORTUNE and many other national
magazines and newspapers. He currently has a studio in
St. Petersburg, FL and works as a fine art photographer
and writer. More about his art can be found at
http://www.thebebopshop.com/acatalog/Herb_Snitzer.html
http://www.allaboutjazz.com/gallery/hsnitzer.htm
Herb’s email is herbsnitzer@aol.com.
9:30 pm
Return to Hilton St. Petersburg
xiii
Tuesday, May 31st, 2005:
8:00 am – 8:30 am
8:30 am – 10:00 am
Half Conference Day and Farewell
Continental Breakfast
Harborview
Plenary Session
Harborview
Don Rahtz
Development & Marketing
1. Terri L. Rittenburg,
"Women and Development in Uganda"
2. Anthony Pecotich and Russel Kingshott,
"Relationship Marketing - Competitive Strategy or Destruction of the Market?"
3. Anthony Pecotich and Kenneth Beng Yap,
"Macromarketing and the Concept of Openness: Toward a Research Agenda"
4. Paul D. Ellis, Anthony Pecotich, and Zhuang Guijun,
"National Development, Knowledge Transfer and Marketing Intermediaries in China"
10:00 am – 10:30 am
Break
10:30 am – 11:45 am
Concurrent Sessions
Stan Shapiro
Micro to Macro:
Innovations
Harborview
1. Hiroshi Kosaka,
"An Application of the Marketing Management Process to the Socio-political System"
2. Rosa Lemel,
"Strategic Factors Affecting the Success Rate of Online Grocers: Is the US Finally Ready
for Online Grocery Shopping?"
3. Marilyn Liebrenz-Himes,
"Everett Rogers and the Influence of Diffusion Research in the Developing World"
Sandy Grossbart
Micro to Macro:
Transition Economies
Williams
1. Katherine C. Sredl and Natasa Renko,
“’Balkan Bricolage’: Consumption Symbolism in Socialist and Post-Socialist Croatia”
2. Shawn Strange,
"The Emerging Consumer Society of Southeast China and
the Growing Divide in China's Transforming Economy "
3. Jay Wu,
"Interdependent Selves and Retail Decision Making with Companions:
The Case of Chinese Mall Patrons"
11:45 am – 12:00 noon
xiv
Wrap-up
Harborview
Beyond Their Imagination: What the Titans Thought and
What is Happening Now
A. Coskun Samli
Research Professor
University of North Florida
Abstract
When “titans” in economic development and marketing interaction formulated their thought
processes and developed their models such as: W. W. Rostow the stages of Economic Growth,
Clark-Fisher hypothesis dealing with sequential economic development or Slater presenting his
model which depended on efficient distribution system, it was assumed that these theories are
applicable to mixed economies where private and public sectors work together in a coordinated
manner. Here macro problems are government’s responsibility and micro problems are handled
by the private sector. But with pressures to deemphasize the role of government and additional
pressures to privatize, a situation has emerged where macro global problems are being turned
over to micro elements. Accordingly, uncertainties are increasing about acerbating societal
inequalities by relying on micro elements to accomplish societal goals through markets. In this
paper, the author raises eight key areas that are “micromanaged” now. Such activity may be
disabling to all of society by complicating further economic development. These are:
technology, infrastructure, access to education, environmental issues, progressiveness of income
tax, investment policies, distribution of profits and entrepreneurship.
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
Technology is used for income generation for a few people or groups, rather than raising
the economic well-being of all.
Infrastructures are not improved or maintained properly and thus the society’s
advancement is impaired.
Progress is hindered by too much emphasis on private education for profit and not
enough access to education on the part of the people with limited means.
Environmental activities are looked at as a cost factor by many private sector activists as
opposed to a major economic stimulant by the government.
Progressiveness of the income tax is necessary to develop a leveled playing-field and pay
for things such as research, education and infrastructure.
Global giants, instead of expanding, investing and developing, prefer mergers and
acquisitions. The resultant lack of competition causes tremendous harm for the markets
and quality of life of the masses.
Giving unnecessary tax cuts along with giving 500 per cent increases to CEO pay during
the decade of the 1990s (as opposed to about 35 per cent increase in the income of middle
class) is creating tremendous hardship and poor direction for the markets as a whole.
The private sector, in its micro-management mode, is not providing great opportunities
for the much-needed entrepreneurial activity.
1
History of Macromarketing
Moderator
M. Joseph Sirgy
Panel
George Fisk, Emory University
Tom Klein, University of Toledo
Roger Layton, University of New South Wales
Robert A. Mittlestaedt, University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Stanley J. Shapiro, Simon Fraser University
Abstract
The macromarketing school of marketing refers to the role
and impact of marketing activities and institutions on society
and vice versa. During the early 1960s, there was a growing
concern about the role of business institutions in society.
Public opinion, which considered business as a viable and
necessary institution in a society founded on the free
enterprise system, had begun to doubt its intentions and
activities. Phrases such as price fixing, military-industrial
complex, and monopoly powers were gaining increasing
attention in the 1960s. As the society began to critically
examine business activities soon after the incidents of
thalidomide and defective automobiles, business schools
began to encourage seminars on business ethics and
stakeholder analysis. Macromarketing emerged as a school
of marketing directly as a consequence of the growing interest
in the role of business in society.
Sheth, Gardner and Garrett 1988, page 138.
Sheth, Jagsdish N., David M. Gardner, and Dennis E. Garrett (1988), Marketing Theory:
Evolution and Evaluation, New York: John Wiley & Sons.
2
Misuse of the Marketing Concept:
The Marketing of Seemingly Healthy Packaged Food
William H. Redmond
Bowling Green State University
Abstract
An increasing number of consumers are seeking to lead healthier lives by eating more healthful foods.
Some, however, are unknowingly consuming foods which are less healthful and wholesome than they
expect. Through packaging and promotion, food marketers highlight (apparently) healthful properties of
foods, while disguising or drawing attention away from the less desirable aspects. To the extent that
consumers are being mislead, their interests are not being well served in the marketplace At question
here are fundamental issues concerning the operation of marketing in society: the marketing concept,
consumer sovereignty, and the role of marketing research. The paper takes a critical stance toward
certain food marketing practices and argues that, in many instances, consumer interests are being
subordinated to sellers’ interests.
Introduction
Among trends that have affected marketers is the growing health-consciousness of an
increasingly large portion of the population. This trend has several marketable facets including physical
fitness equipment and clubs, self-help books and videos, vitamins and supplements. For many
consumers, dietary changes appear to offer a straightforward way to improve health. That is, health is the
desired end result and more wholesome and healthful food choices are seen as a means to achieve that
end. Specific objectives of healthy eating are varied, and may include weight loss, cardiovascular fitness,
diabetes, increased intake of nutrients, avoidance of additives, among other goals. Hence a market
opportunity exists to supply such foods. Among the tens of thousands of packaged food products in a
typical grocery, many display wholesome ingredients and offer various health benefits.
In reality, the appearance of wholesomeness and healthfulness can be deceiving. For instance in
one marketer’s “nutri-grain” bar, the leading ingredient is sugar (Branell and Horgan 2004). Examples
are frequently reported in a monthly feature of Consumer Reports, “Selling It: Goofs, Glitches, Gotchas”.
An orange juice marketer introduces “Light n’ Healthy,” which consists of regular orange juice, diluted
with water and supplemented with artificial sweeteners but sold at the same price (Consumer Reports
2004c). Fruit drink packages frequently highlight fruits which are either not the main ingredient or are
not present at all (Consumer Reports 2004a). A blueberry pancake mix contains no actual blueberries but
instead has blue bits which are composed of sugar, partially hydrogenated oils, cellulose gum and food
coloring (Consumer Reports 2004a).
These types of misleading practices are not the actions of rogue firms at the margins of the
industry, but rather are employed by large food firms that are literally household names, as well as
smaller niche marketers. More accurate, although not necessarily complete, information can usually be
obtained by examining the standardized nutrition label on the back of the package. Comprehending
nutrition labels however, is time-consuming and requires language skills along with some education and
up-to-date health knowledge. Surveys indicate that only 25-33% of consumers use the nutrition labels to
assess health claims (Derby and Levy 2001). Relatively fewer low-income or low-education consumers
3
make use of nutrition labels. For these reasons, a significant portion of consumers do not – or are not able
to – avail themselves of the information contained therein. Hence the trend toward health-consciousness
offers marketers an opportunity to mislead as well as an opportunity to supply.
Social Influence on Marketing
Macromarketing has been defined differently by various researchers. Fisk (1982) articulated a
definition which seems to have been predominant since that time. In this, macromarketing is viewed as
the study of: 1) the impact and consequences of micromarketing on society, 2) the impact and
consequences of society on marketing systems, and 3) the understanding of marketing systems in their
aggregate dimensions.
The impact of society on marketing has been elaborated, albeit briefly, in connection with the
task of defining macromarketing. Society-to-marketing impacts encompass legal aspects of marketing as
well as the consequences of different political and social value systems (Hunt 1981). This impact can
occur in two basic ways: normal cultural and social influences, or public policy designed to affect the
environment or the societal consequences of marketing actions (Fisk 1982). In other words values are
important to understanding the society-to-marketing relationship, as is public policy. In the U.S., food
marketing practices are affected by public policy via the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) and to a
lesser extent by the Department of Agriculture (USDA) and the Federal Trade Commission (FTC).
Aside from cultural values and public policy, how does society affect marketing? The specific
mechanisms by which society, or segments thereof, exercise influence over marketing activities are
highly varied. For present purposes, the most obvious is marketers’ responding to changes in demand by
offering new products. In a competitive marketplace the principle of consumer sovereignty indicates that
sellers must follow the buyers’ desires. Successful marketers are the ones that respond to changing needs
and wants, for example of increasing health consciousness. When consumers’ wants and needs change,
sellers should be alert to this fact and make suitable adjustments to their offerings. This notion has long
been embodied in the marketing concept.
New and more widespread knowledge about the health effects of consumption choices has had an
impact on food purchases for many consumers. Increased demand for more wholesome and healthful
foods is one facet of this phenomenon. The response of food marketers to this trend is the subject of the
following analysis. In particular, assumptions concerning consumer sovereignty and the efficacy of
public policy protections are examined. This analysis begins with a general discussion of the marketing
concept as a guiding principle of organizational behavior. Of particular interest are the roles of consumer
sovereignty and marketing research in the operation of the marketing concept. Next, certain food industry
marketing practices are evaluated with respect to the operations of the marketing concept. The discussion
section evaluates misleading practices.
THE MARKETING CONCEPT
A venerable notion in the discipline of marketing is the marketing concept. The marketing
concept -- or customer orientation – has been expressed in a variety of forms, generally indicating that
firms ought first to understand customer wants and then develop suitable offerings to satisfy them. It is a
staple of marketing texts and, in one variant or another, is not infrequently featured in company mission
statements and publicity releases.
The fundamental notion that firms should be organized so as to serve customer needs and wants
existed prior to formalized statements of the marketing concept. An early edition of a leading text gives a
particularly clear articulation: “The first measure of success of any business is how well it serves the
customers. If an operation is not in the interest of the consumers, it is not justified, no matter how
profitable it may be to its owners,’ (Converse and Huegy 1946, 21). This is a statement of values, and
4
serves as an illustration of how values could be operationalized in the society-to-marketing relationship
outlined above. For present purposes, the following (partial) definition of the formalized marketing
concept is used because it highlights the role of information in operationalizing the concept: “Knowledge
of the customer, which requires a thorough understanding of his needs, wants, and behavior should be the
focal point of all marketing action. It implies the development of products and services to meet these
needs,” (Ball and Emory 1971, 39). This implies that systematic information gathering, e.g. marketing
research, is the primary and pivotal activity in implementing the marketing concept.
Consumer Sovereignty
Two assumptions are said to underpin the marketing concept (Dickinson, Herbst and
O’Shaughnessy 1986). First, consumers know what they want and are informed about the various
offerings. Regarding this assumption we accept the proposition that consumers know what they want. It
may be the case that some consumers’ demand for healthful foods is more directly connected with
symbolism and self-image than specific health outcomes, nevertheless, their market choice is for more
rather than less healthful foods. The proposition that consumers are well-informed, however, is
problematic. A number of studies have shown that many consumers are confused about food health
claims and about the putative health effects of various foods, ingredients, vitamins or supplements (Nestle
2002; Derby and Levy 2001). Confusion may result in varied outcomes, ranging from making choices
which do not promote health to giving up trying to evaluate health claims at all.
The second assumption is that consumer sovereignty prevails. Regarding this assumption, the
basic question is whether consumers are effective at advancing their own interests by their market choices
(Penz 1986). When consumer sovereignty does not prevail, the market system serves interests of sellers
more effectively than the interests of buyers. That is, firms may successfully offer goods and services
which are profitable for them but which are suboptimal for consumers. Whether consumer sovereignty
prevails is not necessarily in the affirmative and may not be of an all-or-nothing variety. Because of
different decision-making styles, some consumers may be more effective at advancing their interests in
the marketplace than others (Redmond 2000). While the following adopts a critical perspective with
respect to food marketers, it should also be borne in mind that lack of vigilance on the part of consumers
is also an issue. A more skeptical orientation by consumers would help to ameliorate the effects of
marketers' inflated and one-sided health claims.
Marketing Research
Returning to the above definition of the marketing concept, because knowledge of consumer
wants is important, marketing research is important. Marketing research is the mechanism by which
consumer needs become known to the firm in a systematic and reliable fashion. Consequently firms
engage in marketing research, and this is particularly crucial in times of changing preferences.
Marketing research is strictly informational in the sense that it is an input to a decision-making
process; the research itself doesn’t provide the decision. From the standpoint of the marketing concept,
this seemingly obvious distinction is highly salient. The information can be used to design and deliver
what the consumers want, or it can be used to design and deliver something that the company wants to
sell but which misleadingly gives the appearance of being what the consumers want. If consumers do
indeed know what they want but cannot reliably determine if they are getting it, they may be ineffective at
advancing their interests in the marketplace. When firms exploit this gap for their own interests, it means
that consumer sovereignty is subverted. Firms that pursue the latter approach thus make a more
sophisticated use of marketing research, first to find out what consumers want and second to find out how
to appear to be offering it while actually offering something different.
5
MISLEADING MARKETING OF PACKAGED FOODS
Consumer awareness of, and concerns about, the connection between eating habits and health
grew significantly during the 1980's due to heightened publicity of a variety of health-related studies.
Most of these studies examined the effects of a single component, such as cholesterol, on a single
outcome, such as heart disease. In other words, much of the publicity could be characterized as being
one-dimensional rather than a balanced approach to overall health. In the mid-1980's, food companies
promoted one-sided health claims for their products (e.g., heart health) and achieved notable sales gains
(Derby and Levy 2001). This was followed by a proliferation of food packages with one-sided health
claims by the late 1980's.
New products play an important role in marketing strategies and many new are targeted to healthconscious consumers (Porter and Earl 1990). Health sells, but fat, salt and sugar generate repeat business
(Brownell and Horgen 2004). Many of these products are marketed with unbalanced health claims.
Unhealthful foods taste good, are convenient and sell well (Brownell and Horgen 2004). Because of the
significance of the consumer trend toward health-consciousness in terms of sales and profits, food
companies had a correspondingly significant motivation to understand the trend in some detail. Some
effects on these firms’ approach to marketing research, packaging, and advertising and publicity are
outlined below.
Marketing Research
Health researcher Marion Nestle (2002) notes that the food industry is highly competitive and the
effort to develop products that will sell well and be profitable requires significant financial resources.
“Some of these funds pay for market research that is simply breathtaking in its comprehensiveness, level
of detail, and undisguised cynicism,” (Nestle 2002, 179). The cynicism here refers to the close attention
paid to how the new or redesigned foods are perceived and are to be sold (package design, graphics, etc.)
with somewhat less concern for the healthful properties of the food products. Similarly, Porter and Earl
(1990) note the intensive use of marketing research by food companies and its connection to the “selling
of health.”
As a result of extensive research, food companies know that many consumers want a healthier
lifestyle and that they believe healthful foods to be a part of that lifestyle (Porter and Earl 1990). Many
consumers are inclined to believe that “natural” foods such as whole grains, fruits and vegetables are
healthful and that specific food components such as sodium, fat, calories and cholesterol may be harmful.
Also as a result of research, food companies know what people like to eat. Humans like the taste of fat,
salt and sugar because evolution has shaped us in that way. These inherent tastes conferred evolutionary
fitness at a time when those items were scarce, but the taste preference is retained although these dietary
components are by no means scarce today.
Misleading marketing with respect to health is sufficiently common that some authors refer to it
by umbrella terms such as healthy-seeming (Brownell and Horgen 2004) and healthful-sounding
(Consumer Reports 2004b). No textbook discussion of the marketing concept includes the idea that
marketing research might be used to mislead customers, yet the design and promotion of healthy-seeming
or healthful-sounding foods is just such an application.
Packaging
How to combine the disparate consumer wants of health and taste? Market foods that appear to
have enhanced healthfulness but do not. This is done by highlighting a healthful property of the food
while not mentioning less healthful aspects such as high levels of salt or sugar or fat. Food packages may
proclaim reduced fat while having high levels of sugar, or promote low sodium while having high levels
of fat. Many consumers who limit fat intake with the intention of losing weight actually gain weight
because their overall caloric intake has risen (Derby and Levy 2001). Highlighting one-sided health
6
aspects without a balancing promotion of less desirable aspects is legal in the U.S. and is commonly done.
This is among the food marketing practices which some observers believe “border on the deceptive”
(Porter and Earl 1990). Others simply call them as deceptive, without qualification (e.g. Brownell and
Horgen 2004). Whether it does or does not cross that border, many would describe these practices as
being of questionable ethics (e.g. Nestle 2002).
Other problematic activities include the practice of prominently labeling foods as being
cholesterol-free, despite the fact that the type of food doesn’t normally contain cholesterol (Porter and
Earl 1990). Recent instances of this particular form of misleading packaging focus on low carbohydrates.
Also, some packages announce that a food is reduced-fat, without indicating the reference point from
which the reduction was made. The terms “light” and “lite” lack a clear meaning because they have been
variously used to indicate fewer calories, less fat, lower sodium, milder flavor or lighter color (Porter and
Earl 1990). Some of these practices are no longer permitted (Derby and Levy 2001), however the
problem of confusion among consumers continues and may persist for an extended period.
Certain ingredients thought to be harmful or undesirable, such as trans fats, are often not listed in
the nutrition label at all. Trans fatty acids are considered so unhealthful that the FDA has recently
required their inclusion on nutrition labels, effective in 2006 (Hugner, Maher and Granitz 2004). Food
companies have fought to avoid mandatory listing of trans fats because trans fats have favorable
production cost and shelf-life properties. Food companies also resisted the original nutrition labeling
requirements and lobbied to obtain the least restrictive forms (Nestle 2002).
Advertising and Publicity
About one-third of all food advertising is thought to be directed at children (Brownell and Horgen
2004). That is, a significant portion of food advertising is directed at the least skeptical and least
informed segment of the population. Unlike cigarette and alcohol advertising, food ads carry no warnings
about the dangers of over-consumption. Whether targeted at children or adults, less healthful foods are
profitable because food companies know how to make them taste good at low cost. Hence Brownell and
Horgen (2004, 201) conclude: “The most heavily marketed foods, and those most convenient and
accessible, tend to be the least healthy.”
The desire to be profitable and the desire for a positive public image sets up a paradox for food
companies, because unhealthful foods are highly profitable (Brownell and Horgen 2004). Food firms are
very image-conscious and are quite aware of the tensions between serving consumer interests and
stockholder interests (Nestle 2002). As a result, publicity has become a more prominent element of food
marketers’ strategy. Food firms make high-profile announcements of initiatives to reduce portion sizes,
cut fat and calories, and promote healthy eating, particularly among children (Economist 2003).
However, some observers see considerable self-interest behind these pronouncements. One reaction to a
major food company’s announcement of a health initiative was that it constituted “. . . an opening salvo
in a PR offensive aimed more at preserving the company’s lucrative snack food business than revamping
it,” (Newsweek 2003). Three months later, and with much less fanfare, the same firm increased the
package size (hence calories, fat and salt) of a popular after-school snack by 50% (Consumer Reports
2004).
To attract health-conscious consumers and reward shareholders is aided by lack of clear and
understandable information:
The food industry’s marketing imperatives principally concern four
factors: taste, cost, convenience and . . . public confusion, (Nestle 2002,
16).
7
The food industry has employed lobbying, advertising and public relations to convince lawmakers as well
as the general public that the science relating to healthy diet is both confusing and contradictory (Nestle
2002). That misleading or one-sided marketing of foods is legal in the U.S. is the result of substantial
lobbying efforts by food firms on Congress and continual pressure on the FDA. In the book Food
Politics, Nestle (2002) examines the industry’s relationship with regulators and legislators, and the
resultant state of consumer knowledge and awareness of health impacts. These efforts have delayed
promulgation of dietary guides such as the food pyramid and nutrition labels and have blocked the FDA
from requiring proof of efficacy for additives and supplements.
DISCUSSION
It may be argued that any form of misleading or deceptive marketing constitutes a misuse of
marketing. Here, however, we are concerned with specific concepts, such as the marketing concept, in
relation to misleading marketing. In “The Misuse of Marketing,” Bennett and Cooper (1981) describe a
situation in which an over-application of the marketing concept was the root of the problem. In
particular, they argued that simplistic reactions to survey data by certain firms resulted in a lack of
genuine product innovation. This occurred through marketing research because consumers could only
articulate their needs and wants in terms of incremental improvements or superficial styling changes to
existing products. Meanwhile, other competitors focused their efforts on substantive technological
development and meaningful product innovation. The result was significant loss of market share for the
non-innovative firms.
The misuse of marketing by food companies is of a different type and proceeds from different
causes. This is not an unthinking over-application of the marketing concept but rather a deceptive partial
application. This misuse of marketing also involves marketing research, but here the misuse is to
discover that health and wholesomeness are attractive to consumers and to design packaging and
advertising which appears to deliver those qualities, but does not. The misleading or deceptive claim is
highly visible in prominent type and graphics on the front of food packages and advertisements, while the
actual contents are in small type on the back of the package and do not appear in advertisements.
The practice of marketing healthy-seeming foods is a misuse of marketing because it uses
marketing techniques to undermine two foundational assumptions of the marketing concept. The first
assumption is that consumers are informed about various offerings (Dickinson, Herbst and
O’Shaughnessy 1986). Here, the distinction between healthy and healthy-seeming is a reflection of
consumer confusion and lack of accurate information. The second assumption is that consumer
sovereignty prevails (Dickinson, Herbst and O’Shaughnessy, 1986). When companies understand what
consumers want but instead provide them with what the companies prefer to sell, consumer sovereignty
cannot be said to prevail.
In Bennett and Cooper’s version of the misuse of marketing, non-innovative firms lost sales and
market share to more innovative competitors. Consumer sovereignty, however, prevailed because
consumers shifted to more desirable offerings. In that instance, failure to deliver what consumers wanted
resulted in harm to the non-innovative firms’ shareholders. In the food industry’s misuse of marketing,
consumers are also not offered what they want, but the practice of deception prevents loss of sales or
market share. Consumer sovereignty is subverted for those buyers who know what they want but do not
understand exactly what it is that they are buying. In this instance, failure to deliver what consumers want
results in harm to the consumers, not to the shareholders.
As the above analysis indicates, the assumption of consumer sovereignty in food marketing
should be regarded as questionable. When consumer sovereignty is undermined, the benefits of market
participation flow to the sellers rather than the buyers. This outcome is a negation of the values set forth
in Converse and Huegy’s (1946) precept: “If an operation is not in the interest of consumers, it is not
8
justified, no matter how profitable it may be to its owners.” In assessing the relationship between society
and marketing, food firm’s misuse of marketing reveals two problematic areas: first a breakdown of
consumer sovereignty and second a breakdown of appropriate guiding values.
CONCLUSION
The use of marketing techniques to mislead consumers is a misuse of marketing. This does not
imply that all marketing of food products is misleading or that all consumers are mislead. Also not
implied is harmful intent on the part of food firms, rather a set of priorities which places competitiveness
and profit at the top. It is surely the right of marketers to show their offerings in the best possible light,
and the principle of caveat emptor is not without application here. In the instance of food consumption
however, the consequences of misinformed choice may have wider ramifications than with other product
types. That numerous instances of misleading marketing derail consumers’ attempts at healthier eating is
particularly disturbing to nutritionists and other health experts. Consequently this misuse of marketing is
not a narrowly economic issue, it is a public health issue. As such, it represents a wide departure from
Converse and Huegy’s (1946) establishment of consumer interests as the value of highest priority.
As noted earlier, among the mechanisms by which society impacts marketing are values,
consumer sovereignty and public policy. The failure of consumer sovereignty to operate properly is a
type of market failure (Harris and Carman 1983; 1984). A general solution to market failure, although
not always the best one, is public policy intervention. Indeed, agencies such as the FDA were established
to protect consumers’ interests in the food marketplace. However as Nestle (2002) points out, a key part
of the problem is that food industry lobbyists have had a significant impact on crafting the regulations
which govern their actions. This is an example of regulatory failure (Carman and Harris 1986). In other
words, the failure of consumer sovereignty and appropriate guiding values has been compounded by
regulatory failure. Thus for society to exercise an impact on marketing practice in the food industry, a
new and more consumer-oriented regulatory regime may be required. It is possible that recent publicity
concerning the relationship between the FDA and pharmaceutical companies will also result in political
incentives to re-examine the relationship between the FDA and food companies.
REFERENCES
Bell, Martin and C. W. Emory. 1971. The Faltering Marketing Concept. Journal of Marketing, 35
(October): 37-42.
Bennett, R. and R. Cooper. 1981. The Misuse of marketing: An American Tragedy. Business Horizons,
(November - December): 51-61.
Brownell, Kelly and K. Horgen. 2004. Food Fight: The Inside Story of the Food Industry. Chicago:
Contemporary Books.
Carman, James and R. Harris. 1986. Public Regulation of Marketing Activity, Part III: A Typology of
Regulatory Failures and Implications for Marketing and Public Policy. Journal of
Macromarketing, 6 (Spring): 51-64.
Consumer Reports. 2004. (January): 13.
Consumer Reports. 2004a. Selling It. (January): 63.
Consumer Reports. 2004b. Adding Health to the Menu. (September): 28-31.
Consumer Reports. 2004c. Selling It. (November): 75.
9
Converse, Paul and H. Huegy. 1946. The Elements of Marketing. New York: Prentice-Hall.
Derby, Brenda and A. Levy. 2001. Do Food Labels Work? in P. Bloom and G. Gundlach (eds.),
Handbook of Marketing and Society. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dickinson, Roger, A. Herbst and J. O’Shaughnessy. 1986. Marketing Concept and Customer
Orientation. European Journal of Marketing (20): 18-23.
Economist. 2003. Thin End of the Wedge. (July 5): 59.
Harris, Robert and J. Carman. 1983. Public Regulation of Marketing Activity: Part I: Institutional
Typologies of Market Failure. Journal of Macromarketing, 3 (Spring): 49-58.
__________ and __________. 1984. Public Regulation of Marketing Activity: Part II: Regulatory
Response to Market Failure. Journal of Macromarketing, 4 (Spring): 41-52.
Hugner, Renee, J. Maher and N. Granitz. 2004. Why Parents Buy, in J. Mittelstaedt and S. Shapiro
(eds.), Macromarketing Scholarship and Education for a Global Century: 258-261.
Nestle, Marion. 2002. Food Politics: How the Food Industry Influences Nutrition and Health. Berkeley
CA: University of California Press.
Newsweek. 2003. Just Saying No to Junk. (July 14): 44.
Penz, G. 1986. Consumer Sovereignty and Human Interests. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Porter, Donna and R. Earl (eds.). 1990. Nutrition Labeling: Issues and Directions for the 1990's.
Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.
Redmond, William. 2000. Consumer Rationality and Consumer Sovereignty. Review of Social Economy,
2 (June): 177-196.
10
__________________________________________________
Consumer Attitudes Toward Organic Foods: A Crosscultural Examination of U.S. and Irish Market Segments
Julie Stanton, Saint Joseph’s University
Renee Hughner, Arizona State University
Pierre McDonagh, Dublin Institute of Technology
Andrea Prothero, University College, Dublin
Extended Abstract
Introduction
Estimates of the size of the organic food industry in developed markets attest to its growing importance to
producers and consumers as well as to a shift in classification from niche to mainstream market (Palmer
2001). In the United States, for example, sales have grown from an estimated $78 million in 1980
(McDonald 2000) to some $7.8 billion in 2000 (Dimitri and Greene) and are expected to exceed $20
billion by the end of 2005 (McDonald; Organic Trade Association, 2001). Annual growth rates of 20
percent are generally reported.
In the United Kingdom, annual growth rates of as much as 40 and 55 percent have been observed in
recent years (Palmer 2001). Sales have risen from approximately $154 million (£100 million) in 1994 to
an anticipated $2.22 billion (£1.47 billion) in 2005 (Boxall 2000). In Ireland, sales of organic products
were estimated at $43.7 million (€38 million) in 2003 (Food and Drink Europe), and both production and
consumption are expected to continue to rise significantly in the next few years. It is anticipated that the
Irish organic food industry will reach mainstream status by 2006 (Nutra Ingredients Europe) with sales of
over $100 million (€86 million) that year (Cowan, NiGhraith and Henchion, 2001).
Despite this rapid growth in organic food sales, the industry still has conflicting indicators of who its
customers really are (Hughner et al. 2003). Further, there is some agreement that consumers themselves
are confused about what ‘organic’ means (e.g. Aarset et al. 2004; Fotopoulos and Krystallis 2002). This
is not surprising when one considers the variety of similar labels such as “all natural”, “no artificial
ingredients”, “GMO-free”, “cage-free”, and “dolphin-safe” with which consumers are confronted
regularly. Discerning the distinctions between each requires detailed information that many consumers
do not possess. That the industry has shown rapid growth in the last two decades despite this confusion
warrants further examination.
The existence of national standards was meant to reduce consumer confusion. Since October 2002, the
U.S. National Organic Standard has set guidelines for how ‘organic’ food should be produced and
marketed, whether in raw or processed form, whether crop or livestock based. In developing the rule, the
U.S. government attempted to incorporate the wide range of ideas held on what ‘organic’ means, but the
national standard has been criticized by some groups for being too lenient in certain areas (see e.g.
Gutman, 1999). In Ireland, the European Union’s standard is implemented through three certification
bodies. European Council Regulations No 2092/91 and No 1804/1999 specify similar conditions as in the
U.S. standard but the two systems have notable differences in the areas of cropping standards, manure
restricting, livestock regulations and processing (Organic Trade Association 2002).
11
That public policy initiatives like these have not yet succeeded in eliminating confusion for consumers is
a strong indication that much is still to be learned – by industry, policymakers, and consumers. What do
consumers understand the term ‘organic’ to convey? What motives do they have for purchasing foods
labeled ‘organic’? Do other eco-labels carry similar importance for consumers? Do consumers trust
government standards to tell them something meaningful? Do European and U.S. consumers differ in
these issues? What are the most effective means to convey to consumers the value in the ‘organic’ label?
As Gutman describes, the term ‘organic’ refers to “a set of philosophical beliefs about our relationship
with the environment, not merely to the physical characteristics of a product.” To set forth a single
definition of ‘organic’ will result, according to Gutman, in the deprivation of the right to make choices in
concert with one’s own beliefs. How then can the goals of reducing consumer confusion and protection
of consumer choice be reconciled? These are all questions which are interesting to explore from a
“marketing in society” perspective.
It stands to reason that a more in-depth understanding of the motivations that lead different consumers to
purchase organic foods – if at all – would bring a measure of clarity to whether the ‘organic’ label is
serving its intended purpose or perhaps signifying other attributes to consumers. This study uses a
mixture of qualitative and quantitative methods to identify the heterogeneity of philosophies that is
actually held by U.S. and Irish consumers in regard to organic foods. The purpose is manifold: one, to
compare values claimed by distinct consumer segments to that espoused in ‘organic’ label requirements;
two, to contrast those segments across cultural dimensions and investigate whether labeling laws better
‘suit’ consumers in one country v. the other; and three, to show the organic food industry that different
(potential) consumer segments could be targeted with better understanding of what matters most to them.
Related Literature
Dozens of empirical studies have been published in the last twenty years on the topic of consumer
purchasing motives, beliefs and behaviors toward organic foods. As detailed in Hughner et al. (2003), the
studies vary widely in their methodologies, geographic focus, and conclusions. Together, they
demonstrate the potentially great differences in values consumers place on individual attributes of the
‘organic’ concept.
For example, while several studies have found that environmental concern rates highly in encouraging
purchase of organic foods (e.g. Hill and Lynchehaun 2002; Wandel and Buggel 1997; Roddy, Cowan and
Hutchinson 1996), others have found perceptions of healthfulness, taste and nutrients to outweigh concern
for the environment (e.g. Magnusson et al. 2003; Zanoli and Naspetti 2002; Shifferstein and Ophuis
1998).
On the possible health attributes of organic foods, some studies have concluded that consumers perceive
organic foods as healthier because of the requirement that they not be produced with pesticides and other
chemicals (e.g. Wilkens and Hiller 1994; Jolly 1991; Misra et al. 1991). Yet according to other studies,
some consumers believe organic food is also more nutritious (e.g. Hill and Lynchehuan 2002), despite
little scientific evidence to support that notion.
Studies also demonstrate that consumer motives for purchasing organic foods can stem from taste
preferences, food safety concerns, consideration of animal welfare, desire to support the local economy,
and other sentimental, nostalgia-oriented factors (Hill and Lynchehaun 2002; Schifferstein and Ophuis
1998; Roddy, Cowan and Hutchinson 1996). It is not surprising when one considers this wide variety of
notions and attitudes that a degree of confusion is detected in the literature regarding consumer
understanding of the term ‘organic’.
It is this general fuzziness of understanding and the variety of notions assigned to it that motivates the
present study. By allowing individuals to self-identify (or not) with statements that reflect the plethora of
12
perspectives observed in published studies, we can more accurately identify types of consumers and their
attitudes toward organic foods. As stated earlier, these segments will be useful not only to marketers who
can orient their promotional campaigns accordingly, but also for a more general discussion of what the
term ‘organic’ should mean when it comes to a national organic standard.
Methodology
In order to reveal clear segments of organic shoppers, in order to better understand such consumers -- a
benefit for organic producers, the environment, consumers and public policy makers alike -- this study
uses the Q-methodology (Stephenson 1967) along with a brief written survey and an in-depth interview
with each respondent.
In a nutshell, Q-methodology allows the researcher to capture the respondent’s own opinions from a selfreferential standpoint, rather than limit data collection to external traits and attitudes. By offering the
respondent the chance to pick and choose among statements which best portray the individual’s
perspective, the researcher captures a truer indication of respondent values while also removing the
potential for starting-point bias. The method relies on the individual creating a single rank-ordering of
offered statements from “most like” to “least like” his/her viewpoints, and allows the respondent to
interpret those statements in the most natural way. This obviates the need for separate scales for aligning
respondents on a continuum (McKeown and Brown 1988).
The results of factor analysis on the responses are self-identified groupings of like-minded people. Since
our goal is to identify individual segments of consumers based on their attitude toward purchasing organic
foods, this method is advantageous. Hughner (2000) demonstrated the potential of this methodology for
identifying healthcare paradigms held by American consumers. By way of illustration, a list of
representative statements for our Q-sample is given in table 1. The segments generated by factor analysis
of the Q-sorts are given more context through study of the in-depth interviews of respondents and a
review of the demographic variables captured for each.
For the Macromarketing Seminar in 2005, we have conducted a pilot study using this methodology as the
first stage of research. Because one of our goals is to examine cross-cultural differences in consumer
segments regarding organic food, the pilot consists of a small sample of Q-sorts collected in Dublin,
Philadelphia and Phoenix that are examined collectively and separately.
As with any pilot study, the results are more indicative than conclusive. Input from Macromarketing
Seminar participants will also help shape the full study: theoretical underpinnings to consumer
motivation and cross-cultural variation need detailing; breadth of consumer types represented in the Qstatements needs reflection; methods for capturing cross-cultural factors need discussion. We also hope
to generate discussion on how this analysis illustrates the impact of organic marketing on consumers, and
what that implies for public policy makers, and macromarketers.
Contributions to Literature
By building on the existing literature on organic food consumers, we are able in this study to make
several advances. First, by allowing respondents to select statements that most (and least) reflect their
own attitudes, we are able to build deeper descriptions of consumer segments. Coupled with in-depth
interviews which explore the thoughts behind the respondent’s choices, the factor analysis results are
vivid descriptions with examples for context. Other studies have relied on more externally-driven
categorization as is often encouraged by classic consumer theory. Further, the interviews allow us to
explore the meanings placed by respondents on terms such as ‘wholesome’ and ‘food safety’ rather than
assume they hold some common understanding. Previous studies have allowed the terms to be
interpreted without further discussion.
13
Second, our psychographic segmentation of consumers is coupled with demographic and behavioral
variables so that a more ‘familiar’ description of each segment can be provided. Demographics include
income, age, education level, occupation, household size and location. Behavioral variables include
frequency of organic food purchases, types of organic foods consumed, etc. These additional variables
give more concreteness to descriptions that might otherwise seem too abstract to be useful.
Third, the in-depth interview helps separate the confusion often seen in the literature regarding purchaser
and consumer. As studies have occasionally referenced ‘buying for small children’ as a motive for
organic food purchases (e.g. Hill and Lynchehaun 2002), and found that women are more likely to be
‘consumers’ of organic foods (e.g. Magnusson et al. 2001; Food Marketing Institute 2001), one wonders
if the information is relevant. If the stereotype that women are the food shoppers for their households
holds true, then their gender can easily mask any number of considerations that yield the purchase
outcome. Our respondents are limited to purchasers, rather than their spouses or children, but the
interviews are structured to reveal any motivations beyond their own beliefs (i.e. a spouse’s wishes).
Fourth, we make a clear choice to include all types of food shoppers in our study, not just regular
consumers of organic foods. The literature has generally left vague the notion of the occasional consumer
of organic food (by frequency or by category), tending instead to focus on regular consumers of organic
food, all consumers of organic foods without distinction, or non-consumers. We believe the difference
between regular and occasional consumption is important to the organic food industry.
Fifth, because our goals include aiding in the match between industry efforts and consumer desires, we
use both the written survey and the interview to unearth likely avenues through which organic food
education initiatives would be most successful. With a nearly endless source of information available
through newspapers, television, magazines, the internet, friends, retail outlets, etc., it is natural to question
which are most likely to reach the consumer in the most effective way as well as how those outlets might
vary by consumer segment.
Finally, because so much has been invested in creating official definitions of “organic” such as the U.S.
national standard, our study offers additional perspective on how well that standard reflects the attitudes
of consumers. While we do not account for how the now-2-year-old standard in the U.S. may have
created or shaped those beliefs, the existence of segments which have preferences that are essentially not
met by the standard should be of some concern to policymakers.
Expected Outcomes for Stakeholders
As indicated earlier, we believe that the results of this study provide valuable information to the organic
food industry, to policymakers and to consumers themselves. For industry, for example, a better
understanding of the variety of consumer ‘types’ should offer new opportunities for tailoring products to
specific segments. Communication – whether general educational materials or more traditional
advertising – can be more effectively employed to reach the specific segment. Shopping habits by
segment will help define the industry’s market more carefully and offer cost-saving and/or revenueenhancing opportunities. For policymakers, on the other hand, discovering that the national organic
standard only correlates to a single type of organic food consumer could re-open a dialogue with
consumers and producers such that more appropriate definitions can be devised.
For consumers, the rapid growth of the industry in recent decades has offered a wide variety of new
organic food products, but limited supply, high prices, and poor segmentation and positioning have
negatively impacted the match between consumer desires and availability. Greater understanding of
consumer interests should help industry better serve the consumer.
14
Respondents
As noted, the respondents to this study are not limited to current purchasers of organic foods and hence
can be drawn from the general population. However, every effort is made to include people with varying
attitudes and a few basic screening questions are used to be sure that the sample is not biased in any one
direction. For convenience, the respondents to the pilot study are drawn from the university environment
(faculty, staff, students) as well as the nearby community. Three separate samples, in Arizona,
Philadelphia and Dublin, are drawn to identify geographic and cultural influences that may be present.
Table 1: Representative statements from Q-sample
As contrasted to conventional foods…
• “Consuming organic foods is better because organic foods are free of pesticides, hormones and
other additives.”
• “Consuming organic foods is better because organic food production is less damaging to the
environment.”
• “Organic foods are a healthier alternative for feeding small children.”
• “Consuming organic foods gives me inner harmony and brings me closer to a unity with nature.”
• “Consuming organic foods is better because organic food production is less harmful to human
laborers who avoid exposure to harmful chemicals.”
• “Consuming organic foods is consistent with a lifestyle of authentic vegetarianism.”
• “Organic foods taste better.”
• “Organic foods are safer and less likely to be contaminated with bacteria, etc.”
• “Consuming organic foods is better because organic food production supports farmers and the
local economy.”
• “Organic foods are just the ‘right’ way to grow food; everything else is unnatural.”
• “Organic foods are more nutritious.”
References
Aarset, B., S. Beckmann, E. Bigne, M. Beveridge, T. Bjorndal, J. Bunting, P. McDonagh, C. Mariojouls,
J. Muir, A. Prothero, L. Reisch, A. Smith, R. Tveteras, J. Young 2004. The European
Consumers’ understanding and perceptions of the ‘organic’ food regime: the case of aquaculture.
British Food Journal 106(2): 93-105.
Cowan, C., D. Ni Ghraith and M. Henchion (2001) The Organic Food Market: An Opportunity for
Ireland? Teagasc National Food Centre, www.teagasc.ie .
Dimitri, C. and C. Greene (2002) Recent Growth Patterns in the U.S. Organic Foods Market, U.S.
Department of Agriculture, Economic Research Service, Agricultural Information Bulletin
Number 777.
Food and Drink Europe (2003) Ireland focuses on €38m organic market,
www.foodanddrinkeurope.com/news , October 13.
Food Marketing Institute. 2001. “Organic Shoppers May Not Be Who You Think They Are,”
Washington, DC: FMI.
Fotopoulos, C. and A. Krystallis 2002. Organic product avoidance: reasons for rejection and potential
buyers’ identification in a countrywide survey. British Food Journal 104 (3/4/5): 233-260.
Gutman, B. N. 1999. Ethical eating: applying the kosher food regulatory regime to organic food. The
Yale Law Journal 108: 2351-2384.
15
Hill, H. and F. Lynchehaun 2002. Organic milk: attitudes and consumption patterns. British Food
Journal 104(7): 526-542.
Hughner R. 2000. A cultural analysis of health belief systems: identifying and understanding consumers’
health care paradigms. Ph.D. diss., Arizona State University.
Hughner R., P. McDonagh, A. Prothero, J. Stanton, C.J. Shultz II, 2003, “Exploring Organic Foods:
Issues, Findings and Prognoses,” in Darach Turley and Stephen Brown, eds. All Changed,
Changed Utterly?, European Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 6.
Jolly D.A. 1991. Determinants of organic horticultural products consumption based on a sample of
California consumers. Acta Horticulture. 295: 41-48.
Magnusson, M.K., A. Arvola, U-K. Hursti, L. Aberg, P-O. Sjoden 2003. Choice of organic foods is
related to perceived consequences for human health and to environmentally friendly behavior.
Appetite. 40(2): 109-117.
McDonald D. 2000. Organic Products Defined Farm Industry News, April.
McKeown, B. D. Thomas. 1988 Q Methodology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Misra S., C.L. Huang and S.L. Ott 1991. Georgia consumers’ preference for organically grown fresh
produce. Journal of Agribusiness. Fall.
NutraIngredients Europe (2002) Ireland: organic farming to be mainstream by 2006.
www.nutraingredients.com/news , April 16.
Organic Trade Association 2001. Organic Consumer Trends. Harleysville, PA: Natural Marketing
Institute.
Palmer A. (2001) “Organic Food”, Economic Review, 19(1), 2-11.
Roddy, G., C. Cowan and G. Hutchinson 1994. Organic food: a description of the Irish market. British
Food Journal. 96(4): 3-10.
Schifferstein H.N.J., and P.A.M. Oude Ophuis 1998. Health-related determinants of organic food
consumption in the Netherlands. Food Quality and Preference. 9(3): 119-33.
Stephenson, W. 1967. The Play Theory of Mass Communication. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Wandel, M. and A. Buggel 1997. Environmental concern in consumer evaluation of food quality. Food
Quality and Preference. 8(1): 19-26.
Wilkins J.L. and V.N. Hillers 1994. Influences of pesticide residue and environmental concerns on
organic food preference among food cooperative members and non-members in Washington
state. Journal of Nutrition Education. 26(1): 26-33.
Zanoli, R. and S. Naspetti 2002. Consumer motivation sin the purchase of organic food. British Food
Journal. 104(8): 643-653.
16
_________________________________________________
Extended Abstract
Genetically Modified Food: A Cross-Cultural Investigation
Timothy J. Burkink, University of Nebraska-Kearney*
Marko Grunhagen, Southern Illinois University-Edwardsville
Klervi N. Le Marre, Brioche Pasquier, France
Gary J. Wells, Clemson University
Carl L. Witte, Roosevelt University
* authors listed in alphabetical order
Introduction
Throughout history, humans have been trying to improve living structures for their own benefit. This is
especially true in agriculture. In the past, geneticists and farmers bred plants to create productive or more
resistant varieties, and the same happened with animals. Through progress in biological science and the
discovery of chromosomes, genes and the structure of DNA throughout the 20th century (INRA, 1998),
researchers are now able to transfer genes from one species to another, including between plants and
bacteria or animals (Chevallier, 2002). The result is called a Genetically Modified Organism, GMO. The
marketing of GMOs has been at the center of public, scientific, political and commercial debates for more
than a decade. And now a second generation of enhanced GMOs is on the horizon.
Macromarketing has long been concerned with marketing issues regarding the environment, economic
development, quality of life, and social welfare (Fisk 1981, Shapiro 1984). Fisk and Nason 1982 also
noted that macromarketing looks at marketing “activities as they relate to society,” e.g., the intersection of
micromarketing and society. In this cross-cultural study, we investigate the attitudes of university students
in two different societies regarding GMOs, specifically food crops, as they relate to all of the
macromarketing issues mentioned above. In order to introduce the issue of possible benefits to consumers
associated with GMOs, we also attempt to examine the question of whether there are significant
differences in acceptance of so-called second generation value enhanced GMOs compared to first
generation GMOs.
LITERATURE REVIEW
First Generation and Second Generation Genetically Modified Food
The aim of genetic engineering is to introduce, enhance or delete particular characteristics of an organism
(Nottingham, 1998). It is a process in which foreign genes are spliced into a related or non-related
species, creating a new organism. A genetically modified organism (GMO) is the actual organism (e.g.,
plant) that is created through genetic engineering. Biotech food, bioengineered food, and GM food are all
terms used to describe food that has been created through genetic engineering, or that contains products
that were created using genetic engineering (Cummings and Lilliston, 2000).
17
The so-called “first generation of GMOs” possesses enhanced input properties such as a resistance to a
particular insect or a tolerance of an herbicide. Bt corn, for instance, is enhanced with a gene from a soil
bacterium to produce proteins that kill insects that infest the plant (Seralini, 2000). Earlier technological
changes in agriculture have had opponents, but hostility to crops produced using recombinant DNA
technologies has been much stronger, and has arisen much more quickly (Ho, 1998).
Researchers are now developing “second generation GMOs” with output properties. The food industry
hopes that this next generation of bioengineered products will deliver consumer benefits, such as
functional foods with improved nutritional content, removing natural toxicants from foods like peanuts,
and improving the shelf life of fruits and vegetables (Harlander, 2001).
Consumer Acceptance: Benefits versus Risks
Risks
When researching acceptance differences between French and American consumers, it becomes important
to investigate perceptions of potential risks and benefits of GMOs to determine how similar the
assessment of benefits and risks is in the two countries (for a discussion of GMO-related risks, see also
Wells, Le Marre, Grünhagen, Surak and Miller 2004). The opposition from consumers to first generation
GMOs, driven by concerns about consumer food safety, the environment, corporate control of agriculture,
and ethics, is strong in Europe, and resistance to their use in developing countries has also arisen (Nelson,
2001).
Common concerns about GMOs among consumers worldwide include:
1) The dissemination of GMOs into the environment (Tangley, 1999; Darmency, 1997; Habert,
1994; INRA, 1998).
2) The uncertainty about the consequences of modified genes in the cells (Perrier, 1999; Ho,
1998).
3) The potential for new allergens and growing antibiotic resistance (Jackson and Villinski,
2002; Scalla 1997; Philippon, 1999; Wal, 1997; Perrier 1999)
4) The economic dependence on multinational biotechnological firms for farmers, and (Jackson
and Villinski , 2002; Vogt and Parish, 1999).
5) Ethical issues: Man playing God (Kemp, 1992)
Even though value-enhanced second generation GM products are not commercialized yet, they have been
criticized by consumer associations for many reasons. (Briand-Bouthiaux, 2001). In fact, critics combine
criticism regarding non-GM functional foods (functional foods developed naturally), with those already
made about first generation GMOs mentioned previously:
1) Some people are worried about false health claims because of a lack of government
regulation (Briand-Bouthiaux, 2001).
2) GM functional foods are not necessary; all the vitamins and supplements that are needed are
available through a balanced diet, and there is a risk that with increasing reliance on
functional additives consumers might shift from a diverse balanced diet to an unhealthy one
(Briand-Bouthiaux, 2001).
3) Functional food products could be obtained with non-GM methods (Briand-Bouthiaux,
2001).
4) New GM products will not be accessible for consumers in developing countries, because they
will be too expensive (Briand-Bouthiaux, 2001).
Benefits
The initial idea behind plant genetic engineering was to improve agronomic performance. The main GM
plants commercialized today are either resistant to insects or tolerant of herbicides (Seralini, 2000).
18
Perceived first generation GMO benefits include (Briand-Bouthiaux, 2001):
1) more competitiveness for agriculture,
2) reduced amounts of chemicals used in production,
3) enhanced quality of food,
4) less expensive food for consumers, and
5) improved quantity and quality of food produced in developing countries.
Second generation products offer (Harlander, 2001; Briand-Bouthiaux, 2001).
1) a better conservation of food products,
2) the production of enzymes by plants,
3) modification of nutritional contents for animal feeding,
4) better taste for human foods,
5) production of therapeutic molecules by plants,
6) agro-forestry, and
7) nutritional value enhanced crops (Briand-Bouthiaux, 2001).
The last second generation GMO benefit mentioned, is a principal object of this study.
Benefits of Second Generation GMOs Compared to First Generation
One of the most important differences between the first and second generation of GMOs is that first
generation agricultural biotechnology products do not offer direct benefits to consumers. On the other
hand, second generation value enhanced products that offer nutritional advantages, have a direct appeal to
consumers, as they can offer a better nutritional composition, or products free from toxic residues and
additives (Young, 1992).
In the past, several surveys of consumer attitudes toward GM foods have been conducted (e.g., Hoban,
1996; Kelley, 1995; Norton, Lawrence and Wood, 1998; Smith and Riethmuller, 1999; Wolf and
Domegan, 2000). Previous research has focused mainly on the first generation of GM foods, and not on
second generation products.
Cross Cultural Variations In Consumer Behavior
Hofstede (1994) suggests that the cultures of different nations can be compared in terms of five
dimensions. This paper only refers to those dimensions that may pertain to GMO acceptance. The most
important difference in the context of this study is “uncertainty avoidance.” An index Hofstede devised
shows that France is indexed 86 for uncertainty avoidance (rank 10-15) and the United States is indexed
41 (rank 43) (Keegan and Green, 2003, p.150), which means that French people on average are less
contemplative, less relativistic or less tolerant in their behavior than American people when they face
situations that are uncertain.
Another dimension that can be useful in understanding the attitudes toward GMOs in France and the U.S.
is the “individualism-collectivism” dimension. France is given the index 71 (rank 11) for individualism,
whereas the United States is indexed 91 (rank 1) (Keegan and Green, 2003, p.150). Consumers in the U.S.
might be expected to react on a more individualistic basis to GM food, rather than viewing it collectively
as an issue for the whole population.
Additional insights about cultural differences between French and American consumers can be derived
from other research that has been conducted. Europeans in general were surveyed for the Eurobarometer
52.1 (INRA, ECOSA 2000) regarding levels of “usefulness, risk, moral acceptance and encouragement of
development” relating to the use of biotechnology. Two applications of biotechnology were judged least
useful: the production of food using biotechnology, and the insertion of human genes into animals to
obtain organs for humans. The survey also showed the following: the less an application is considered to
19
be useful for society, the less people tend to accept it to be morally acceptable. Further, the less it is
believed that applications are morally acceptable and/or useful for society, the less one tends to believe
that they should be encouraged. Risks related to applications in food production were seen to be relatively
high. More positive attitudes about applications to micro-organisms are found than about applications of
genetic technology to plants and foods (INRA, ECOSA 2000).
In France, people in general have mixed feelings about genetically engineered foods, but they remain
mostly hostile (Briand-Bouthiaux, 2001). The general public's perception of scientific information is also
negative (Jackson and Villinski, 2002). The adoption of food biotechnology has been more difficult
because consumers are still traumatized by recent food-related contamination, such as the B.S.E. (Bovine
Spongiform Encephalithis) scandal, the dioxin contamination of poultry, and a contaminated blood
scandal (Le Marre, Grünhagen and Wells, 2004; McHughen, 2000). The result is a general rejection of
GM crops and GM foods (Chevallier, 2002). In August 2000, a survey conducted by the French national
newspaper “Liberation” revealed a growing sensitivity to food safety. Seventy-three percent of those
surveyed said that they were concerned about the presence of GMOs in their food (Vincendon, 2000).
The traditional meaning of food in the two countries adds an important dimension and provides another
reason for a negative French view of GM foods. France has a strong gastronomic tradition and GM foods
are viewed as threats to the traditional “natural” diet (Paillotin, 1999). Concerns for health as well as an
aversion to “unnatural” or artificial foods are additional criticisms regarding first generation GMOs by
French consumers (Paillotin, 1999). Warde (1997) and Bourdieu (1984) also indicated that French eating
habits are more about social experiences than American eating habits. French people take more time to
eat, and usually eat dinner together with the members of the family, whereas meals are shorter in the U.S.,
and less often a social experience. From an overall European perspective, food has greater importance in
European culture than in American culture (Ekici, 2000). It has been argued that the main reason for
resistance to GM food in Europe is the fact that it deals with food, and not with technology (Ekici, 2000).
Americans in general prefer eating out more often than Europeans. Europeans, prefer to dine at home.
Homemade food that is related to “natural food” is believed to be healthier (McIntosh, 1996). GM foods
are not considered a “natural product” and tend to be rejected. Furthermore, Europeans fear that the
“McDonaldization” of their food culture (Ritzer, 1996) will expand. Therefore a negative response to
GMOs may also be seen as an attempt to preserve a tradition and culture of food.
Americans in general are more positive about new technologies. They believe that scientists provide
valuable, unbiased information that can benefit society (Chevallier, 2002; Buzby and Ready, 1996). A
1987 survey revealed that most Americans expressed a generally positive attitude toward science and
technology, although there was certain ambivalence towards biotechnology’s role in improving life
(OTA, 1987). In this report, U.S. consumers said that they trusted government regulations over other
sources of food-safety information. However a more recent report, revealed that there were increasing
concerns about food safety issues, and that skepticism about the ability of public institutions to maintain
food safety was rising (OTA, 1992). This tends to show that U.S. consumers do consider new information
about risks and change their perception accordingly. Overall, however, the general acceptance of GM
foods in the United States appears to be higher than in Europe.
The differences in culture, particularly regarding food, between France and the United States are the main
reasons why the decision was made to conduct the survey in those two countries. The cross-cultural
comparison of the survey results is expected to provide additional insights, especially about the way
respondents perceive second generation nutritionally enhanced GM products.
SURVEY AND ANALYSIS
A total of 124 completed questionnaires were collected from French students enrolled at the Ecole
Supérieure dAgriculture dAngers (ESA), and 236 questionnaires were collected from American students
20
enrolled at Clemson University, South Carolina. The survey contained a series of single item Likert-type
scale questions (1=strongly agree through 5=strongly disagree) regarding an evaluation of what the
respondents in the two respective national samples thought about first generation GMOs and second
generation nutritionally enhanced GMOs in terms of the variables usefulness, moral acceptability, risk,
necessity, and encouragement of development.
Consistent with previous research, a two-stage approach was used to study differences between groups
(i.e., the two national samples) on multiple dependent variables (Darden and Perreault, 1975; Pedhazur,
1982; Spector, 1977). During the first stage of the analysis, where appropriate, a multivariate analysis of
variance (MANOVA) was used to test the null hypothesis of no simultaneous differences between the two
samples on the dependant variables. The second stage of analysis consisted of an analysis of variance
(ANOVA) of each variable tested under the overall MANOVA, as well as ANOVAs of individual
questions. Additional analysis was performed using paired t-tests to measure the differences in answers
given in the survey between first and second generation enhanced GM foods for each national sample, to
investigate whether respondents within each national group perceived enhanced second generation GM
foods differently from first generation GM foods.
CONCLUSION
The aim of this research was a cross cultural investigation of attitudes toward Genetically Modified
Organisms, specifically food crops, to determine if there were significant differences in the views of
French and American respondents. In addition, we sought to introduce the issue of possible consumer
benefits of GMOs into the research by examining differences in acceptance of value enhanced second
generation GMOs compared to first generation GMOs for both French and American respondents.
For all variables (usefulness, moral acceptability, risk, necessity, and encouragement of development), the
differences between the answers in the two national samples were significantly different. American
respondents perceived both first and second generation GMOs significantly more positively than French
respondents on all items, e.g. usefulness, acceptability, etc. These findings provide further support for
previous research results on the relative acceptance of GM foods by European and American consumers
that indicate a greater level of acceptance by American consumers.
Results of paired t-tests found that there were significant differences across the means between first and
second generation enhanced GMOs for the American respondents (except for morally acceptable). For all
other variables, American respondents favor enhanced GMOs more than first generation GMOs, which
indicates that American respondents believe that enhanced GMOs are significantly more useful, less
risky, and more necessary than first generation GMOs, and that the development of enhanced GMOs
should be encouraged to a greater extent than that of first generation GMOs.
The answers given by the French respondents were also significantly different between first and second
generation GMOs, (except for GMOs morally acceptable / enhanced GMOs morally acceptable, and
GMOs useful/ enhanced GMOs useful).
Again, the results show that the difference is consistently in favor of enhanced GMOs, which means that
French respondents also believe that second generation GMOs are significantly less risky, more necessary
than first generation GMOs, and that the development of enhanced GMOs should be encouraged to a
greater extent than that of first generation GMOs.
The questions that did not show a significant difference between first and second generation GMOs asked
about the moral acceptance of GMOs (for both the French and the American respondents) and about the
usefulness of GM foods (for the French respondents only). It makes sense that the question of morality is
not different between enhanced and first generation GMOs, because the ethical concern of “disturbing
nature” is the same for both generations of products, as the technical way of inserting genes is the same.
21
French respondents indicated that enhanced GM foods were no more useful to them than first generation
GMOs. This may be explained when taking respondents majors into account. As most French respondents
were agriculture majors, they might have seen the potential benefits and costs for farmers with first
generation GMOs as well as the potential benefits and costs for consumers with second generation
GMOs, and not only focused on enhanced GM foods that bring direct benefits to consumers. The
American respondents (mostly non-agriculture majors) on the other hand, paid more attention to enhanced
GM foods with direct benefits for consumers.
To summarize, both first and second generation GMOs are better accepted by American respondents than
by French respondents, but respondents in both countries perceive a direct benefit from second generation
GM foods with a least a less negative attitude toward them, which suggests that consumers in both
countries may be more likely to accept these new products in light of potential improved nutritional
benefits.
One limitation of this study is that data collected from university students may have shortcomings in
terms of the generalization of findings to consumers as a whole. However, in a study of Dutch consumers,
Laros and Steenkamp (2004) found no strong relationship between consumer’s sociodemographic
makeup and fear of GM foods. Another limitation is the differing majors of the respective student
samples. As mentioned earlier most French respondents were agriculture majors, while the American
respondents were mostly non-agriculture majors. This limitation resulted from the available opportunity
to collect data from these two groups. The French agricultural students’ view of GM food was not
positive. Yet, as agricultural majors they might be expected to be somewhat more sympathetic toward
GM foods. Therefore, in terms of French students in general and French consumers as a whole, it would
seem reasonable to expect a more negative view.
To overcome these limitations, for future research, it would be advisable to sample consumers across
different generational segments, educational levels, income levels and to collect larger samples of
comparable occupations to be able to generalize the current results. Future research might also refine and
replicate this study across other countries.
Our research findings highlight the fact that the case for acceptance or rejection of GM food is not black
and white, it can be colored in shades of gray. From a policy standpoint, this research supports the view
that consumers should be provided with scientifically validated information regarding not only the costs
but also the benefits associated with GM food products. A balanced picture including both the pros and
cons of GM foods should be provided to consumers in order to allow informed decision making not only
in terms of their purchasing behavior but also in terms of the exercise of their civic responsibilities,
particularly in the area of regulation. This information may be provided by manufactures, retailers, trade
associations, public interest organizations and/or through government agencies.
REFERENCES
Bourdieu, P. 1984. Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgment of Taste, London, United Kingdom:
Routlege and Kegan Paul.
Briand-Bouthiaux, A. 2001. OGM, Brevets Pour l’Inconnu (GMO: Patent For the Unknown, Dijon,
France: Editions Faton.
Buzby, J. C. and R. C. Ready. 1996. “Do Consumers Trust Food-Safety Information?” Food Review, Vol.
18 No. 1, January-April, pp.46-49.
Chevallier, C. 2002. Les OGM dans Notre Assiette (GMOs on Our Plate), Paris, France: Sang de la Terre.
22
Cummins, R. and B. Lilliston 2000. Genetically Engineered Food: A Self-Defense Guide for Consumers,
New, York, NY: Marlowe and Company.
Darden, W. R. and W. D. Perreault 1975. “A Multivariate Analysis of Media Exposure and Vacation
Behavior with Life-Style Covariates.” Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 2, September, pp. 93103.
Darmency, H. 1997. “Possibilités de Croisements entre Cultures Transgéniques et Plantes Sauvages”
(Crossing Possibilities between Transgenic Crops and Wild Plants). Génie Génétique: Les
Chercheurs et les Citoyens sexpriment, No. 4, December, pp. 103-105.
Ekici, A. 2000. “The Impact of Biotechnology on U.S. Farming, and U.S. Consumers Potential Reactions
to Genetically Modified Food Products.” In C. J. Shultz and B. Grbac (Eds.), Marketing
Contributions to Democratization and Socioeconomic Development: 25th Annual
Macroeconomic Conference, Lovran, Croatia, June 22-25, pp. 88-102.
Fisk, G. 1981. “An Invitation to Participate in Affairs of the Journal of Macromarketing.” Journal of
Macromarketing, 1, (Spring), pp.3-6.
Fisk, G. and R.W. Nason 1982. “Working Definition of Macromarketing.” Journal of Macromarketing, 2,
(Spring), pp.3-4.
Habert, P. 1994. “Le Génie Génétique Teste dans les Champs” (Field Testing for Genetic Engineering).
La Recherche, Vol. XXV No. 270, November, pp. 1126-1132.
Harlander, S. 2001. “A U.S. Food Industry Perspective on Biotechnology.” In the Second Join
Symposium on Food Safety and Nutrition: Current Issues in Food Biotechnology, July 11-13. On
line. Available: http://www.jifsan.umd.edu/ Harlander/harl-abs.htm. Accessed April 14, 2002.
Ho, M. W. 1998. Genetic Engineering: Dream or Nightmare? Bath, United Kingdom: Gateway Books.
Hoban, T. J. 1996. “Trends in Consumer Acceptance and Awareness of Biotechnology.” Journal of Food
Distribution Research, Vol. 27 No. 1, pp. 1-10.
Hofstede, G. 1994. “The Business of International Business is Culture.” International Business Review,
No. 1 Vol. 3, pp. 1-14.
INRA. 1998. “Quel Avenir pour le Bt?” (What Future for Bt?). Courier de lenvironnement de lINRA, No.
35, November, pp. 25-32.
INRA, ECOSA. 2000. “Eurobarometer 52.1: The Europeans Biotechnology Report By Managed and
Organized by the Directorate-General for Education and Culture “Citizens Center.” (Public
Opinion Analysis Unit), 15 March.
Jackson, L. A., and T. Villinski 2002. “Reaping What We Sow. Emerging Issues and Policy Implications
of Agricultural Biotechnology.” Review of Agricultural Economics, Vol.24 No. 1, SpringSummer, pp. 3-14.
Keegan, W. J. and M. C. Green 2003. Global Marketing, 3rd edition, Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson
Education.
23
Kelly, J. 1995. “Public Perceptions of Genetic Engineering: Australia 1994.” Final Report to the
Department of Industry, Science and Technology, Canberra, Australia: International Social
Science Survey.
Kemp, R. 1992. “Social Implications and Public Confidence: Risk Perception and Communication.” In D.
E. Stewart-Tull and M. Sussman (Eds.), The Release of Genetically Modified Microorganisms,
New York: Plenum Press, pp. 99-114.
Le Marre, Klervi, Marko Grünhagen and Gary J. Wells 2004. “Regulating Genetically Modified Foods in
the European Union and the United States: A Comparison of Public Policy and Marketing
Implications”, Marketing and Public Policy Conference, Salt Lake City, UT, Session 7.2, 170173.
Laros, F.J.M. and J.B.E.M. Steenkamp 2004. “Importance of Fear in the Case of Genetically Modified
Food.” Psychology and Marketing, Vol.21, No.11, November, pp. 889-908.
McHughen, A. 2000. Pandoras Picnic Basket: The Potential and the Hazards of Genetically Modified
Foods, New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
McIntosh, W. A. 1996. Sociologies for Food and Nutrition, New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Nelson, G. C. 2001. Genetically Modified Organisms in Agriculture, Economics and Politics, San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Norton, J., G. Lawrence and G. Wood 1998. “The Australian Publics Perception of Genetically
Engineered Foods.” Australasian Biotechnology, Vol. 8 No.3, pp. 172-181.
Nottingham, S. 1998. Eat Your Genes: How Genetically Modified Food Is Entering Our Die, New York,
NY: Zed Books Ltd.
Office of Technology Assessment 1987. “New Developments in Biotechnology: Public Perception of
Biotechnology.” Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Office of Technology Assessment 1992. “A New Technological Era for American Agriculture.”
Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Paillotin, G. 1999. “Faut-il Avoir Peur du Génie Génétique?” (Is Genetic Engineering To Be Afraid Of?).
Lusine Nouvelle, No. 1628, March 25.
Pedhazur, E. J. 1982. Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research, 2nd ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart and
Wintson.
Perrier, J. J. 1999. “Vers de Nouveaux Gènes Sélecteurs?” (Towards New Selective Genes?). Biofutur,
No. 192, September, pp. 22-26.
Philippon, P. 1999. “Aliments Transgéniques: le Doute sInstalle” (Transgenic Foods: Doubt is Arising).
Biofutur, No. 192, September, pp. 16-20.
Ritzer, G. 1996. The McDonalization of Society: An Investigation into the Changing Character of
Contemporary Social Life, Thousand Oaks, CA, Pine Forge Press.
24
Scalla, R. 1997. “Biotransformation des Herbicides par les PlantesTransgéniques Résistantes”
(Biotransformation of Herbicides by Resistant Transgenic Plants). OCL, Vol. 4 No. 2, MarchApril, pp. 113-119.
Seralini, G. E. 2000. “OGM, le Vrai Débat” (GMO, the True Debate). Evreux, France: Dominos
Flammarion.
Shapiro, S. 1984. “Looking Backward and Ahead.” Journal of Macromarketing, 4, (Spring), pp.2-3.
Smith, D. and P. Riethmuller 1999. “Consumer Concerns about Food Safety in Australia and Japan.”
International Journal of Social Economics, Vol. 26 No. 6, pp. 724-741.
Spector, P. E. 1977. “What to Do with Significant Multivariate Effects in Multivariate Analyses of
Variance.” Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 62 No. 2, pp. 158-163.
Tangley, L. 1999. “How safe is Genetically Modified Food?” U.S. News, July 26, p. 12.
Vincendon, S. 2000. “Les Français et les OGM: Pas Touche à Mon Assiette: Sondage et article réalisé
pour libération” (French People and GMO: Dont Touch My Plate). Ifop-Libération, August 3.
Vogt, D. U. and M. Parish 1999. “Food Biotechnology in the United States: Science, Regulation, and
Issues.” Washington DC: Congressional Research Service. On line. Available:
http://usinfo.state.gov/topical/global/biotech/crsfood.htm. Accessed May 16, 2002.
Wal, J. M. 1997. “Gènes, Transgènes, Allergènes” (Genes, Transgenes, Allergenes). INRA Mensuel, No.
93, September, pp. 9-10.
Warde, A. 1997. Consumption, Food, and Tastes, London, United Kingdom: Sage Publications.
Wells, Gary J., Klervi Le Marre, Marko Grünhagen, John G. Surak and Stephen E. Miller 2004. “French
and American Consumer Perceptions of Risks Associated with Genetically Modified Foods”,
Southeast Decision Sciences Institute Proceedings, Charleston, SC, 47-49.
Wolf, M. M. and C. Domegan 2000. “A Comparison of Consumer Attitudes Toward Genetically
Modified Food in Europe and the United States: A Case Study Over Time.” Paper presented to
the 4th International Conference on the Economics of Agricultural Biotechnology, International
Consortium on Agricultural Biotechnology, Ravello, Italy, August 24-28.
Young, A. L. 1992. “An International Perspective on Biotechnology and Nutrition.” In D. D. Bills and S.
D. Kung (Eds.), Biotechnology and Nutrition: Proceedings of the Third International Symposium,
Stoneham, MA: Butterworth-Heinemann, pp. 5-12.
25
__________________________________________________
Relationships Among Individual, Institutional, and System
Level Public Trust: A Case for Consumer Evaluations of
Food Safety
Ahmet Ekici, Bilkent University
Linda L. Price, University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Extended Abstract
Many of the most challenging macro issues of our century hang on the problem of public trust. Despite
the central importance of public trust to macromarketing and public policy research, relatively little is
known about the origins and relationships between various types/levels of public trust. For example, it is
not clear why consumers trust their own grocery stores and yet distrust grocery stores in general in
providing safe food. Further, it is not clear why as consumers distrust food retailers, regulatory agencies,
law-makers, food manufacturers, scientists, they still trust the food system to supply them safe food. Our
paper provides a unique perspective for understanding the origins of public trust and relationships
between various levels of public trust.
Specifically we investigate the underpinnings of American consumers’ trust in individual/local
establishments, as well as in institutions and in systems as the larger collectives of locals with respect to
food safety, biotechnology and genetically modified foods (GMF) as public trust has been argued as the
central issue for the policy debates and acceptability of these new food products (Juannillo 2001;
McGarity and Hansen 2001). Understanding public trust can provide insights into the resolution of the
policy debates surrounding GMF, and any resolution on the debates would help clearing the cloud over
the future of GMF and other biotechnology applications. Further, understanding consumer perceptions of
this technology can have important global (macro) ramifications. Agriculture and food applications of
biotechnology promise solutions for feeding an ever-growing global population. This promise is
particularly important to underdeveloped countries where there is an immediate need not only for
increasing productivity in agricultural output but also for nutrition-rich food. American consumers’ view
of genetically modified foods are particularly important as consumers’ views in developed countries
(where most biotechnology research is conducted) are likely to affect the extent to which public and
private organizations commit to biotech research. For example, consumer reactions toward GMF in many
European countries have reduced funding for biotech research over the last years (ISIS Press Release,
2001). Private organizations conduct and therefore control most biotechnology research and its outputs
(including various consumer products). Even though many underdeveloped countries may look favorably
upon food and agriculture applications of biotechnology, it will still be in the hands of consumers in
developed countries to encourage or discourage biotechnology research.
We focused on food safety beliefs in general and beliefs about the safety of biotechnology and genetically
modified foods in particular because public trust is believed to play a central role in shaping these beliefs.
Public trust in the safety of food is intriguing to study as food safety has been under attack from multiple
angles during the age of (bio)technology. First, the paradoxical nature of technology (i.e. making food
both safe and unsafe at the same time, e.g. Mick and Fournier 1998; Boorsteen 1978) coupled with a
number of food related scares (e.g. the mad cow disease) left consumers with a lot of distress and
resulting distrust in the food supply (Pennington 1999; Marris et. al, 1996). Second, agricultural statistics
show a big drop in the number of family-owned farming/food production and increased collection of
26
once-family owned farms in the hands of a fewer number of corporate agribusiness operations (Hobbelink
1991; Kloppenburg 1988). In addition, since the mid-1980s transnational chemical companies have
acquired many seed companies with interests in pesticides and pharmaceuticals. These vertical integration
attempts are expected to have direct effects on U.S farm ownership structure and indirect effects on
citizen’s beliefs about food safety. In other words, as food production collects into the hands of fewer
companies (instead of a greater number of families) consumers appear less comfortable. As sarcastically
explained by Phelan (1998) “the big wheel of agricultural competitiveness grinds on, concentrating
resources in the hands of fewer and fewer people, crushing small farmers and small businesses,
threatening the environment and changing the countryside and a food production system that has made a
useful contribution to society over decades”. Changes in the farming structure are making citizens more
concerned about the environment, animal welfare and food safety (Phelan 1998). Third, Nestle (2002)
notes that food and agriculture-related departments at public and private colleges and universities are
eagerly seeking funding from corporations for research, equipment, facilities, etc. The author questions
the objectivity and “independence” of such research. The author contents that increased use of “exclusive
contracts” are adding to the concerns over food safety. (Nestle 2002, p.120, italic is in original).
Consumer views of food related outbreaks and scares, trends in the composition of the farming industry,
involvement of the commodity boards and corporations in research in agriculture and food science,
relationships between regulatory agencies, universities, and corporation, quality and quantity of the food,
nutrition, and health related news reports in the media are likely to inform us about public trust in food
safety.
Public trust has been studied largely at the institutional level where the focus has been to understand the
citizens’ trust in specific social institutions (e.g. Lazarus 1991; McGarity 1986; Rampton and Stauber
2001; Renn and Levine 1991; Abbott and Dalton 1999). So far, the focus in understanding public
attitudes toward GMF has also been limited at the institutional level-which we find shortsighted. In order
to understand public trust in food safety and in the safety of GMF, one should investigate citizen’s views
of various individual establishments (e.g. family farms) and institutions (e.g. farmers in general) through a
holistic approach. Approaching public trust through a systems perspective (Patton 1990) is likely to
inform us about the relationships between various levels of public trust. Further, we believe that
understanding the relationships between consumers’ evaluations of social institutions and systems are
likely to shed light on policy debates, and thus shape the future of genetically modified foods.
As we noted, the relationships between various levels of public trust are paradoxically conflicting.
Various polls conducted over the past few decades suggest that the public has generally positive
perceptions of individual politicians (such as congressional representatives) and quite negative
perceptions of political institutions (such as the Congress). These perceptions are not limited to politics.
For example, people’s overall assessments of their own doctors are systematically more positive than
their assessments of doctors in general (Jacobs and Shapiro 1994). Despite negative evaluations by the
public at the institutional level, the polls over the same period of time suggest that collectively, across
institutions, public trust is still very high. The great majority of people find the United States the best
place to live; and have confidence in the political system, the health system, the food safety system, etc
(e.g. Cooper 1999; Muntz and Flemming 1999).
Although social scientists (mostly in the area of political science) have attempted to explain the
paradoxical conflict between individual and institutional levels of public trust (e.g. Asher and Barr 1994;
Cook 1979; Fennon 1975; Muntz and Flemming 1999; Ripley et al. 1992), there has been no attempt to
explain the relationship between individual, institutional, and system levels of public trust. In other
words, we do not know how citizens’ evaluations at the local level affect their evaluations of systems. In
addition, we do not know how negative evaluations at the institutional level can collect into positive
evaluations at the system level. The purpose of this article is to understand the origins and the
relationships between institutional and system level public trust in the context of food safety and
27
consumer evaluations of genetically modified foods. We introduce the idea that consumers use “balancing
strategies,” in that their trust in one institution can offset their lack of trust in other institutions, and as a
result, they feel safe about the food they consume. In addition, public trust in the food safety system may
depend on the perceived capabilities of the most powerful institution of the system, and/or perceived
negative and positive interactions between component social institutions that form the food safety system.
We also explore the possibilities that institutional and/or system levels of public trust do not matter in
consumers’ beliefs about food safety.
In this paper, we first set out to conceptualize public trust at three different levels, individual, institutional,
and system levels. Then we review the prior research related to public trust. After a brief overview of
research on public (institutional) trust, we specifically focus on the research that deals with the
paradoxical disparity between various levels of public trust. We then provide an overview of our research
context and activities that took place between 1999-2003 in the Midwest. After reporting our findings, we
conclude with a general discussion of contributions and implications for macromarketing, and offer
directions for future research.
References
Abbott, Alison, and Rex Dalton (1999), “Science Comes to Terms with the Lessons of Fraud,” Nature,
398, 6722, 13-17.
Asher, Herb B., and Mark Barr (1994), “Popular Support for Congress and Its Members,” in
Congress, the Press, and the Public, ed. Thomas E. Mann and Norman J. Ornstein, Washington,
DC: American Enterprise Institute/Brookings Institution, 15-44.
Boorstin, Daniel J. (1978), The Republic of Technology, New York: Harper and Row.
Cook, T.J. (1979), “Legislature Versus Legislator: A note on the Paradox of Congressional
Support,” Legislative Studies Quarterly, 4, 43-61.
Cooper, Joseph (1999), Congress and the Decline of Public Trust, Westview Press.
Fennon, Richard F. Jr. (1975), “If, as Ralph Nader Says, Congress Id ‘the Broken Branch,’
How Come We Love Our Congressmen So Much?” in Congress in Charge: Evolution and
Reform, ed. Norman J. Ornstein, New York: Preager, 277-287.
Hobbelink, Henk (1991), Biotechnology and the Future of the World Agriculture,
London: Zed Books.
ISIS Press Release (2001), http://www.connectotel.com/gmfood/is260101.txt
Jacobs, L.R. and R.Y. Shapiro (1994), “Questioning the Conventional Wisdom on Public
Opinion Toward Health Reform,” PS: Political Science and Politics, 27, 208-214.
Juannillo, Napolean K. Jr. (2001), “The Risks and Benefits of Agricultural Biotechnology,”
American Behavioral Scientist, 44 (8), 1267-1282.
Kloppenburg, Jack Ralph, Jr. (1988), First the Seed: The Political Economy of Plant
Biotechnology 1492-2000, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
28
Lazarus, R.J. (1991), “The Tragedy of Distrust in the Implementation of Federal
Environmental Law,” Law and Contemporary Problems, 54, 311-374.
Marris, C., I. Langford, and T. O’Riordan (1996), “Integrating Sociological and
Psychological Approaches to Public Perceptions of Environmental Risks: Detailed Results From
a Questionnaire Survey,” CSERGE Working Paper GEC, School of Environmental Sciences,
University of East Anglia, Norwich.
McGarity, Thomas (1986) “Risk and Trust: The Role of Regulatory Agencies,”
Environmental Law Reporter, 16, 10198-10229.
McGarity, Thomas and Patricia I. Hansen (2001), “Breeding Distrust,” Report prepared
for the Food Policy Institute of the Consumer Federation of America.
Mick, David Glen and Susan Fournier (1998), “Paradoxes of Technology: Consumer
Cognizance, Emotions, and Coping Strategies,” Journal of Consumer Research, 25, September,
123-143.
Muntz, Diana C. and Gregory N. Flemming (1999), “How Good People Make Bad
Collectives,” in Congress and the Decline of Public Trust, ed. Cooper, Joseph (1999), Westview
Press.
Nestle, Marion (2002), Food Politics: How the Food Industry Influences Nutrition and
Health, University of California Press.
Patton, Michael Quinn (1990), Qualitative Evaluation and Research Methods, 2nd ed.,
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Pennington, Hugh (1999), “The Media and Trust: E-coli and Other Cases,” in Risk
Communication and Public Health, Peter Bennett and Kenneth Calman, eds. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Rampton, Sheldon and John Stauber (2001), Trust Us, We’re Experts!, New York:
Penguin Putnam, Inc.
Renn, Ortwin and D. Levine (1991), “Credibility and Risk in Risk Communication,” in
Communicating Risk to the Public: International Perspectives, R.E. Kasperson and P.M. Stallen,
eds., Amsterdam: Kluwer.
Ripley, R.B., S.C. Patterson, L. Maurer, and S.V. Quinlan (1992), “Constituents’ Evaluations
of U.S. House Members,” American Politics Quarterly, 20: 442-456.
29
What Marketing Does to Family
James W. Gentry, University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Stacey M. Baker, University of Wyoming
Abstract
Marketing and consumer research has placed insufficient emphasis on the family as the unit of analysis.
Further, and more pertinent to this paper, marketing (and marketing academics) have paid even less
attention to what marketing does to the family. While “family issues” have seemingly been captured by
the political right wing in the US, there is concern across the political spectrum as to the deterioration of
family relationships. We discuss a number of areas in which marketers’ opportunistic behaviors or
benign neglect have had a deleterious effect on family, and argue that there is need within marketing to
investigate the issues, hopefully with the result of enhancing social marketing’s influence on appropriate
industry norms.
Introduction
Consumer research has a long (and relatively wretched) history of work on how to market to the US
household (Commuri and Gentry 2000). However, there has been little concern in the marketing literature
as to what marketing does to the family. The intent of this paper is to foster discussion of this topic.
To be sure, work in social marketing has dealt systematically with marketing-generated harm, but the
focus has been on individuals almost exclusively. This makes sense to some extent given that the US is
the most individualistic nation in the world; we are a country of individuals. Nonetheless, scholars such
Alderson (1957) have focused on the household as the basis for consumer behavior. Not only are
individuals affected by marketing phenomena, but obviously their close-knit collectives are as well. This
paper will argue that more work is needed to sensitize marketers to the harm done to the emotional center
of most civilizations, the family. “Families are the fundamental building block of society, and being in
families is a fundamental human trait” (Kennedy 2000, p. 1).
We will look at several areas in which marketing and the family interface, review literature in those areas,
and suggest needed research extensions.
Life Event Transitions
Marketing has consistently focused on trajectories in lives, given the field’s preference for steady-state
perspectives. However, transitions in one’s life often provide incentive for change, and thus opportunities
to attract new business (Andreasen 1984). Consumer research has dealt with topics such as immigration
(Metha and Belk 1991; Penalosa 1994; Wallendorf and Reilly 1983), moving (Belk 1992), job loss
(Munton 1990), divorce (McAlexander, Schouten, and Roberts 1993; Rindfleisch, Burroughs, and
Denton 1997; Schouten 1991), going to college (Silver 1996), the birth of a child (Fischer and Gainer
1993), the death of a loved one (Bonsu and Belk 2003; Gentry et al. 1994, 1995), disasters (Sayre 1994),
and adjustment to learning one has cancer (Pavia and Mason 2004). All of the transitions have
implications for family; some (divorce, death, birth of a child) change the composition of the family itself.
However, the focus of the research has largely been on individuals and has ignored the resulting changes
in family dynamics.
30
Even those investigating the family implications of the life-event transition (for example, Gentry et al.
1995; Munton 1990) covered changes in the family without much discussion of the role of the marketing
system during the life-event transition. Gentry et al. (1995) did discuss instances when service providers
interacted positively with the survivors of the death of a close loved one in a positive manner (usually the
funeral industry) or a few instances in a negative manner (the medical and insurance industries), but their
informants were more into decision avoidance than into decision making per se and, consequently, did not
appear to be very observant of interactions with the market place.
Baker, Gentry, and Rittenberg (2005) argue that consumers undergoing life-event transitions are
experiencing “situational vulnerability” due largely to the newness of their circumstances. There is a
short-run “opportunity” for marketers to take advantage of this situation; at the same time, there is likely
to be an opportunity to develop a long-time relationship with the consumer if understanding is shown at
this sensitive time. But the limited coverage of this issue again has had an individual focus, without much
consideration of the changing family dynamics. For example, all members of a family will grieve the loss
of a loved one, but people grieve in very different manners. A marketer’s well-intentioned reaching out to
help one family member may actually turn out to be divisive for the family as a whole.
A focus on the family (and its underlying dynamics) as the unit of analysis in this area of social marketing
is clearly needed. Transitions require the family and its individual members to redefine the self, the unit,
and perhaps even their “sense of family.” Marketing efforts that facilitate this redefinition process could
represent a positive social consequence of marketing.
The Promotion of Excessive Consumption
Marketers are aware of the downsides of consuming to excess, and academe has a stream of research on
compulsive consumption (O’Guinn and Faber 1991) and, to a much less extent, on addiction (Hirschman
1992). Obesity seems to be the “evil” of the current day, but excessive drinking, drug use, and gambling
(and even smoking at all) also receive much attention, especially in the popular press. Again, though,
most of the focus is on the individual involved and not on the subsequential family processes. To be sure,
excessive consumption can play a role in the breaking down of family ties (Belk et al. 1991; Stephens,
Hill, and Gentry 2005), but most of the marketing literature has ignored this issue.
Marketers do de-market excessive consumption with campaigns such as the “drink responsibly” ones, but
again the focus tends to be more on individuals and/or friends (“Friends don’t let friends drive drunk”),
and not family. The maintenance of harmony within the family and the nurturing of the health of loved
ones are important themes that should be used to deter excessive consumption. One of the first author’s
favorite anti-smoking ads showed a father and son playing, and then the two of them drop down at the
base of a tree. The father pulls out a cigarette, and the son asks for one too. The only verbal message in
the ad is then heard, “Like father, like son.”
Another example is the current campaign run by the Office of National Drug Control Policy/Partnership
for a Drug-Free America that provides a written message from a teenager to a parent, with the parts
revealing their drug or alcohol usage crossed out. One interpretation is that the child only tells the parent
the innocuous part of the message; another interpretation is that the parent is hearing only the innocuous
part. In any case, the underlying message is that parents need to listen and to probe beneath the surface in
terms of their child’s communication.
The advertising done by the Church of the Latter-Day Saints also consistently takes a strong family
perspective. Note, however, that these examples are not promoting products per se, but rather are of the
“public service” genre. Admittedly, there are exceptions such as the “Kodak moments/creating memories”
ads offering a means of capturing family joy for future reminiscence, but why should such ads be the
31
exception? Further, there is little literature within marketing discussing advertising and its effects on
family relations.
The Provision of Normative Roles
Media, ultimately paid for through the marketing system, provide the world with biased perceptions of
“others.” We come to “know” others through the stereotypes we infer from the media. We know that
television is not real life, but it taints our perceptions nonetheless. TV shows, movies, and books provide
us with definitions of right and wrong, many in conflict with messages sent from parents to children on
the matter. To some extent, the marketing system can excuse itself by saying that we only sponsor the
shows because they are what our consumers wish to see, but the content of commercials cannot be
attributed elsewhere. And, quite frankly, many commercials do not coincide with anyone’s sense of
family values.
Whether it is Joe the Camel or some other attractive nuisance used to appeal to young consumers in the
intent of encouraging illegal (for them) adult pleasures, we are aware of steady criticisms of sin-industry
ads aimed at children. Even benign products like cereal use appeals teaching children to be rebellious; for
example, consider the cereal ad showing two young children talking (and apparently communicating)
with their mouths full of cereal and sending the message that the universal admonition by parents to not
talk with their mouths full may be disregarded. Also, consider all of the ads that say “Tell your parents
you want to ….” TELL your parents, don’t ask them.
The process of becoming independent from parents is a complex one, and at times a rebellious one.
Taking advantage of that latent (or sometimes quite blatant) sense of rebellion by advertisers is a practice
illegal per se in many nations, but merely a disgusting one in the US. This is one area in which the family
has been the unit of study, which makes much sense as the protection of the young is probably the most
fundamental function of family.
On the other hand, there are areas in which the individual focus is still predominant. There is concern that
the use of unnaturally thin models in ads may create unrealistic preferred body images for adolescent and
even pre-adolescent females (Martin and Gentry 1997; Stephens, Hill, and Hansen 1994). The marketing
system sends the message to young females that “thin is in” just when nature is telling them that their
bodies are going to expand. The extreme response to this confusing set of mixed messages is anorexia
and/or bulimia. While there is a growing literature on this problem area, the focus is on the individual and
not on the gut-wrenching family dynamics played out when the young female has to separate from her
family and go into rehabilitation.
Another concern is the media’s presentation of male and female gender roles in commercials. With the
entry of middle-class females in large numbers into the work force in the 1970s and 1980s, one might
expect that household roles would have changed. After all, more than one-third of US wives earn more
than their spouses, calling into question the traditional “male provider” premise. What the family
literature (Commuri and Gentry 2000) tells us is that less housework is being done today, but men are not
doing any more in absolute terms (men are doing more in relative terms, though, as women are doing
less). Men still get sanctioned for displaying parental perspectives in inappropriate venues, such as the
workplace (Coltrane 1989). The male ego is a barrier to true egalitarianism in the home, and the
marketing system perpetuates traditional norms. Men do cook meals for the family and many cook quite
well, but the competent male in the household is not visible in television commercials, where the male
cooking role is limited to opening a can or using the microwave.
Also disturbing are the gender roles evident in children’s commercials. At a time when political
correctness is so prominent in the adult world, how is it possible that the violent boy and nurturing girl
commercials are still so evident? Boys and girls have few physical (size) differences until part-way
32
through elementary school; why segment them so differently in their formative years? Why is there such
clear segmentation of girl toys and boy toys? Is there fear that gender neutral commercials will be seen as
promoting the development of gay and lesbian young people? Would that be worse than the continuance
of male violence within the family?
Additionally, normative family structures depicted in advertisements may not be representative of the
reality that exists within many families. When family members view advertisements, they may use the ads
as a basis of social comparison for their own family. For example, Baker and Gentry (2005) report on an
interview with a woman who feels vulnerable when she sees ads depicting a ‘typical’ or intact family
(mother, father, two kids). The stereotypical depiction of family elicits guilt for her divorce, which she
blames largely on herself, including its effect on her children, and also evokes self-blame for not being a
‘soccer Mom’ with a van anymore (she could not afford the payments) and for not living the role she
envisioned for herself for most of her life. To this woman, advertisements depicting an intact family
represent the way her life ought to be and her feelings of guilt and self-blame affect family dynamics.
Similarly, children may view the wealth depicted in commercials or on television and wonder why their
families do not live like that, and they may even engage in illegal behaviors to try to move closer to what
they perceive as the norm (Ozanne, Hill, and Wright 1998). Also, for families where “face” is an
important value (e.g., in many Asian American families), shame may come to the family when the family
structure and family experiences do not meet with social norms.
Messages sent to the family and its members suggest that it is necessary to be young, beautiful, rich,
surrounded by friends and material wealth, and capable of buying whatever one wants with no delayed
gratification. So much for promoting the American work ethic and the message that hard work pays off. If
American family values are indeed shifting away from such an ethic, advertising messages seem to play
no small role in this shift.
The Family Friendliness of Servicescapes
How do retail servicescapes “communicate” whether different types of families are welcome? Are
families with kids welcome at all? Americans are well aware of some apartment complexes’ disallowance
of pets, but as the US ages, we are seeing “no kid” policies as well. As Baby Boomers reach retirement
and start downsizing from houses to apartments, such policies are likely to increase. What message does
that policy send to grandparents who might like to have grandchildren visit?
Baker, Holland, and Kaufman-Scarborough (2005) report the problems of a young mother with mobility
impairment who encountered problems shopping with her young daughter. She felt that a special bonding
process was being denied her. Servicescapes and society set the ‘norm’ that this is what mothers and
daughters do. They shop. What happens when the environment isn’t conducive to making this happen?
Certainly many low-margin, high-volume operations intentionally structure the retail environment so as to
be somewhat uncomfortable and to increase turnover. But should this be the case in terms of outlets with
a focus on children (and thus on families)?
Elements of the servicescape (e.g., store layout, signage, assortment) indicate whether an individual (or a
family) is welcome or not (Baker, Holland, and Kaufman-Scarborough 2005). There are many examples
of family friendly servicescapes. Grocery stores with carts in the shape of cars, play areas in malls, beach
sand boxes in mall parking lots, restaurants with kid meals, crayons, balloons, and so forth, all
communicate that kids are expected and welcome. An orthodontist in Toledo has a video arcade; parents
going to him do not have to fight their kids to get them to see him. Many of these elements help reduce
stress (the shopping has to get done or the orthodontist needs to be seen), and these offerings promote
harmony within the family.
33
There are also many examples of far less family-friendly servicescapes. Sacks of candy stacked at child
eye level in grocery stores can turn an unpleasant shopping experience into a horrendous one. Pet toys and
treats are usually place higher on shelves, in part because the heavier pet food bags are rightfully placed
lower. But, many elderly and young people have injured themselves as they fell while climbing up to
reach the treats for their special (extended) family member. Some stores are aware of such issues and
offer “candy free” check out lanes or assistance in acquiring products, but many do not. How the
servicescape promotes family unity and bonding (or not) and how it communicates acceptance of the
family (or not) is an area ripe for future research.
Family Planning
Babies and puppies are cuddly objects used extensively to generate warm emotions among those seeing
the advertisement. Who wouldn’t want one? Maybe we should ask, “Who shouldn’t want one?” The
authors’ white, middle class values suggest that the answer to the last question is young teenagers and,
especially, pre-teens. Parents know the awesome responsibility associated with the care of an infant, but
that is not part of the messages associated with the use of babies to promote products.
One area that the media has largely ignored is that of abortion. Over 20% of women in the US have had
an abortion, but such topics are rarely touched upon in television programs. The interface with family
issues is obvious, as we are considering the formation or expansion of one (or not). Societal concern has
focused on whether the abortion act should be legal and whether the potential mother’s parents have to
provide consent for the act. Consumer research has provided definitive insight that the mental health of
the expectant woman will be greater in the long run if she is allowed to make the decision herself
(regardless of its nature) (Patterson, Hill, and Maloy 1995). Clearly a family focus is mandatory in this
context, and just as clearly the results indicate that the individual most affected should be the decision
maker.
Social marketers have been involved in attempts to curb teen and other unwanted pregnancies. However,
to be truly effective, social marketing attempts need to be based on an understanding of all influencers,
including the family and advertising, that impact decisions to have sex and/or engage in family planning.
Future research that discusses these issues could contribute to social marketing efforts.
Conclusion
Social marketing has experienced a resurgence in the last decade and a half, of which we are very pleased.
However, more emphasis on “social” is needed, as the implications of marketing actions on the family
have been greatly understudied. Macromarketing is the sub-discipline within marketing most concerned
with the societal implications of marketing action, yet it has paid very little attention to society’s basic
building block, the family. Given the problem which the family is having in the US (we like family so
much that many of us have two or three of them), our discipline must generate research to sensitize our
less perceptive brethren to the harm that we do to this revered institution.
References
Alderson, Wroe (1957), “The Motivation of Consumer Buying,” Chapter 7, Marketing Behavior and
Executive Action, Homewood, IL; Irwin.
Andreasen, Alan R. (1984), “Life Status Changes and Changes in Consumer Preferences and
Satisfaction,” Journal of Consumer Research, 11 (December), 784-794.
Baker, Stacey Menzel and James W. Gentry (2005), “Vulnerability: It’s Not Just for the ‘Other,’”
Working Paper, University of Wyoming.
34
Baker, Stacey Menzel, James W. Gentry, and Terri L. Rittenberg (2005), “Building Understanding of the
Domain of Consumer Vulnerability,” Journal of Macromarketing, Forthcoming.
Baker, Stacey Menzel, Jonna Holland, and Carol Kaufman-Scarborough (2005), “How Consumers with
Disabilities Perceive ‘Welcome’ in Retail Settings: A Critical Incident Study,” Working Paper,
University of Wyoming.
Belk, Russell W. (1992), “Moving Possessions: An Analysis Based on Personal Documents from the
1847-1869 Mormon Migrations,” Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (December), 339-361.
Belk, Russell W., Melanie Wallendorf, John Sherry, and Morris Holbrook (1991), “Collecting in a
Consumer Culture,” in Highways and Buyways: Naturalistic Research from the Consumer
Behavior Odyssey, Russell W. Belk (Ed.), Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 178215.
Bonsu, Sammy K. and Russell W. Belk (2003), “Do Not Go Cheaply into That Good Night: Death-Ritual
Consumption in Asante, Ghana” Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (June), 41-55.
Coltrane, Scott (1989), “Household Labor and the Routine Production of Gender,” Social Problems, 36,
473-490.
Commuri, Suraj and James W. Gentry (2000), “Opportunities for Family Research n Marketing,”
Academy of Marketing Science Review, http://amsreview.org/amsreview/theory/commuri0800.html.
Fischer, Eileen and Brenda Gainer (1993), “Baby Showers: A Rite of Passage in Transition,” Advances in
Consumer Research, 20, 320-324.
Gentry, James W., Patricia F. Kennedy, Catherine Paul, and Ronald Paul Hill (1994),
“The Vulnerability of Those Grieving the Death of a Loved One: Implications for Public Policy,”
Journal of Public Policy & Markeing, 14 (Fall), 128-142.
Gentry, James W., Patricia F. Kennedy, Catherine Paul, and Ronald Paul Hill (1995),
“Family Transitions During Grief:: Discontinuities in Household Consumption,” Journal of
Business Research, 34, 67-79.
Hirschman, Elizabeth C. (1992), “The Consciousness of Addiction: Toward a General Theory of
Compulsive Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (September), 155-179.
Kennedy, Jane (2000), “Family Law in 2000 and Beyond,” Second European Congress on Family Law,
www.dca.gov.uk/speeches/2000/jk240500.htm
Martin, Mary C. and James W. Gentry (1997), “Stuck in the Model Trap: The Effects of Beautiful Models
in Ads on Female Pre-adolescents and Adolescents,” Journal of Advertising, 26 (Summer), 19-34.
McAlexander, James H., John W. Schouten, and Scott D. Roberts (1993), “Consumer Behavior and
Divorce,” in Russell W. Belk (Ed.), Research in Consumer Research, 6, 231-260.
Metha, Raj and Russell W. Belk (1991), “Artifacts, Identity, and Transition: Favorite Possessions of
Indians and Indian Immigrants to the United States,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (March),
298-411.
35
Munton, Anthony G. (1990), “Job Relocating Stress, and the Family,” Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 11 (September), 401-406.
O’Guinn, Thomas C. and Ronald J. Faber (1991), “Mass Communication and Consumer Behavior,” in
Handbook of Consumer Behavior, eds. Thomas S. Robertson and Harold H. Kassarjian,
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 349-400.
Ozanne, Julie L., Ronald Paul Hill, and Newell D. Wright (1998), “Juvenile Delinquents’ Use of
Consumption as Cultural Resistance: Implications for Juvenile Reform Programs and Public
Policy,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 17 (2), 185-196.
Patterson, Maggie Jones, Ronald Paul Hill, and Kate Maloy (1995), “Abortions in America: A Consumer
Behavior Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (March), 677-694.
Pavia, Teresa M. and Marlys J. Mason (2004), “The Reflexive Relationship between Consumer Behavior
and Adaptive Coping,” Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (December), 441-454.
Penaloza, Lisa (1994), “Atravesando Fronteras / Border Crossings: A Critical Ethnographic Expiration of
the Consumer Acculturation of Mexican Immigrants,” Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (June),
32-54.
Rindfleisch, Aric, James E. Burroughs, and Frank Denton (1997), “Family Structure, Materialism, and
Compulsive Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (March), 312-325.
Sayre, S. (1994), “Possessions and Identity in Crises: Meaning and Change for Victims of the Oakland
Firestorm,” Advances in Consumer Research, 21, 109-114.
Schouten, John W. (1991), “Personal Rites of Passage and Reconstruction of Self,” Advances in
Consumer Research, 18, 49-51.
Silver, Ira (1996), “Role Transitions, Objects, and Identity,” Symbolic Interaction, 19 (1), 1-20.
Stephens, Deborah L., Ronald Paul Hill, and James W. Gentry (2005), “A Consumer-Behavior
Perspective on Intimate Partner Violence,” Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 34
(February), 36-67.
Stephens, Deborah L., Ronald Paul Hill, and C. Hansen (1994), “The Beauty Myth and Female
Consumers: The Controversial Role of Advertising,” The Journal of Consumer Affairs, 28 (1),
137-153.
Wallendorf, Melanie and Michael Reilly (1983), “Ethnic Migration, Assimilation and Consumption,”
Journal of Consumer Research. 10 (December), 292-302,
36
Father Involvement: Who’s Your Daddy and Why
Marketer’s Care
Kyle A. Huggins, University of Arkansas
______________________________________________________________________________
Father Involvement: Who’s Your Daddy and Why Marketers Care?
Much of consumer behavior begins to become engrained in each one of us at a very young age. From the
time we are born, we consume, and as we replicate this process, we begin to establish preferences for
certain items and brands of items. These preferences are reinforced by our relationships with friends,
family, and other acquaintances. One key relationship that is instrumental in the forming of these
preferences is the relationship that we form with our parents when we are children. Their ambition,
status, desires, and consumption processes begin to mold us from the time we come out of the womb. As
we grow, we as individuals begin to make decisions for ourselves. We choose to either follow their lead
or choose a different path, but we can not deny the fact that their influence, in some way, helps determine
the outcome of our decisions.
A problem for marketing researchers has developed over the last few decades as the traditional American
family has undergone tremendous structural changes. Divorce is at an all time high and more than half of
all American children will spend some time growing up in a single-parent family (Cherlin 1992). Not
only has divorce changed the family structure in America, but so has the rise in women taking a
significant role in the workforce outside of the home, which has ultimately changed the structure of
families that are still intact having not been victimized by divorce. Therefore, there remain very few
traditional family structures that have not been modified by change over the last thirty-five years.
An area of particular interest within consumer behavior is materialism. While the origination of this
phenomenon is unknown at this time, materialism has emerged as a topic of interest across a number of
disciplines. Materialism has continued to be an important research interest even while the family
structure in the United States has undergone significant changes. However, little research has been
conducted to show if and how materialism is linked to the changes within family structure, and more
specifically, if there is a particular parental gender role that produces more materialistic behavior in
offspring. Therefore, this paper seeks to fill that gap in current literature by hypothesizing that
materialistic attitudes in offspring are directly linked with the role of the father within the household,
explicitly theorizing that the lack of nurturant fathering in a child’s life leads to an increase in the child’s
materialistic attitudes. The paper will begin with a review on the role of gender within the household and
how these sociologically defined roles are shifting. In addition to this review, a specific look into the
father-child dyad will be evaluated. Next, a review of materialism as it is defined within a consumer
behavior context will be conducted. Finally, a model will be proposed for studying the male gender role
as a father within the household and its possible connection with materialistic attitudes present within our
culture.
The History of Gender Roles in the Traditional Household
Talcott Parsons (1949) took a functionalist viewpoint of the family when he positioned the traditional
family as one in which husbands embrace the responsibility for providing for the family long term while
wives embrace the instrumental task of meeting the everyday needs of family members (Gentry et al.
2003). However, Allen and Walker (2000) note that this perspective is based on a small portion of
37
American history which peaked in popularity at the height of the industrial era. They claim that prior to
the 1940s, wage work for women was completely non-existent for working class and minority women,
while it was concealed from the economy for middle-class women. Although this may have been true for
women before the 1940s, there was an obvious paradigm shift with the emergence of women in the
workforce during WWII. However, a backlash to the temporary empowerment of women occurred when
men returned home from the war causing women to return to their “place” in the home; and it was at the
point that the traditional “inside/outside” dichotomy of gender roles become prevalent in the United
States, as well as across the world (Gentry et al. 2003).
Gentry and his colleagues (2003) explain that this dichotomy transpired by the notion that women are
responsible for the inside of the home, while men are responsible for outside the home. This is based on
the assumption that women have a stronger link to young children due to the birth process and generally
have smaller physiques. Men are then left with the function of providing more instrumental roles such as
providing food on the table, handling financial matters, or, more recently, yard work. The authors
conclude that to a great extent, sex-differentiated roles have become perpetuated without being questioned
sufficiently as to their appropriateness to modern (or post-modern) society, but may simply form under
the implicit assumption that men and women embrace responsibilities consistent with respective
biologically based capabilities.
This assumption sustained the “inside/outside” theory until the late 1970s and early 1980s when women
started emerging back into the workforce. At this point, household decision making patterns began to
change all together. It was at this stage the functionalist perspective began to become questioned. Risman
(1998) stated that gender is a human invention which is subject to re-invention and re-creation.
Therefore, because of the social construction of gender, it is allowed to change within the context of
social constraints. Because of this belief, Sussman (1993) predicts that changes will never revert to the
old superordinate/subordinate pattern, but rather that equity and sharing will grow in both prevalence and
incidence in the upcoming years. Ferber and Birnbaum (1980) suggest that the sharing of housework will
also become more balanced allowing husbands more time in getting to know their children. In fact,
Gardyn (2000) shows that fathers in the 1990s did spend significantly more time with their children than
their fathers did with them when they were children. Likewise, there are some who believe that the
change that has been brought about by women returning to the workforce has come to typify and describe
post-modernity. Firat (1994) predicted that the post-modern society would be associated with the break
between gender and sex categories, arguing that “men as masculine” and “women as feminine” are no
longer seen as exclusive representations.
Nevertheless, several studies (Berk and Berk 1979; Robinson 1977; and Walker and Woods 1976) found
husbands’ behavior regarding household production, such as housework and taking care of the children,
to be the same regardless of the wife’s working status. In fact, Allen and Walker (2000) state that there is
no better predictor of the division of household labor than gender. However, this does not mean that the
nurturant abilities of fathers are not important. As demonstrated in the following section, nurturant
fathering has been found to be essential in children’s lives.
The Father’s Role in the Family
Kinship has been a topic in consumer behavior for some time with studies on role-model behavior (Clark
et al. 2001), family group decision making (Corfman et al. 1987), and relationship theory in relation to
branding (Fournier 1998). It has long been a focus in the psychology and sociology literature, but just
recently has the father-child dyad been specifically studied. In the February 2004 issue of Educational &
Psychological Measurement, Finley and Schwartz produced two scales on fathering abilities, the
Nurturant Fathering Scale and the Father Involvement Scale. These scales on father participation have
become increasingly important considering the family form of American society has become so jumbled.
38
Family forms that Finley and Schwartz (2004) mention are composed of the traditional married parents,
single divorced parents, father deceased families, stepfather headed families, and adopted parent families.
Also added to this list can be same-sex parents (as seen in the case of Rosie O’Donnell), mother deceased
families, both parents deceased-grandparent families or grandfather headed families, as well as possible
grandmother headed families. Today’s American culture experiences a divorce rate that hovers around
50% and parent separation rates which hover around an additional 17% (Finley and Schwartz 2004).
Therefore, any and all of these family types are prevalent in today’s culture. These statistics demonstrate
that the father-child relationship is constantly changing in regards to time. However, the authors propose
that the key to this dyad is not the amount of time that father and child spend together, but the quality of
the time that will have a profound impact on the nature of the relationship. At this stage, it is critical to
point out that the nature of the relationship is the vital component in the development of the consumption
processes by the child.
Finley and Schwartz (2004) make a few key assumptions about the father-child relationship. First, father
involvement within a child’s life is a highly complex construct that is spread out over many facets and
time periods within a child’s life, in which the father may or may not be involved. Second, the time spent
revolves around quality, not quantity. Thirdly, long-term fatherly impact is a function of perception and
how the child perceives the involvement level of this father. Finally and most importantly, this perception
of quality involvement can only be determined retrospectively. As a result, the father involvement scale
was designed to allow the participants to rate their father involvement in terms of both perceived actual
involvement versus the desired level of involvement that participants wished their father had in their
childhood. Upon testing the scale, Finley and Schwartz found that when the level of actual father
involvement was low, the participant most always desired for more father involvement. On the other
hand, when participants rated their actual father involvement as high, most always the participants rated
the desired as just right. The primary conclusion from the father involvement scale demonstrated that
father involvement was important to almost every one of the 2300 respondents that participated in the
study.
A second study from the area of social psychology that has been published recently is on Human
Affection Exchange by Floyd and Morman (2003). Now that it has been established by Finley and
Schwartz (2004) that father involvement is important to individuals, Floyd and Morman (2003) allow us
to take a closer look into the importance of the exchange of affection that occurs specifically between a
father and son. Numerous studies have documented the effects that the father-son relationship has on the
son’s emotional health and relative success (Berry 1990), the son’s attitudes toward sexuality (Fisher
1986), and even the relationships that the son will have with his own sons (Floyd and Morman 2000;
Simons and Beaman 1992). However, many of these studies have focused on conflict within these
relationships and not the affection and loving aspect of the father.
Floyd and Morman (2003) point out that there are three primary findings about the affection exchange
between fathers and sons. First and foremost, both father and son value affection from one another,
which is a fundamental understanding of kinship. It is important to both parties to communicate this
affection. Secondly, father and son seem to communicate this more often today than in years past. This
finding coincides with women emerging back into the work force and men having to take up extra
responsibilities within the home. This allows more time and more opportunity for affectionate
communication. But, communication of this affection must be performed in an appropriately “manly”
way. Therefore, Floyd and Morman’s (2003) third finding relates to this stereotypical manner. They
found that father and son tend to communicate their affection by doing favors for one another, which
allows them to say “I love you” without having to actually say it. As sad or humorous as this situation
may seem, it is within this scope of their interpersonal communication where consumer behavior becomes
so intriguing. It is within this primal gathering of manual labor in which men form deep relational bonds
not only with each other but with the tools that help them support each other.
39
Both of these studies, Finley and Schwartz (2004) and Floyd and Mormon (2003), demonstrate the
significance of a father’s role in a child’s developmental years. Finley and Schwartz offer two new scales
to the marketing literature that will help researchers of family structure to measure the impact of the
fathering role on latent constructs of interests. Floyd and Morman help us understand how affection is
translated between father and son. Although the traditional role of the father was prescribed to be
primarily instrumental, these studies help illustrate that father-child relations since the mid 1970s have
emphasized an increased nurturing role for fathers within a traditional household setting. The review of
gender roles in the household as well as the inspection on the specific role of the father within the
traditional household leads to the discussion that these role effects have on the materialistic attitude of the
child.
Family Structure and Materialism
Richins and Dawson (1992) define materialism as a “set of centrally held beliefs about the importance of
possessions in one’s life.” In other words, these centrally held beliefs are manifestations of a person’s
primary source of individual satisfaction or dissatisfaction with his or her own life (Rindfleisch et al.
1997). Individuals who are more materialistic place a greater emphasis on possessions than on
interpersonal relationships (Belk 1985; Richins 1994). Many researchers feel that materialism is part of
the “dark side” of consumer behavior (Hirschman 1991) because materialists exhibit reduced life
satisfaction (Richins and Dawson 1992), diminished levels of happiness (Belk 1985), and higher levels of
depression (Kasser and Ryan 1993). However, there are social psychologists, like Csikszentmihalyi and
Rochberg-Halton (1981), who contend that some forms of materialism serve a functional role, providing
consumers the ability to cope with stress, create value experiences, and enhance interpersonal relations.
In fact, McAlexander et al. (1993) found that divorced adults use material objects and acts of
consumption as means to cope with the stress produced from the altered family structure. Hence,
materialism may not always be bad in a temporary stasis, but if not monitored, long-term compulsive
consumption practices can be harmful and have adverse consequences (Hirschman 1992).
Materialism has emerged as a topic of considerable interest across a number of disciplines, yet the
origination of this phenomenon has yet to be determined. Rindfleisch et al. (1997) hypothesized that
materialism spawned from a disrupt in family structure, specifically whether the family had experienced a
divorce. The authors drew from McAlexander et al. (1993) when they hypothesize that if divorced adults
appeared to use material objects as a coping mechanism to divorce, then it would seem reasonable for
children to react in a similar manner, perhaps to even a greater extent. In addition, they noted that if
divorced parents competed for their child’s affection through gift giving, then the child could equate
material goods with love and self esteem, socializing them early to a materialistic behavior.
By using a t-test of mean differences between intact versus disrupted families, Rindfleisch et al. (1997)
found that young adults whose parents were divorced or separated demonstrated higher levels of
materialism than those whose parents were not divorced. This suggests that young adults from disrupted
families may use material objects as surrogates for absent parents. However, this does not mean that
there are no materialistic adults who came from intact families. To probe deeper, the authors through
previous conducted research (Amato 1993; McLanahan and Booth 1989; Cherlin 1992) theorized that
divorce caused a decrease in financial resources and increase in family stress, which would serve as
mediators between family structure and materialistic values. Although, these effects were found nonsignificant as mediators, Rindfleisch et al. (1997) did test other possible mediators and found a partial
mediation of interpersonal resources such as love and affection, rather than financial resources, that link
family disruption and materialism, which is consistent with Floyd and Morman (2003) as previously
discussed.
40
Buried within the results of Rindfleisch et al. (1997) was, in their opinion, a small secondary finding
about a partial mediation effect between family structure and materialism. However, this could be a key
finding that results in the missing link needed to explain the development of materialistic values.
Specifically, this author would propose that it is the lack of nurturing from a father that is the catalyst to a
materialistic lifestyle. Likewise, Rook (1987) found that children’s capacity to delay gratification (or
control consumption) is positively correlated with the presence of a father in the home.
Discussion
It has been demonstrated that the gender roles established by Parsons have become antiquated with
changing culture. No longer does the “inside/outside dichotomy” referred to by Gentry and his colleagues
(2003) hold for today’s society. Before the re-emergence of women back into the workforce, there were
strong gender roles constructed by social norms. In this instance, husbands served an instrumental role to
provide for the long-term well being of the family, while wives provided for the family’s short term
needs. Historically, the wives served the nurturing role for the children taking care of most of the
household duties. Fathers had little involvement in the nurturing role.
However, after the 1970s, women took more of the functional breadwinner role from the husbands as dual
income families became socially acceptable. The classification of gender roles became blurry inevitably
adding conflict to many marriages, which consequently increased the divorce rate among traditional
families. As Figure 1 displays, weaker gender roles allowed for traditional families to become
fragmented into different segmentations. Therefore, children of divorced families were forced to choose
to live with either mom or dad, creating even more conflict to the already troubled situation. It was
within this arena that Rindfleish et al. (1997) has already demonstrated higher levels of materialism in
children with divorced parents. However, the research did not specifically look at from which parental
role the materialism may have been spawned. This paper seeks to take their research another step forward
by specifically theorizing the role of the father in all of these contexts.
Consequently, as women took more of a functional role, men began to take on more responsibility around
the house. Because they increased their household duties, husbands began to make a more concerted
effort in the lives of their children. As research has already shown, fathers in the 1990s did spend
significantly more time with their children than their fathers did with them when they were children
(Gardyn 2000). Hopefully, this fatherly movement will have a promising effect on their children leading
to a generation that is not bound my materialistic attitudes, but find satisfaction in life by the relationships
they build. Hence, it is this author’s belief that the level of nurture that a father exemplifies in all of these
classifications will predict materialistic values in children. If a father has been highly involved in a
child’s life and the child rates his or her father high on the nurturant fathering scale, then the child’s
materialistic values should be low. However, if a father has neglected to successfully nurture his child for
whatever reason, then the child is more likely to develop a materialistic attitude, one where the child
replaces interpersonal relationships with material possessions.
Conclusion
One of the most important issues within this literature is the fact that there has yet to be a sound
theoretical model that helps marketers understand the impact that fathers have on the consumptive
processes of their children, especially experiences that could lead their children towards a materialistic
lifestyle. A basic set of questions that every child is asked in life is “what do you want to be when you
grow up” and “what does your father do?” These are impacting questions that we begin to debate within
our own selves from the time we become socialized in kindergarten. As marketers, we interpret those
questions as “how do you want to live life” and “how much discretionary income will you have to spend
when you grow up?” So many children’s answers to these questions exemplify a statement like “I want to
be a fireman like my dad” or “I want to be businessman like my father.” But what are the answers to
these questions after a child has experienced a dysfunctional home life involving divorce or an
41
uninvolved father who pays little attention to his family. Will the answers to these questions change? I
believe this is a complex question that psychologists and marketers alike do not fully comprehend.
However, we do have newly designed tools in place that can hopefully help researchers begin to answer
these questions.
REFERENCES
Aaker, Jennifer L. (1997), “Dimensions of Brand Personality,” Journal of Marketing Research, 34
(August), 347-356.
Allen, Katherine R. and Alexis J. Walker (2000), “Constructing Gender in Families.” In Robert M.
Milardo and Steve Duck (Eds.), Families as Relationships, New York: John Wiley &
Sons, 1-17.
Amato, Paul R. (1993), “Children’s Adjustment to Divorce: Theories, Hypotheses, and Empirical
Support,” Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55 (February), 23-38.
Belk, Russell W. (1985), “Materialism: Trait Aspects of Living in the Material World,” Journal of
Consumer Research, 12 (December), 265-280.
Berk, Richard A. and Sara Fenstermaker Berk (1979), Labor and Leisure at Home. Beverly Hills, CA:
Sage.
Berry, Patricia (1990), Fathers and Mothers, Dallas, TX: Spring Publications.
Cherlin, Andrew (1992), Marriage, Divorce, Remarriage, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Clark, Paul W., Craig A. Martin, and Alan J. Bush (2001), “The Effect of Role Model Influence on
Adolescents’ Materialism and Marketplace Knowledge,” Journal of Marketing Theory &
Practice, 9 (Fall), 27-36.
Corfman, Kim P. and Donald Lehman (1987), “Models of Cooperative Group Decision-Making and
Relative Influence: An Experimental Investigation of Family Purchase Decisions,” Journal of
Consumer Research, 14 (June), 1-12.
Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly and Eugene Rochberg-Halton (1981), The Meaning of Things: Domestic
Symbols and the Self, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ferber, Marianne and Bonnie Birnbaum (1980), “One Job or Two Jobs?” Journal of Consumer Research,
7 (December), 263-271.
Finley, Gordon E. and Seth J. Schwartz (2004), “The Father Involvement and Nurturant Fathering Scales:
Restropective Measures for Adolescent and Adult Children,” Educational & Psychological
Measurement, 64 (February), 143-164.
Firat, A. Fuat (1994), “Gender and Consumption: Transcending the Feminine?” In Janeen Arnold Cost
(Ed.), Gender Issues and Consumer Behavior, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, 84-103.
Fisher, Terri D. (1986), “An exploratory study of communication about sex and the sexual attitudes of
early, middle and late adolescents and their parents,” Journal of Genetic Psychology, 147, 543557.
42
Floyd, Kory and Mark T. Morman (2000), “Affection Received from Fathers as a Predictor of Men’s
Affection with Their Own Sons: Tests of the Modeling and Compensation Hypotheses,”
Communication Monographs, 67, 347-361.
Floyd, Kory and Mark T. Morman (2003), “Human Affection Exchange: II. Affectionate Communication
in Father-Son Relationships,” Journal of Social Psychology, 143 (October), 599-612.
Fournier, Susan (1998), “Consumers and Their Brands: Developing Relationship Theory in Consumer
Research,” Journal of Consumer Research, 24 (March), 343-373.
Gardyn, Rebecca (2000), “Make Room for Daddy: Fathers Become a Potentially Lucrative
Demographic,” American Demographics, June, 34-36.
Gentry, James W., Suraj Commuri, and Sunkyu Jun (2003), “Review of Literature on Gender in the
Family,” Academy of Marketing Science Review, 1, 1-20. Available at
http://www.amsreview.org/articles/gentry01-2003.pdf.
Hirschman, Elizabeth C. (1991), “Secular Morality and the Dark Side of Consumer Behavior: Or How
Semiotics Saved My Life,” in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 18, ed. Michael Solomon
and Rebecca Holman, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 1-4.
Hirschman, Elizabeth C. (1992), “The Consciousness of Addiction: Toward a General Theory of
Compulsive Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (September), 155-179.
Kasser, Tim and Richard M. Ryan (1993), “A Dark Side of the American Dream: Correlates of Financial
Success as a Central Life Aspiration,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65
(August), 410-422.
Kasser, Tim and Richard M. Ryan (1996), “Further Examining the American Dream: Differential
Correlates of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Goals,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22
(March), 280-287.
Kourilsky, Marilyn and Trudy Murray (1981), “The Use of Economic Reasoning to Increase Satisfaction
with Family Decision Making,” Journal of Consumer Research, 8 (September), 183-188.
McAlexander, James H., John W. Schouten, and Scott D. Roberts (1993), “Consumer Behavior and
Divorce,” in Research in Consumer Behavior, Vol. 6, ed. Janeen A. Costa and Russell W. Belk,
Greenwich, CT: JAI, 153-184.
McLanahan, Sara S. and Karen Booth (1989), “Mother-Only Families: Problems, Prospects and Politics,”
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51 (August), 557-580.
Parsons, Talcott (1949), “The Social Structure of the Family.” In R. Anshen (Ed.), The Family: Its
Function and Destiny, New York: Harper, 173-201.
Richins, Marsha L. and Scott Dawson (1992), “A Consumer Values Orientation for Materialism and Its
Measurement: Scale Development and Validation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 19
(December), 303-316.
Richins, Marsha L. (1994), “Special Possessions and the Expression of Material Values,” Journal of
Consumer Research, 21 (December), 522-533.
43
Rindfleisch, Aric, James E. Burroughs, and Frank Denton (1997), “Family Structure, Materialism, and
Compulsive Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (March), 312-325.
Rindfleisch, Aric and James E. Burroughs (2002), “Materialism and Well-Being: A Conflicting Values
Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Research, 29 (December), 348-370.
Risman, Barbara J. (1998), Gender Vertigo: American Families in Transition, New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Robinson, John P. (1977), How Americans Use Time: A Social-Psychological Analysis. New York:
Praeger.
Rook, Dennis (1987), “The Buying Impulse,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (September), 189-199.
Simons, Ronald L. and Jay Beaman (1992), “Gender Differences in the Intergenerational Transmission of
Parenting Beliefs,” Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54 (November), 823-836.
Sussman, Marvin B (1993), “Families in Time to Come: Taking a Position on Trends and Issues.” In
Barbara H. Settles, Roma S. Hanks, and Marvin B. Sussman (Eds.), American Families and the
Future: Analyses of Possible Destinies, New York: The Haworth Press, 303-313.
Walker, Katherine E. and Margaret E. Woods (1976), Time Use: A Measure of Household Production of
Family Goods and Services. Washington D.C.: Center for the Family, American Home
Economics Association.
Wilkes, Robert E. (1995), “Redefining Family in America: Characteristics and Expenditures of SingleParent Families,” in Marketing Theory and Applications, Vol. 6, ed. David Stewart and Naufel
Vilcassin, Chicago: American Marketing Association, 270-276.
Figure 1
The Effect of Father Nurturing on Materialistic Attitudes Across Gender Roles
Before 1970s
Strong Gender
Roles
Traditional Married
Family
Nurturing
Level of
Father:
44
High
Non-existent in
most cases
Low
High Materialistic
Attitudes
After 1970s
Weaker Gender Roles
Traditional
Family PeerMarriages
Low
Materialistic
Attitudes
High
Materialistic
Attitudes
Divorced
Husband
Divorced Wife
Low
Materialistic
Attitudes
High
Materialistic
Attitudes
Low
Materialistic
Attitudes
High
Materialistic
Attitudes
The Effect of Household Decision Behavior on Retailing
Structure of the Transitional Country
Vesna Brcic-Stipcevic, University of Zagreb
Sanda Renko, University of Zagreb
Abstract
Understanding customer needs, buying behavior and buying decisions is critical for effective retail
business. However, customers are constantly changing as are their habits (starting with demographic
trends in population, age and household structures, etc.). The last 10-20 years has seen significant
changes in food retailing (the introduction of new store formats with the growth of large retail formats,
the large number of new products and services, new technological innovations, etc.) which were driven
mainly by changing consumer characteristics This paper, using the results of a study, conducted on a
representative nation wide sample of the Croatian population, examines the role of marital status in the
process of store choice as well as the effect of consumer behavior on the changes in the retailing
structure.
Introduction
Understanding customer needs, buying behavior and buying decisions is critical for effective retail
business. However, customers are constantly changing as are their habits (starting with demographic
trends in population, age and household structures, etc.). The last 10-20 years has seen significant changes
in food retailing (the introduction of new store formats with the growth of large retail formats, the large
number of new products and services, new technological innovations, etc.) which were driven mainly by
changing consumer characteristics (Keh & Park, 1997).
Successful retailers have realized that they have to adjust their retailing mix to consumers` needs and
preferences. They should continually observe the whole environment to be sure that their business actions
anticipate changing consumer characteristics and stay ahead their competitors. The through understanding
of customers provides some valuable insight into the processes of purchasing products and/or services and
of store choice. Flemmer (1996, 39) suggests that there have been many studies, carried out in different
sectors, and different store types, showing that demographic and socio-economic factors are good
predictors of store choice.
Many marketing researchers take the individual consumer as the unit of analysis. They try to understand
and to describe how individuals make purchase decisions so that marketing strategies can be developed to
more effectively influence this process (Peter & Olson, 2002). In the retail context, they investigate what
draws consumers to one store rather than another. However, retail consumers may not be analyzed well
without taking into consideration such factors as demographic factors, socio-economic categories, social
influence, and family influence (Gilbert, 2003) as well as the strategies and characteristics of the retailer.
The main focus of this paper is on the household - the basic consumption unit. The paper begins with a
discussion of the household and the influence of family members in the process of purchasing products
and/or services and of store choice. As the majority of research into these areas has centered on the
45
families from economically advanced countries, focusing on the market of a transition country this paper
gives us the insight into different cultural aspect of family (i.e. husband-wife) purchase decision making.
This paper has been influenced by the work of Robert A. Mittelstaedt and John D. Mittelstaedt (2004) and
their examination of possible interactions and relationship between Macromarketing and Consumer
Behavior. In their work they suggested that consumer initiated behaviors could and did affect marketing
decision makers. This paper is an attempt to contribute to understanding the relationship between those
fields. As it investigates the effects of one of the socio-economic factors on the store choice, it finds out
the explanation for some recent changes in the retailing structure in the Republic of Croatia.
Based on the prior discussion, the following main objectives of the study are forwarded:
- to investigate whether culturally defined gender roles go through changes in the case of the
Republic of Croatia as it happened in the case of economically advanced countries,
- to find out the possible changes in role structures and the purchase influence of the Croatian
family members,
- to examine the effect of the factors mentioned above on the Croatian retailing structure.
Background
The role of household and the marital role influence in the product purchase decision-making have been
studied by marketing researchers for a number of years. Actually, marketers are interested in both
households and family members. Although the distinction between a family and a household is important
when examining the data, this study will not discuss the difference between these terms, because it has
been focused on one type of household – family. Moreover, the investigation of the socioeconomic
structure of the sample suggests that Croatian population mostly (63,1 percent of respondents) live in the
traditional family-based household with Mom and Dad, one or two kids.
Some of the fundamental questions of various researches are who (within the family) determine whether
to purchase product or service, who actually makes the purchase and how family members influence and
how they interact between each other when making purchasing decision.
There is much decision-making research focused primary on products, for example, the work of Davis
and Rigaux (1974), Henthorne, LaTour and Hudson (1997), Lee and Beatty (2002), Park (1982), etc.
Also, some studies (Davis 1976; Levy and Kwai-Choi Lee 2004; Stafford 1996) have focused on the
influence of household and marital roles in the decision-making process for services.
There are researchers who suggest the necessity to study children roles and influence on the purchase
decisions and choices made by the family (Atkin, 1978; Belch, Belch & Ceresino, 1985; Foxman &
Tansuhaj, 1988; Foxman, Tansuhaj & Ekstrom, 1989; Lee & Beatty, 2002). This study takes into
consideration the efforts and the findings of those researchers, but follows Spiro`s suggestions (1983)
about children’s limited impact on day-to-day family decision making. This study has focused on the
relative roles of the husband and wife in the purchasing process.
Most researchers do not agree with the view that only one family member makes purchase decision
(Kesić, 1999). This the reflection of the notion that decision making by a group such as a household or
family differs in many important respects from the decision that are completely individual (Neal, Quester
& Hawkins, 1999) and the each family member is influencing one or more stages of retail buying process.
For example, a husband may decide to buy a car but other family members can influence in purchasing
the family car concerning color, brand, additional equipment, etc.
Accordingly, family members could play only one or more different roles in the buying process, as
follows (Kotler, 2003):
a) initiator – the person who first suggests the idea of buying the product or service,
b) influencer – the person whose view or advice influences the decision,
c) decider – the person who decides on any component of a buying decision,
46
d) buyer – the person who makes the actual purchase,
e) user – the person who consumes or uses the product or service.
This paper has focused on the roles of influencer, decider and buyer. Therefore, the fundamental
questions are “who actually acts physically to complete the purchase process” and “who influences and
has the authority to make the buying decision”.
Since the relative roles and influences of husband and wife has been a topic of this paper, the following
role-structure categories are analyzed (Engel, Blackwell, Miniard, 1995; Kesić, 1999; Neal, Quester, &
Hawkins, 1999; Stafford, 1996):
- autonomic decisions – the individualized decisions when equal number of decisions is made by
each spouse, but each decision is individually made by one spouse or the other,
- husband dominant decisions,
- wife dominant decisions,
- syncratic decisions – the joint-decision type when most decisions are made by both husband and
wife.
A landmark study of Davis and Rigaux (1974) suggests that these situations vary widely by product class.
Thus, autonomic decisions involve purchase of alcoholic beverages, camera, Hi-Fi, etc. Husbanddominant decisions generally occur with the purchase of life insurance, family car, etc. Wife-dominant
decisions occur in the purchase of clothing, food, cleaning products, etc. Joint decisions involve, for
example, purchase of house and TV and choosing holidays.
Specifically, this article examines the husband-wife involvement in the purchase of fast moving consumer
goods. As theory suggests that wife has traditionally acted as the family’s main purchasing agent
especially for food, we should take this into consideration in analyzing the results of the study.
Factors of change
In recent years, there have been many changes in culturally defined gender roles, and these changes affect
retailing structure as well as retail management decisions. Traditional husband and wife roles have been
substantially altered, mainly as the result of the changing character of working population. Namely, the
growing number of women working has caused the consequent lack of family shopping time. Gilbert
(2003) suggests that such an impact of more women working results in:
- women having more independence over purchase decisions, sometimes seeking identity from
purchases,
- shopping being skewed more to evening and extended opening hours or perhaps being
increasingly based upon direct purchasing,
- limited time leading to greater opportunity for pre-prepared and convenience foods or takeaway
outlets,
- service companies having to ensure that services are offered outside normal work hours or at
weekends,
- advertisers promoting more heavily to the employed female, as a segment, etc.
The changes occurring in the context of the changing character of the working population are dynamic
and lead to changes in men’s and women’s roles. For example, the growing number of working women
has resulted in changes in traditional household purchasing patterns. We should consider the standard
conceptions such as “women buy all the household goods”.
In their Review of Literature on Gender in the Family, Gentry, Commuri and Jun (2003) argue that much
of the research dealing with the impact of wife’s changing work status on family decision-making was
based on the premise that working wives would be more time crunched, and would seek “time-saving”
products and services in order to fulfill traditional gender roles.
47
Marshall and Anderson (2000) suggest that females still tend to take the major role in food shopping
within families, but shopping has increased in importance for males (Evans, Christiansen & Gill, 1996;
Torres, Summers & Belleau, 2001). Food retailers are making mistakes if they continue to think of
women as the number-one business opportunity.
Some 40 years ago, the typical role of American women was a homemaker. Nowadays, more than half of
all women are in the labor force. Statistics New Zealand (2000) suggests that nearly 60 per cent of women
in New Zealand are employed in the workforce.
Central Bureau of Statistics of the Republic of Croatia (2004) shows that 45 per cent of Croatian women
are in the labor force. Generally speaking, there is small decrease in the total number of paid employment
in Croatia and the increase of 2 per cent in the number of women in work (comparing the previous years).
Increasing numbers of women in work reflected the number of women-drivers. Additionally, it influences
the choice of store type because the limited time caused the decrease of small shopping trips in favor of
weekend shopping, where husbands and wives shop jointly for products traditionally thought to be under
the separate control of one spouse or the other.
Socio-economic changes fueled retail change and the location of retail business; such as increasing
numbers of women in work, the length of the working day and the escalating importance of shopping as a
leisure activity (Newman & Cullen, 2002). Also, the huge rise in car ownership has made much easier for
consumers to travel further in order to shop in large retail formats with wide range of merchandise. The
successful retailers have to make various adjustments to accommodate the needs of this changing market.
For example, the increased number of working women as well as longer work schedules has caused many
tremendous changes in operating hours regulations. Additionally, Sunday and evening opening, the
extension of store hours and even 24-hour shopping have all made retailing a more intense business
(Anonymous, 1996). Limited time for shopping lead to expansion of large store formats which facilitate
quicker shopping, offers technological innovations and large variety of products.
Recent research (Leinert Novosel, 2003) about the role of women in the Croatian society suggests that
Croatian women try to balance the responsibility of running a household with their careers. Statistics
show that Croatian women are more educated than men. However, we have still observed a more
traditional orientation of Croatian women because the family is the priority and after that the career. Thus,
the traditional household purchasing patterns considerably have still existed in the case of Croatian
households. However, large international retail chains, entering the Croatian market, have identified the
possible trends and acted in relation to them. The Report of the Croatian Chamber of Economy (2003)
compares today’s situation in the retail sector with those in some previous years, and found the evident
growth of large-store formats. For example, in 2002 there were 91 retail outlets with more than 1000 m2
selling space, and that number increased to 131 retail outlets in 2003 (a growth of 44 per cent).
METHODOLOGY
Questionnaire development
The research was based on the face-to-face in-home interviewing with a highly structured questionnaire.
In this case this approach has some advantages over other type of interviews because it encourages
interviewees to speak openly and frankly, enabling the interviewer to sense non-verbal feedback and
encouraging personal thought, (Churchill, 1991; Sykes, 1990; Turnbull & Moustakatos, 1996). Zeithaml
(1985) suggests that this technique should be used in studies of purchasing behavior.
The field work and data entry was conducted by PULS Ltd., an independent Croatian market research
agency. Statistical data analysis was done by the authors of the paper using SPSS 12.0.
48
The research instrument consisted of two parts. Part I required respondents to indicate the importance of
store attributes when shopping for small quantities of fast moving consumer goods (we could consider it
as «everyday shopping»). Part II of the research instrument asked questions about the importance of store
attributes when shopping for larger quantities of fast moving consumer goods (we could consider it as
«monthly or weekly shopping»). Accordingly, the questions were created in keeping with the shopping
categories mentioned above.
The questionnaire was developed in such a way because the preliminary research (which was conducted
in September 2003 on the sample of 103 respondents in the city of Zagreb - the Metropolitan City of
Croatia) showed essential differences in the evaluation of store attributes in the two shopping categories
(shopping for small and large quantities of fast moving consumer goods) and the differences in the roles
played by each of the family members in relation to the purchase of small and large quantities of fast
moving consumer goods.
The type of questions used in the instrument included all aspects of the purchase of fast moving consumer
goods, such as the frequency of shopping, the average amount of money spent in the purchase, travel to
the store, etc. Specifically, two questions representing the role in the family purchase decision (i.e. the
“decision-maker” and the “purchaser”) were included in the both parts of the questionnaire. Question
responses included horizontal scale points coded as follows:
“1” – my own decision
“2” – my husband’s/wife’s decision
“3” – my father’s/mother’s decision
“4” – my children’s decision
“5” – joint decision
“6” – others
In order to reveal the interaction between the family decision-making roles and the retailing structure,
there was also the question of the store type where the family usually made its purchase. The respondents
were asked to specify what types of stores they used for the purchase of fast moving consumer goods
(considering both shopping categories: small and large shopping). The following seven types were
presented to the respondents who were then required to specify which pattern best fitted their shopping
habits:
“1” – only in neighborhood small convenience stores
“2” – only in supermarkets
“3” – only in hypermarkets
“4” – only in discount stores
“5” – only in cash & carry wholesalers, such as Metro (This type of store had to be included in the
questionnaire because of high percentage of Croatian customers who acquired a habit to make their
purchases there.)
“6” – only in public market outlets
“7” - others
Respondents and data collection
The data collected and reported in this study are the part of a larger comprehensive study of retail store
patronage being conducted in the Republic of Croatia on the nationwide representative sample of the
adult population (18+years) in Croatia (total sample = 931 households including 931 respondents) in
January 2004.
A random stratified sample was used, with the regions and the size of residence as stratification criteria.
Towns/cities, as well as the households addresses were selected according to a random procedure
(random starting points) for each residence. The sample was large enough to ensure a very high level of
49
statistic reliability. With n=931, considering normal approximation, the maximum margin of error for an
estimate of a proportion with 95 percent confidence interval is + 3.2 percent.
The demographic characteristics of surveyed sample are outlined in Table 1.
Table 1. Demographics of sample (Total sample=931households including 931 respondents)
Item
Percentage
Region
Zagreb and surroundings
North Croatia
Slavonia
Lika, Kordun, Banovina
Istria, North Coast, Gorski Kotar
Dalmatia
23.2
18.4
18.2
7.8
12.6
19.8
Size of residence
Up to 2000 inhabitants
From 2001 to 10000
From 10001 to 100000
Over 100000
39.4
15.3
21.2
24.1
Gender
Female
Male
54.8
45.2
Age (Years)
18-24
25-34
35-44
45-54
55-64
65+
14.0
18.6
18.2
18.4
14.2
16.8
Education
No school
Unfinished Elementary
Elementary
3 years-High school
Secondary/High
College/University
2.5
7.2
17.1
19.8
37.5
16.0
Occupation/Employment status
Not employed
Pensioners
Housewives
Students
Managers
Clerk
Industrial workers
Farmers
Businessmen
Others
10.5
27.5
9.8
8.8
6.5
9.2
19.2
2.3
3.3
2.9
50
Analysis and results
Univariate analysis was used to investigate the frequency of the research questions. A chi-square test
(with the cross-tabulation) was used to ascertain the differences between consumers across all strata
(across the regions and the size of the residence) and to discover whether there was a significant
relationship (χ2 test) between the family purchase decision role in the process of purchasing fast moving
consumer goods and the type of store the household used to buy.
The analyses, as well as the findings, refer to Part I relating to “small everyday shopping” and Part II
relating to “big monthly/weekly shopping”: the results include both categories. Thus, the study
investigates the changes in the roles of family members in the context of small and large shopping.
As indicated in Graph 1, Croatian population mostly daily used to purchase small quantities of fast
moving consumer goods (71 percent of respondents).
(%)
Graph 1. The frequency of small shopping
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
70,8
12,8
3
4,1
Don`t buy
small
quantities
Once a
week
9,3
2-3 times a 4-5 times a
week
week
Daily
The frequency
Graph 2 shows that mostly women (49,7 percent) actually physically make purchase process. Graph 3
indicates that the highest percentage of respondents (53,8 percent) that makes decisions about the
purchase of small quantities of fast moving consumer goods refers to women. Moreover, Graph 3 shows
high percentage of joint purchases (19,6 percent) with men playing significantly fewer roles in decisionmaking process than wives. They used to buy small quantities of fast moving consumer goods but wives
dominated decisions about the purchase.
The chi-square value for household roles in both situations (influencer/decider and buyer) was significant
(all at p<0,05), suggesting a significant relationship between the roles in the buying process of small
quantities and gender.
51
Graph 2. Who actually physically makes small purchases?
60
50
49,7
(%)
40
30
24,1
20
9
10,4
10
2,9
3,9
0
wife
husband
parent
child
joint
others
The member of the household
Graph 3. Who influences and has the authority to make the buying decision (small shopping)
60
53,8
50
(%)
40
30
19,6
20
13,6
10
10,9
1
1,1
0
wife
husband
parent
child
joint
others
The member of the household
The cross-tabulation was used to find out the differences between consumers across all strata (across the
regions and the size of the residence). It found a significant relationship (χ2 test) between the role of
households in the purchase process and living in a specific region and the size of the residence (but only
in the case of the regions of Slavonia, Dalmatia and Istria as well as in small and large towns). Dalmatia
was unusual as almost 2/3 of respondents noted the role of the wife as the buyer and decider/influencer.
An examination of store types where the family usually makes its purchase (Graph 4) suggests that
Croatian consumers mostly (69 percent of respondents) buy small quantities of fast moving consumer
goods in small stores, located near their residence.
The chi-square test suggests significant relationship (p=0,000) between the members of the household
who have the authority to make the buying decision and types of stores the household used to buy small
quantities (Table 2). Analyzing the categories of shopper (household members) and types of stores, Table
2 shows very interesting result: high percentage of joint purchases in small convenient stores (14,2
percent).
52
Graph 4. Types of outlets for small shopping
cash&carry
0%
hypermarkets
2%
others
1%
discount stores
2%
public market
outlets
1%
supermarkets
25%
small convenience
stores
69%
Table 2. Cross-tabulation decider/type of the store (small shopping)
Types of stores (%)
Roles in the
household
Small
convenience
store
Supermarket
Hypermarket
Discount
store
Cash&carry
Public
market
outlets
Others
Total
Wife
Husband
Parent
Child
Joint
Others
37,4
9,3
7,1
0,6
14,2
0,4
13,4
3,5
3,5
0,1
4,3
0,2
0,9
0,3
0,2
0
0,2
0,1
1,2
0,3
0
0
0,4
0,1
0,2
0
0
0,1
0
0
0,6
0,1
0,2
0
0,2
0
0,1
0,1
0,1
0,1
0,3
0,2
53,8
13,6
10,9
1,1
19,6
1,0
Total
69
25
1,7
2,0
0,3
1,1
0,9
100
However, the chi-square test showed that there wasn’t significant relationship (p>0,05) between the
members of the household who actually complete the purchase process and types of stores the household
used to buy small quantities (Table 3).
53
Table 3. Cross-tabulation buyer of the store (small shopping)
Types of stores (%)
Roles in the
household
Small
convenience
store
Supermarket
Hypermarket
Discount
store
Cash&carry
Public
market
outlets
Others
Total
Wife
Husband
Parent
Child
Joint
Others
34,4
16,3
6,6
3,3
6,4
2,0
12,7
6,4
3,3
0,5
1,8
0,3
0,8
0,3
0,1
0
0,3
0,2
0,7
0,7
0,2
0,1
0,2
0,1
0,1
0
0
0
0,2
0
0,6
0,2
0,2
0
0
0,1
0,4
0,2
0
0
0,1
0,2
49,7
24,1
10,4
3,9
9,0
2,9
Total
69
25
1,7
2,0
0,3
1,1
0,9
100
Analyzing the purchase of large quantities of fast moving consumer goods, Graph 5 shows that Croatian
population mostly once a month used to purchase large quantities of fast moving consumer goods (37,4
percent of respondents).
(%)
Graph 5. The frequency of large shopping
40
37,4
30
23,1
18,7
20
12,8
8
10
0
Don`t buy
large
quant it ies
Once in t wo
mont hs
mont hly
every 14-15
days
more oft en
t han 15 days
The frequency
Graph 6 shows that husbands and wives jointly physically make purchase process (34,4 percent), but
there is also very high percentage in the case of women purchases (31,4). Graph 7 indicates the different
situation: the highest percentage of respondents (44,1 percent) that makes decisions about the purchase of
large quantities of fast moving consumer goods refers to women.
54
Graph 6. Who actually physically makes large purchases?
40
(%)
30
34,4
31,4
20
17,2
10,3
10
5
1,7
0
wife
husband
parent
child
joint
others
The member of the household
Graph 7. Who influences and has the authority to make the buying decision (large shopping)
50
44,1
40
31,5
(%)
30
20
11
10
10,7
0,6
2,1
0
wife
husband
parent
child
joint
others
The member of the household
Investigating the differences between consumers across all strata (across the regions and the size of the
residence) discovered the same results as in the case of small shopping, because the region Dalmatia and
small towns showed “the dominance of wives” in both categories. The highest percentage of joint
purchases is presented in the case of respondents from region Zagreb.
The chi-square value for the household roles in both situations (influencer/decider and buyer) was
significant (all at p<0,05), suggesting the significant relationship between the roles in the buying process
of large quantities and the gender.
An examination of store types where the family usually makes it purchase (Graph 8) suggests that
Croatian consumers mostly (59 percent of respondents) buy large quantities of fast moving consumer
goods in supermarkets.
55
In both situations (influencer/decider and buyer), the chi-square test showed that there wasn’t significant
relationship (p>0,05) between the members of the household and types of stores the household used to
buy large quantities (Table 4 and Table 5).
Graph 8. Types of outlets for large shopping
others
1%
cash&carry
3%
small convenience
stores
p ublic market outlets
2%
0%
discount stores
22%
sup ermarkets
59%
hy p ermarkets
13%
Table 4. Cross-tabulation decider/type of the store (large shopping)
Types of stores (%)
Roles in the
household
Small
convenience
store
Supermarket
Hypermarket
Discount
store
Cash&carry
Public
market
outlets
Others
Total
Wife
Husband
Parent
Child
Joint
Others
1,3
0,3
0,4
0
0,4
0
25,1
5,9
6,8
1,7
18,6
0,4
4,9
2,3
0,8
0,1
4,4
0,1
10,9
2,2
2,2
0,3
6,7
0,1
1,4
0,3
0,2
0
1,0
0
0,1
0
0
0
0,3
0
0,4
0
0,3
0
0,1
0
44,1
11,0
10,7
2,1
31,5
0,6
Total
2,4
58,5
12,6
22,4
2,9
0,4
0,8
100
Table 5. Cross-tabulation buyer of the store (large shopping)
Types of stores (%)
Roles in the
household
Small
convenience
store
Supermarket
Hypermarket
Discount
store
Cash&carry
Public
market
outlets
Others
Total
Wife
Husband
Parent
Child
Joint
Others
0,8
0,6
0,3
0,3
0,4
0
19,0
10,7
6,5
2,7
18,6
1,0
3,0
2,4
0,8
0,3
5,8
0,3
7,0
3,0
2,2
1,4
8,4
0,4
1,0
0,5
0,4
0
1,0
0
0,2
0
0
0,1
0,1
0
0,4
0
0,1
0,2
0,1
0
31,4
17,2
10,3
5,0
34,4
1,7
Total
2,4
58,5
12,6
22,4
2,9
0,4
0,8
100
56
CONCLUSION
Many marketing researchers take the individual consumer as the analysis unit, but this research has been
focused on the household as the basic consumption unit. Results in the Republic of Croatia indicate that
gender roles changed as in economically developed countries but at a slower pace. We have still observed
a more traditional orientation of Croatian women because the family is the priority and after that the
career. Thus, the traditional household purchasing patterns considerably have still play a considerable role
in the case of Croatian households. However, large international retail chains, entering the Croatian
market, have identified the possible trends in Croatian economy and acted in relation to them (expanded
opening store hours are one of competitive services). The results of the study conducted on the
nationwide representative sample of the population in Croatia show the dominance of women in “small
everyday shopping” with some relevant regional differences. The analysis of the purchase of large
quantities of fast moving consumer indicates the importance of wives and jointly shopping.
We agree with Commuri and Gentry (2000) that family has been witness to changes in economy over the
years, but the paper did not find the evidence of the changing household purchasing patterns. Also, the
retail sector in Croatia has undergone a structural shift in recent years (the evident growth of large-store
formats) but it was connected with the establishment of the new free market system
REFERENCES
Anonymous, 1996. A new look at one-stop shopping: A TIMES model approach to matching store hours
and shopper schedules, International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, vol. 24, no.
10, Winter, pp. 9-11.
Atkin, C.K. 1978. Observation of parent-child interaction in supermarket decision-making, Journal of
Marketing, vol. 42, October, pp. 41-45.
Belch, G.E., Belch, M.A. & Ceresino, G. 1985. Parental and Teenage Child Influences in Family
Decision Making, Journal of Business Research, vol. 13, pp. 163-176.
Central Bureau of Statistics of the Republic of Croatia, 2004, www.dzs.hr
Churchill, G:A. 1991. Marketing research: methodological foundations, The Dryden Press, SAD.
Croatian Chamber of Economy, 2003. Reports-Trade
Commuri, S. & Gentry, J.W. 2000. Opportunities for Family Research in Marketing, Academy of
Marketing ScienceReview [Online], http://www.amsreview.org/articles/commuri08-2000.pdf
Davis, H.L. 1976. Decision making within the household, Journal of Consumer Research, no. 2, pp. 241260.
Davis, H.L. & Rigaux, B.P. 1974. Perception of marital roles in decision processes, Journal of Consumer
Research, no. 1, pp. 51-62.
Engel, J.F., Blackwell, R.D. & Miniard, P.W. 1987., 1995. Consumer Behavior, The Dryden Press, SAD.
Evans, K.R., Christiansen, T. & Gill, J.D. 1996. The impact of social influence and role expectations on
shopping center patronage intentions, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, vol. 24, no.
3, pp. 208-218.
Flemmer, M. 1996. Store choice models and consumption in Denmark and Spain: Towards a consumer
convergence, Dissertation, Manchester Metropolitan University.
57
Foxman, E., Tansuhaj, P. 1988. Adolescents` and mothers` perceptions of relative influence in family
purchase decisions: patterns of agreement and disagreement, Houston, M.J. Advances in
Consumer Research, 15, Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 449-453.
Foxman, E., Tansuhaj, P. & Elstrom, K. 1989. Family members` perception of adolescents` influence in
family decision making, Journal of Consumer Research, vol. 15, March, pp. 482-491.
Gentry, J.W., Commuri, S. & Sunkyu, J. 2003. Review of Literature on Gender in the Family, Academy of
Marketing ScienceReview [Online],
http://www.vancouver.wsu.edu/amsrev/theory/gentry01- 2003.html
Gilbert, D. 2003. Retail Marketing Management, 2 ed, Pearson Education Limited, UK.
Henthorne, T.L., LaTour, M.S. & Hudson, T.W. 1997. Japanese couples` marital roles in stages of
product purchase decision making, International Marketing Review, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 39-58.
Keh, H.T. & Park, S.Y. 1997. To Market, to Market: the Changing Face of Grocery Retailing, Long
Range Planning, vol. 30, no. 6, pp. 836-846.
Kesić, T. 1999. Ponašanje potrošača (Consumer Behavior), ADECO, Zagreb.
Kotler, P. 2003. Marketing Management, 11 ed, Pearson Education, Inc., New Jersey.
Lee, C.K.C. & Beatty, S.E. 2002. Family structure and influence in family decision making, Journal of
Consumer Marketing, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 24-41.
Leinert Novosel, S. 2003. Policy of Women`s Employment, Politička misao, vol. XL, no. 3, pp. 103-127.
Levy, D.S. & Kwai-Choi Lee, C. 2004. The influence of family members on housing purchase decisions,
Journal of Property Investment & Finance, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 320-338.
Marshall, D.W. & Anderson, A.S. 2000. Who`s responsible for the food shopping? A study of young
Scottish couples in their “honeymoon period”, International Review of Retail, Distribution and
Consumer Research, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 59-72.
Mittelstaedt, R.A. & Mittelstaedt, J.D. 2004. Macromarketing Approaches to Consumer Behavior: A
Review and Research Agenda, The 29th Annual Macromarketing Seminar, Vancouver, British
Columbia, May 29-June 1, 2004, pp. 234-240.
Neal, C., Quester, P. & Hawkins, D. 1999. Consumer Behaviour: Implications for Marketing Strategy,
2ed, The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., Australia, 1999.
Newman, A.J. & Cullen, P. 2002. Retailing: environment & operations, Thomson Learning, London.
Peter, J.P. & Olson, J.C. 2002. Consumer Behavior and Marketing Strategy, 6ed, McGraw-Hill/Irwin,
NY.
Spiro, R.L. 1983. Persuasion in family decision making, Journal of Consumer Research, vol. 9, pp. 393402.
Stafford, M.R. 1996. Marital influence in the decision-making process for services, Journal of Services
Marketing, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 6-21.
Statistics New Zealand, 2000, New Zealand Time Series, www.stats.govt.nz
Sykes, W. 1990. Validity and reliability in qualitative market research: a review of the literature, Journal
of the Market Research Society, vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 289-328.
Torres, I.M., Summers, T.A. & Belleau, B.D. 2001. Men`s shopping satisfaction and store preferences,
Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, no. 8, pp. 205-212.
Turnbull, P.W. & Moustakatos, T. 1996. Marketing and investment banking II: practical and theoretical
challenges, International Journal of Bank Marketing, 14/2, pp. 38-49.
Zeithaml, V. 1985. The new demographics and market segmentation, Journal of Marketing, vol. 49, pp.
64-75.
58
Living on the Edge:
Financial Services for the Lower Socio-Economic Strata
Lisa Szykman, The College of William & Mary
Don R. Rahtz, The College of William and Mary
Michael Plater, Brown University
Greg Goodwin, Jackson-Hewitt, Inc.
Introduction
The purpose of this study is to investigate how an individual’s needs and attitudes towards different
financial services available to consumers in the lower socio-economic strata influence their choice of
financial services. Such services include traditional banks and credit unions, as well as those services
often described as predatory (services such as payday loans, rent-to-own, check cashing services, money
order services, refund anticipation loans, etc.) At the moment there is a paucity of research that has
examined this issue from any perspective. Agnew and Szykman (in press) suggest that people in the
lower socio-economic classes possess a much lower financial literacy and were more likely to select the
easiest (or default) option when given choices regarding retirement funds, even when those decisions
might well yield much lower economic returns.
In recent trends, banks have attempted to attract this group of consumers by locating branches in lower
socio-economic communities. However, many consumers in this market continue to resist doing business
with these institutions, choosing instead to pay higher fees to non-banking financial service providers to
cash checks, obtain money orders and obtain short-term loans, etc. By better understanding the rationale
behind these decisions, financial services providers (offering a wide portfolio of services) could
potentially initiate new services and features that would increase the financial options available, and
ultimately improve the financial well-being of the community’s members.
Method
The data for this study were collected in Chicago, Illinois from November 2004 to March 2005. The process
involved a two phase methodology. Phase one was a focus group and Phase two involved a survey
questionnaire. A focus group of nine individuals (five female, 4 males) was conducted in November of 2004
with members of the lower socio-economic strata. The results from this focus group were then used to
develop a survey instrument that was used to collect quantitative data regarding the topic. This instrument
was then used to collect data from 120 individuals from the lower socio-economic population between
February and March 2005. The latter data collection took place in a local tax preparation office of JacksonHewitt, Inc. Individuals who consented to be part of the study received a $10.00 deduction in their tax
preparation fee.
Phase One: Focus Group Results Overview
The lower socio-economic population seems to understand the fee structures associated with their
financial decisions. This suggests that ignorance is not a driving force behind their decisions and that it
may be that these consumers are actively and consciously avoiding banks. This avoidance may come
from a few different sources. First, it appears that there may be an underlying mistrust of traditional
59
financial institutions. Since the customers do not trust banks to do the right thing, they are reluctant to do
business with them. Secondly, there is preliminary evidence that this population may feel alienated by
traditional financial institutions. For example, a concern raised by members of this social group is that
they are “not wanted” by the traditional financial services providers, and expressed that they felt that they
were not treated as well as other more “acceptable” customers. This level of alienation may also be
contributing to their avoidance of banks. Additionally, while banks seem to be willing to offer traditional
savings and checking accounts to this target market, they are less willing to assist these customers when
they need short term financial assistance.
This lower socio-economic group’s simple access to loan services can often put them under significant
financial duress and force them to use very high cost alternatives in the marketplace (e.g. rent to own,
short term high interest loans, etc.). In addition, they may view banks’ fee structures that are based on
account balances as unfair. Finally, the focus group results also suggested that this group has higher
service expectations from traditional banking institutions than for the less traditional service providers.
The focus group stressed the differences as being a “relationship” for the banking industry vs. a
“transaction” from services such as the Currency Exchange (Currency Exchange was the common term
used by our focus group participants to identify a nontraditional provider of basic financial transactions,
such as check cashing, money orders, etc). With these higher expectations for a “relationship,” there was
a much more pronounced dissatisfaction with previous bank service experiences. It seems that the
“relationship/transaction” distinction between banks and currency exchanges, combined with any past
disappointments play a critical role in these customers’ decisions regarding which type of service provider
to use.
Phase Two: Survey Model and Design
A general model is shown in Appendix A. It lays out the hypotheses regarding the reasons why this
consumer group may choose a nontraditional provider of financial services over a bank.
In our preliminary survey, recently administered during the 2005 tax season, consumers’ attitudes and
opinions regarding the financial services industry and their needs in relation to those services are
assessed. The survey contains measures of the perceptions of banks and less traditional financial service
providers, current financial services used, the frequency of use of checking and savings accounts,
alienation by banks, perceived fairness, perceptions of their tax return, and banking service expectations.
In addition, the survey obtained actual 2004 income, family size, and use of the Refund Anticipation Loan
option(Refund Anticipation Loans are short term loans that allow filers to obtain their tax returns very
quickly. These loans are an example of the short term loans that have been criticized as being predatory.).
Discussion
Obviously, a person’s financial well being is critical to their overall quality of life. Therefore, if policy
makers and traditional bankers want to assist this “at risk” group, a better understanding of their financial
decision making process is a necessary piece of the puzzle. Based on the initial focus group study, the
efforts being made by many banks simply is not enough to meet this group’s financial service needs.
Locating branches in their neighborhoods is a start, but if the customer still does not trust banks or still
feels alienated, they will continue to make higher cost financial decisions. While public policy officials
and social advocates have repeatedly argued that this group is exploited by financial service providers
who offer a questionable portfolio of high cost products (such as Return Anticipation Loans and Rent-toown services), it appears that these financial services are meeting a current unmet need in this target
population. While many of these advocates propose to eliminate the less traditional financial service
providers, this step may actually do more harm than good.
60
REFERENCES
Agnew, Julie R. and Lisa R. Szykman (2005) “Asset Allocation and Information Overload: The
Influence of Information Display, Asset Choice and Investor Experience,” Journal of
Behavioral Finance (in press).
Aizcorbe, Ana, Arthur B. Kennickell, and Kevin B. Moore (2003), “Recent Changes in U.S.
Family Finances: Evidence from the 1998 and 2001 Survey of Consumer Finances,”
Federal Reverve Bulletin, January, 1-32.
Braunstein, Sandra and Carolyn Welch (2002), “Financial Literacy: An Overview of Practice,
Research, and Policy,” Federal Reserve Bulletin, November, 445-457.
Durand, Richard M. and Zarrel V. Lambert (1985), “Alienation and Criticism of Advertising,”
Journal of Advertising, 14(3), 9-17.
Hilgert, Marianne A. and Jeanne M. Hogarth (2003), “Household Financial Management: The
Connection between Knowledge and Behavior,” Federal Reserve Bulletin, July, 309-322.
Iglesias, Victor (2004), “Preconceptions About Service: How Much Do They Influence Quality
Evaluations?” Journal of Service Research, 7(1), 90-103.
McCausland, Richard (2004), “Many Happy Returns: Refund Anticipation Loans and Other
Bank Products Gaining in Popularity,” Accounting Technology, January/February, 22-26.
McKinnon, John D. (2004), “Cashing In on a Tax Credit for the Poor – Retailers Help Prepare
Earned-Income Claims, but Steer Refunds Toward Loans, Purchases,” Wall Street
Journal, New York: NY: May 29, 2003, p. 4.
61
Appendix A
General Model of Nontraditional Financial Providers Usage
Income
_
Mistrust of
Banks
+
Frequency of use of
Nontraditional
Financial Providers
+
Feelings of
Alienation
+
Service
Expectations/
Preconceptions
62
Do We Really Care?
A Macromarketing Perspective of Consumer Vulnerability
Special Session Organizers
Ron Hill, USF St. Petersburg
Debra Stephens, University of Portland
Consumer Vulnerability: What Do We REALLY Mean?
Stacey Baker, University of Wyoming
Jim Gentry, University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Terri Rittenburg, University of Wyoming
The Good, the Bad, and the Ugly of Media Portrayals of “Model Minorities”
Charles R. Taylor, Villanova University
Stacy Landreth, Villanova University
Hae-Kyong Bang, Villanova University
When Does a Dollar Buy Less that a Dollar’s Worth?
Racial Profiling and Marketplace Discrimination
Anne-Marie Harris, Salem State College
Geraldine Henderson, University of Texas at Austin
Jerome Williams, University of Texas at Austin
If We Can Feed Surfers, Why not the Poor?
The Moral Case for Universal Consumption Adequacy
Ron Hill, USF St. Petersburg
63
_________________________________________________
Australian Industry Responses to Current Ethical Dilemmas –
Fast Cars and Fast Food
Sandra C. Jones
University of Wollongong
_________________________________________________
Abstract
This paper reviews two ethical dilemmas currently facing advertisers in Australia, which have resulted
from lobby group activities and public concern over health and safety issues, and discusses industry
responses to these dilemmas. In Australia the main response mechanism for advertisers has been the
introduction of self-regulatory codes of practice. A review of the general and trade press, and of the
records of the Australian Advertising Standards Board, finds that industry responses tend to be based on
teleological ethics (focus on the ends) rather than deontological ethics (focus on the means), with the
primary teleological motive being to avoid deleterious outcomes for the industry rather than for society
as a whole.
Introduction
Advertisers in Australia, as in the U.S. and the U.K., are bound by an industry-designed self-regulatory
code of ethics which covers permissible advertising content. A 1993 Morgan poll reported that the
Australian public rated the advertising industry at 9%, where the maximum is 100%, on ethics and
honesty (Phillips 1993). More recently, Brinkmann expressed the view that “there is almost a
suspicion that marketers' references to values and ideals are a marketing trick, an oxymoron”
(Brinkmann 2002, p. 160).
Classifying ‘ethics’
Within this paper, we will draw the distinction between the deontological and teleological perspective of
the regulation of advertising. Deontologists believe that to be ethical we must use the right means
regardless of the outcome (Emmanuel Kant’s categorical imperative). Proponents of this view would
argue, for example, that advertisers should not use overt sex appeals in cigarette advertising as this
behavior is frowned upon by society – regardless of whether doing so leads to harm. Teleologists (or
utilitarianists), such as the pragmatist Jeremy Bentham, believe that it is the goodness or badness of the
outcome that counts. Proponents of this view would argue, for example, that advertisers should not use
characters in cigarette ads who have high appeal to young people as these characters may encourage kids
to smoke, which is a bad outcome.
As we will see in the codes, some prohibited advertising practices are deontologically unethical, some
teleologically unethical, and others are both, but either type of unethicality should be sufficient to prohibit
an advertisement.
The following section provides a brief overview of the advertising regulation system in Australia,
including the operation and recent outcomes of the complaints system. This is followed by a review of
some ethical issues which are currently facing Australian advertisers and discussion of industry responses
to these issues.
64
Regulation of Advertising in Australia
Self-regulation of advertising is the favored option of industry groups in most countries, with selfregulatory systems being readily proposed whenever there is a fear of tighter or stronger government
regulation.
Following the demise of the Australian commonwealth government's Advertising Standards Council in
1996, the major industry body, the Australian Association of National Advertisers (AANA), developed
the Advertiser Code of Ethics (which applies to all forms of advertising), and established the Advertising
Standards Board (ASB) and the Advertising Claims Board (ACB) to deal with complaints and breaches of
this code. Under the new regulatory system, the ASB deals with complaints about taste and decency in
advertising, and the ACB deals with rival advertiser complaints (Baker et al. 1998). The authority of the
two boards rests on the willingness of advertisers to adhere voluntarily to ethical standards.
This paper focuses on complaints lodged with, and decisions made by the ASB. Section 2 of the AANA
Code (the section administered by the ASB) specifies that advertisements shall:
• not portray people in a way which discriminates or vilifies a person or section of the
community on account of race, ethnicity, nationality, sex, age, sexual preference, religion,
disability or political belief;
• not present or portray violence unless it is justifiable in the context of the product or service
advertised;
• treat sex, sexuality and nudity with sensitivity to the relevant audience and, where
appropriate, the relevant program time zone;
• for any product which is meant to be used by or purchased by children not contain anything
which is likely to cause alarm or distress to those children;
• use only language which is appropriate in the circumstances and strong or obscene language
shall be avoided;
• not depict material contrary to prevailing community standards on health and safety;
• comply with the Federal Chamber of Automotive Industries Code of Practice relating to
Advertising for Motor Vehicles.
The ASB administers the code, which is funded by an industry body called the Australian Advertising
Standards Council Ltd (AASC). It is interesting to note the very different messages that are conveyed to
the two key target groups by the AASC.
On the ASB website, which provides information to the general public on the function of the ASB and the
process of lodging a complaint about advertising, the following statement of intent is provided: “The selfregulatory system, now managed by the Advertising Standards Bureau Ltd ACN 084 452 666 and funded
voluntarily by the industry through the Australian Advertising Standards Council Ltd, recognizes that
advertisers share a common interest in promoting consumer confidence in and respect for general
standards of advertising”. This feel-good statement implies a deontological perspective on the purpose of
regulation – the industry cares about the consumer and wants advertising to be a positive thing.
In the full-page ads that the AASC placed in B&T Weekly (a major trade publication) in June and July of
2001, a very different perspective is conveyed to the advertising industry readership. Each ad features a
quote in large print, taking up over three-quarters of the page, from a leading industry figure (such as
“Half a page of legals? We can squeeze that in. – Ron Mather, Creative Director, The Campaign Palace”;
and “One person complained and you want us to pull it? No problem. – Reg Bryson, CEO, The Campaign
Palace”). In each case, the smaller text at the bottom of the page reads: “Can’t imagine him saying that?
Neither can we. However, if the Government were to regulate our industry he may not have a choice.
Thankfully, there’s the Australian Advertising Standards Council which is an industry funded body. The
Council promotes creative freedom within what is acceptable under law and community standards.
65
Without it, advertising and advertisers would be at the mercy of overly restrictive legislation. The small
levy of 35c for every $1000 of media spend is all it takes to maintain the Advertising Standards Bureau.
For more information call 02 9233 8874. PAY THE LEVY OR PAY THE PRICE”. This campaign
presents a far more teleological, industry-centred, argument for self-regulation of advertising.
Handling of Complaints
Harker (2003) reviewed 12 years of complaints to the now defunct Advertising Standards Council, which
assessed complaints from both the public and rival advertisers, and found more than ten thousand
complaints and two thousand breaches of the codes (i.e., approximately 20% of complaints were upheld).
In comparison, of the total of 1,190 decisions on complaints against advertisements which were reported
by the ASB between 1999 and 2001, the most recent year for which figures are available (ASB 1999;
2000; 2001), only 24 of the decisions (i.e., 2%) were to uphold the complaint.1 The only categories of
products in which more than one complaint was upheld in the three-year period were: retail (5); vehicles
(4); clothing (2); travel (3); and community awareness (2). One complaint was upheld in each of the
categories entertainment, housegoods/services, media, office goods/services, restaurants, sport & leisure,
toiletries, and toys & games. There were an additional 15 product categories for which none of the
complaints were upheld, including those which have self-regulatory codes (alcohol, therapeutic goods,
and slimming).
Table 1 shows the proportion of complaints lodged under each of the clauses of the AANA Code of
Ethics which were upheld by the ASB.
Table 1: Complaints lodged, and upheld, under each section of the Code 1999-2001
Lodged
Discrimination/vilification
505
Violence
178
Portrayal of sex/sexuality/nudity
419
Causes alarm or distress to children 40
Language
96
Health & safety
354
Other (not under specific clause)
173
Upheld
10
5
3
2
1
10
0
Percentage
2.0%
1.6%
0.7%
5.0%
1.0%
2.8%
0.0%
The following section reviews two of the major ethical issues – under the headings of fast cars and fast
food – which have arisen for Australian advertisers in the last two years, generally as a result of public
opinion or pressure from lobby groups or government.
Fast Cars
Road safety advocates and government authorities in several countries have raised the possibility that ads
for new cars which emphasize “performance” and often explicitly show vehicles being driven at excessive
speed may increase the likelihood of people exposed to these ads driving in an unsafe manner (i.e.,
teleological ethical argument). There has been much media attention on fast car ads over the last two
years, with government spokespeople, the Australian Automobile Association, and other groups drawing
a direct link between portrayals of unsafe driving and road injuries (Masanauskas 2002; Hagon 2001).
It is important to note, however, that this supposition has not been put to an empirical test; and the sole
voice of dissent, the Federal Chamber of Automotive Industries (Brewer 2002), has been given very little
1
Note that this number is considerably lower than the number of complaints received (e.g., 1,705 in 2001 alone) as
many ads attract multiple complaints, and other are not considered by the ASB as they fall outside of the Board’s
Charter.
66
opportunity to respond. Despite the absence of evidence for a negative impact, the teleological argument
that such ads may cause harm has resulted in an increased focus on the regulation of new car advertising.
For example, in May 2001, the New Zealand LTSA ordered the withdrawal of two television ads for
Mitsubishi Diamante and Ford XR8 vehicles which were deemed to be in breach of a section of the
Advertising Codes of Practice, which states that ads should not “glorify excessive speed and/or unsafe
driving practices” (LTSA 2001). The Mitsubishi Diamante commercial showed the car beating a bullet in
a sprint. The Ford XR8 commercial showed a specialized vehicle (propelled by a jet engine) travelling
along salt plains at extreme speed; the XR8 pulls up alongside, and the two passengers joke that the other
vehicle “must be stuck in third” before accelerating past it. When the same advertisements banned in
New Zealand were aired in Australia they drew public criticism from state transport departments and the
Automobile Association of Australia. Interestingly, complaints about both ads banned by the New
Zealand LTSA were dismissed by the Australian ASB. The complaint about the Ford XR8 ad was
dismissed as “the advertisement was clearly fanciful and fictitious and…did not contravene prevailing
community standards on health and safety” (ASB 327/00). The complaint about the Mitsubishi ad
(branded a Mitsubishi Magna VR-X in Australia) was dismissed as “the advertisement did not contravene
prevailing community standards on health and safety” (ASB 320/00). As with all the other reported
complaint dismissals, no survey of community standards was conducted, nor a survey of opinions of the
ads, to prove this deontological denial.
In the period 1999-2001, a total of 97 decisions on complaints against motor vehicle advertisements were
reported by the ASB (ASB 1999; 2000; 2001), four of which (4.1%) were upheld. However, of these
complaints, only 54 were considered under clause 2.6 (health and safety), with the remainder of
complaints relating to issues such as vilification and portrayal of sexuality. Of the 54 healthy and safety
complaints, exactly half related to portrayals of speeding or unsafe driving (with the remainder being
largely about other unsafe behaviors portrayed by characters in the ads). All four of the upheld
complaints fell into this category (speeding or unsafe driving), thus approximately 15% of complaints
about motor vehicle ads portraying unsafe driving were upheld by the ASB.
In response to the outcry over motor vehicle advertising, the Federal Chamber of Automotive Industries
(FCAI) introduced a voluntary code of practice on August 8, 2002, with a grandfather clause allowing
previously produced ads to continue airing until December 2002. This voluntary code of practice restricts
the use of advertisements depicting speeding on public roads (which theoretically would not have
excluded either of the advertisements mentioned above). Specifically, the FCAI code prohibits:
obviously unsafe driving, including
•
reckless and menacing driving to the extent that such practices would breach any Commonwealth
law or the law of any State or Territory in the relevant jurisdiction in which the advertisement is
published or broadcast dealing with road safety or traffic regulation, were they to occur on a road
or road-related area.
•
people driving on a road or road-related area at speeds in excess of speed limits in the relevant
jurisdiction in Australia in which the advertisement is published or broadcast.
•
driving practices which clearly take place on a road or road-related area and which breach any
Commonwealth law or the law of any State or Territory in the relevant jurisdiction in which the
advertisement is published or broadcast directly dealing with road safety or traffic regulation.
•
people clearly driving under the influence of drugs or alcohol to the extent that such driving
practices breach any Commonwealth law or the law of any State or Territory in the relevant
67
jurisdiction in which the advertisement is published or broadcast dealing directly with road safety
or traffic regulation.
•
motorists driving while clearly fatigued to the extent that such driving practices breach any
Commonwealth law or the law of any State or Territory in the relevant jurisdiction in which the
advertisement is published or broadcast dealing directly with road safety or traffic regulation.
•
motorcyclists or their passengers not wearing an approved safety helmet, while the motorcycle is
in motion.
•
deliberate and significant environmental damage, particularly while advertising off-road motor
vehicles.
As with other self-regulatory codes, the industry expressed support for the new code, with the Advertising
Federation of Australia spokesperson quoted as saying that the AFA “welcomed the code, saying it would
preserve the ability of advertisers to use fantasy, humor and creativity, but addressed people's concerns”
(Plaskitt 2002).
The new voluntary code, like the existing voluntary codes, is administered by the ASB. It will not be
until the 2003 case reports are made available that we will be able to comprehensively examine the extent
to which this new code has been applied. However, within months of the code being implemented the
system was being criticized as being ineffectual by the Australian Automobile Association, Australian
College of Road Safety, and the Pedestrian Council of Australia; with the main complaints being that
advertisers were using loopholes in the code to continue promoting unsafe practices (such as the clause
which applies only to ads involving public roads), and the lengthy delays in the complaints process which
result in campaigns finishing before complaints about them are heard (Anonymous 2003a; Smith 2003).
Clearly the advertiser has two options: comply with the spirit of the code or comply with the word of the
code. Both Holden (owned by General Motors) and Ford have been criticized for choosing the latter
option. Holden’s previous ad showed an SS utility vehicle creating a dust-storm by performing
doughnuts in a paddock; the new ad “ad implies a young driver has created crop circles with the new car,
but there is never any direct footage of it happening. The same Thunderstruck theme song is played in the
background to remind viewers of the previous ad” (Anonymous 2003b, p. 51). Ford’s previous ad
showed an XR8 utility vehicle blasting past a land-speed record car on a salt pan; the new ad “shows the
two drivers from the first salt-pan ad dawdling along and being trailed by police cars and helicopters,
apparently waiting for the two to start speeding” (Anonymous 2003b, p. 51).
Fast Food
“The snack food industry alone spends hundreds of millions of dollars each year pushing its
products to teens and pre-teens, with the likes of Coke, Pepsi, McDonald's and Pizza Hut vying
for exclusive rights to cafeterias, hallways and athletic fields” (Applebaum 2003, p. 20). The
obesity epidemic in the U.S. has led to heated debate about the ethicality of such advertising in schools,
with many school districts moving towards limiting or banning the advertising of snack foods and other
“unhealthy” products in the school environs. These prohibitions are based on the teleological perspective
that such advertising leads to an increase in unhealthy eating among young people. Abbarno (2001)
expressed the view that the commercialization of education by business-school partnerships with
companies such as Channel One is wrong on deontological grounds. However, the deontological
argument for the banning of all advertising in schools (including software, toys etc.) leads to an apparent
68
inconsistency in the encouraging of “healthy” food advertising in schools, such as the Dole 5 A Day
Program (reviewed by Applebaum 2003).
The debate in Australia has focused largely on the advertising of fast foods, soft drinks, and high-calorie
snack foods – with the fast food industry particularly being held responsible for the obesity epidemic by
many critics – and widespread calls for restrictions on advertising of “unhealthy” foods during children’s
viewing times (see, for example, Pritchard 2003).
Mass media interest has been fanned by the release of a series of major studies of food advertising to
children. In New South Wales, a 2001 State Government survey reported that the majority of the state’s
children are watching between seven and 28 food advertisements per day, most of which are for foods
high in fat, sugar, or salt. Headlines were sensational, such as “Children fed an unhealthy diet of ads”,
and were supported by alarming figures such as “one in four Australian children aged between two and 17
now classified as overweight or obese… children are obese and developing conditions like Type 2
diabetes and high blood pressure” (Patty 2002, p. 11). In Queensland, a 2001 University of Queensland
study funded by a major health insurer found that three in four advertisements shown between 3.30p.m.
and 7.30p.m. in Brisbane were for confectionery, soft drinks, chips, desserts, processed snacks and fast
food. The survey results were reported in the major weekend newspaper as “The junk your children see
on television” and supported by figures on childhood obesity (Tallon 2001). In South Australia, a 2002
Flinders University study found that 80 per cent of food ads shown during Children’s and General
Viewing TV shows were for food with marginal nutrition value, and that most of these ads were for fast
foods. News stories appeared with headlines such as “Ads for snacks breach TV laws” and the study
results were again presented alongside alarming obesity statistics (Crouch 2003). An Australia-wide
study conducted by the Australian Division of General Practice during the 2002/2003 Summer vacation
found that more than 99 per cent of food ads shown during children's TV were for fast foods and there
were no healthy eating messages broadcast. This story received national coverage, with headlines like
“GPs demand ban on junk food adverts” and even more far-reaching implications than the previous two
studies, such as “Type 2 diabetes, a disease linked with poor diet and previously seen very rarely in
children…children who developed this disease were at risk of an early onset of kidney failure, eye
problems, poor circulation and heart disease” (Anonymous 2003c).
Industry concern was heightened after the New Zealand national press reported that the NZ government
was examining a ban on junk food ads aimed at children, and Australian childhood obesity forums began
to call for a ban on fast food, confectionery and soft drink advertisements targeting children (Esplin
2002). The AANA and AFA responded with strongly worded arguments that obesity was the result of
personal choices not of advertising (Ligerakis 2002). Thus, the industry heartily welcomed an Australian
federal government proposal for a joint task force (including the Australian Association of National
Advertisers, the Advertising Federation of Australia, Commercial Television Australia, the Federation of
Commercial Radio Stations, the Australian Press Council and Magazine Publishers of Australia) to
develop a public awareness campaign to target childhood obesity. The AFA’s executive director Lesley
Brydon was quoted as saying that “the AFA was pleased the Government had flagged that it did not
consider the banning of ads an appropriate solution” (Ryan 2002). The Sun Herald newspaper
interviewed Robert Koltai, AANA vice-chairman, about the new campaign and reported that “he
conceded it was designed to take the heat off the advertising industry for its role in promoting fast food to
children” (Leggatt 2003).
At an individual company level, one response to this public outcry has been to modify foods to improve
their nutritional content; but a far more noticeable and prevalent response has been to modify food
advertising to improve perceived nutritional content. A good example of the former is the sandwich chain
Subway with its low-fat sandwich options (which have been both heavily advertised and widely
69
applauded); and a good example of the latter is the current McDonald’s TV ad campaign with claims like
“only real milk goes into our milkshakes” (as opposed to artificial milk). Many companies are making
effective use of advertising to imply that the food is good for you rather than actually ensuring that it is –
in the words of an Advertising Federation of Australia (AFA) executive, “…the trend is to tie products to
healthful pursuits. Even in ads for burgers or pizza, you increasingly see people playing sports, working
out or playing at the beach. They're working up an appetite – and burning up calories” (Pritchard 2003, p.
7). This is an ethically questionable response from both the deontological (deception in advertising) and
teleological (promoting food that is bad for people will have a negative impact on the nation’s health)
ethical perspectives. Across the other side of the globe, McDonald’s France apparently has incurred the
wrath of McDonald’s U.S. for its new ad campaign which advises that children shouldn’t eat at
McDonald’s more than once a week; not surprisingly, McDonald’s U.S. said it “strongly disagreed” with
the nutritionist quoted in the French ad (Anonymous 2002a).
In the period 1999-2001, a total of 176 decisions on complaints against food or beverage (not including
alcohol) advertisements were reported by the ASB (ASB 1999; 2000; 2001). Of these, none was upheld.
The clauses under which the complaints were assessed were health & safety (69),
discrimination/vilification (68), portrayal of sex/sexuality/nudity (40), violence (31), other (26), causes
alarm or distress to children (10) and language (10). These numbers add to more than the number of
complaints as complaints can be lodged and assessed under more than one clause. It is interesting to note
that the complaints in the health and safety category were not about the nutritional value of the food, the
targeting of children, or the accuracy of nutritional complaints; rather, they were about other unsafe or
unhealthy practices people were engaging in while preparing or eating the foods advertised.
CONCLUSION
First, the advertising industry, at least in Australia, appears to be doing a reasonable job of demonstrating
to the public that it places a high value on teleological ethics. In the two cases discussed above, the
industry took action to tackle the believed negative consequences of advertising on the community.
When it was argued that portrayals of fast or unsafe driving in car ads leads to speeding and unsafe
driving among the general public, the industry proposed a voluntary code to regulate such portrayals.
When it was argued that junk food advertising causes children to become obese and inactive, the industry
offered to develop an ad campaign to encourage healthy eating and exercise. These actions, however,
must be viewed with a certain degree of scepticism – particularly in light of the AASC ad campaign, the
appearance of new ads that skate dangerously near the edge of the new codes, and some of the more
candid comments by industry spokespeople, which may imply that the bad outcome that the industry was
really trying to avoid was the industry-damaging outcome of increased regulation. However, from a
teleological perspective, the important thing is that the hypothesized bad outcomes from advertising (such
as increases in childhood obesity and unsafe driving) may be avoided, or at least reduced, by the
industry’s actions. It is also important to note that in these two, as in several other, cases there is no
conclusive evidence to demonstrate that the now-prohibited advertising practices do actually lead to the
bad outcomes.
Second, the advertising industry, at least in Australia, appears to be doing a poor job of demonstrating to
the public that it places a high value on deontological ethics. Of the six clauses of the AANA Code of
Ethics, at least four are based on a deontological perspective of right and wrong: (2.1)
discrimination/vilification; (2.2) violence; (2.3) portrayal of sex/sexuality/nudity; and (2.5) offensive
language. For each of these, less than 2% of the complaints lodged with the ASB have been upheld. The
contradiction is seen in the communications issued by the AASC as discussed in this article: the public
face of an industry that claims to freely donate a proportion of its earnings to ensure “consumer
confidence in and respect for general standards of advertising” and the more private face that motivates
members to contribute in order to avoid “overly restrictive legislation”. While it could be argued, and
indeed is in one of the AASC’s trade newspaper ads, that an ad should not be removed because it
70
offended one person, there is no clear yardstick by which we can decide how many or what proportion of
people must be offended by an ad before it is deemed unacceptable. As Fletcher (2001) argues in relation
to regulation in the U.K. “…the Advertising Code of Practice does not say advertisements must not give
serious widespread offence. It says advertisements must not give serious or widespread offence. An
advertisement that gives serious offence to a minority - to gay men, say, or to TBWA creatives - is just as
unacceptable as an ad that gives less offence to more people” (Fletcher 2001).
The ASB is composed of a group of people chosen to represent the community and currently consists
largely of individuals from media-related industries. Whereas these people are arguably very
knowledgeable about mass media and the arts, they could not be said to constitute a broad spectrum of
everyday Australians. It could be argued that part of the problem arises from the very nature of this
Board; a group of broadminded individuals who are repeatedly exposed to potentially offensive ads are
likely to become jaded over time and to see the ads from a different perspective to the “average”
Australian. Perhaps one solution would be to create a more reasonable yardstick for the measurement of
prevailing community attitudes, such as annually selecting a random sample of Australians and obtaining
their laypersons’ opinions on whether particular ads are within or outside community standards in relation
to portrayals of sexuality, vilification, violence, and offensive language.
It is also important to maintain a realistic view of the purpose of advertising, and of the role of the
industry in the commercial world. If the industry were to agree to never use an image that may offend
anyone or a message that may lead to an unhealthy behavior, there would be little left for advertising to
do. As one writer pointed out in response to the current U.S. debate over whether to end the moratorium
on liquor ads, “Whatever the outcome, one thing is certain - U.S. advertisers are not known for their
subtlety, so ads featuring unattractive, older people not having good time would make for some very
unusual viewing” (Devaney 2002, p. 33). Already in Australia, ads parodying the new codes have
appeared.
REFERENCES
Smith, J. R. 2001. Reference style guidelines. Journal of Guidelines 4 (2): 2-7 [or 4:2-7].
Abbarno, G.J.M. 2001. Huckstering in the classroom: Limits to corporate social responsibility. Journal
of Business Ethics 32(2): 179-189.
Anonymous. 2002a. Le Mac warning. Herald Sun November 1: 11.
Anonymous. 2002b. Advertisers support Puttnam proposals over self-regulation. Campaign Aug 2: 1.
Anonymous. 2003a. Calls for tougher stand on car ads. B&T Weekly: 4.
Anonymous. 2003b. Fast work gets ads over the line. Herald Sun April 4: 51.
Anonymous. 2003c. GPs demand ban on junk food adverts. Manly Daily March 28.
Applebaum, M. 2003. Don t spare the brand. Brandweek 44(10): 20-26.
ASB. 1999a. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board January-June 1999. Sydney: Advertising
Standards Bureau Ltd.
ASB. 1999b. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board July-December 1999. Sydney:
Advertising Standards Bureau Ltd.
ASB. 2000a. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board January-June 2000. Sydney: Advertising
Standards Bureau Ltd.
71
ASB. 2000b. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board July-December 2000. Sydney:
Advertising Standards Bureau Ltd.
ASB. 2001a. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board January-June 2001. Sydney: Advertising
Standards Bureau Ltd.
ASB. 2001b. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board July-December 2001. Sydney:
Advertising Standards Bureau Ltd.
Brewer, P. 2002. Lobby rejects ads-accidents link. Canberra Times March 26: A3.
Brinkmann, J. 2002. Business and marketing ethics as professional ethics. Concepts, approaches and
typologies. Journal of Business Ethics 41(1/2): 159-177.
Crouch, B. 2001. Ads for snacks breach TV laws. Sunday Mail August 5: 19.
Devaney, P. 2002. Prohibition persists on the airwaves of America. Marketing Week Mar 28: 32-33.
Esplin, H. 2002. NSW: Calls for food ads on TV to be banned. AAP Newsfeed: September 11.
Fletcher, W. 2001. Rubegg off, fcuk: Your ads are blloocks that just peddle smut. Marketing Nov 22: 20.
Gutwirth, N. 2002. The right to remain silent. Advertising Age 73(38): 22.
Hagon, T. 2001. Car industry urged to put brakes on ads. The Age December 13: 9.
Harker, D. 2003. The importance of industry compliance in improving advertising self-regulatory
processes. Journal of Public Affairs 3(1): 63-75.
Leggatt, J. 2003. Ad industry urges kids to eat right. Sun Herald March 9: 30.
Ligerakis, M. 2002. Industry slams food ad bans. B&T Weekly September 11: 2.
Masanauskas, J. 2002. Brakes applied to car ads. Herald Sun March 25: 5.
McOstrich, N. 2002. Crossing the line. Marketing Magazine 107(45): 24.
Patty, A. 2002. Children fed an unhealthy diet of ads. The Daily Telegraph July 22: 11.
Phillips, M. 1993. Tough times in adland. Australian Business Monthly 13(4): 78-82.
Plaskitt, S. 2002. Brakes put on fast car ads. B&T Weekly August 19: 3.
Powell, C. 2003. The devil may care. Marketing Magazine 108(1): 14.
Pritchard, C. 2003. Food marketers feel the heat Down Under. Marketing Magazine 10(12): 7.
Ryan, R. 2002. Taskforce tackles obesity ads. B&T Weekly December 6: 2.
Smith, R. 2003. Car ads defy ban. The Sunday Telegraph April 6: 42.
Sutter, S. 2002. Innovation or Satanism? Marketing Magazine 107(46): 30.
Tallon, S. 2001. The junk your children see on television. Sunday Mail September 9: 32.
72
What Does It Take to Get Published
in a Top Marketing Journal from Australasia?
Daniela Rosenstreich, University of Otago, NZ
Ben Wooliscroft, University of Otago, NZ
Abstract
Australasian marketing academics frequently report a perception of being at considerable disadvantage
when submitting papers to the top, USA based, marketing journals. This paper investigates those
perceptions and finds that the playing field of top peer reviewed marketing journals may not be level.
Editorial board membership is heavily dominated by USA based marketers and there are reports of
difficulties publishing from Australasia in USA based (top) journals. A number of suggestions are made
by the panel to aid in being published in USA marketing journals.
Introduction
Journals are the primary medium for the communication of scholarly knowledge in most academic
disciplines, including marketing (AMA Task Force, 1988; Baumgartner & Pieters, 2003; Coe &
Weinstock, 1983; Jensen et al., 1995; Pecotich and Everett, 1989). The importance of journals to the
discipline also has a financial basis due to the emphasis on research outputs in the systems employed to
determine promotion and allocation of funding within the academic world (Ganesh, Chandy and
Henderson, 1990; Gross, Harmon & Reidy, 2002; Klein & Chang, 2004; Rotfeld, 1997a, 1997b). If
published marketing research and theory is to reflect the richness of diversity in the world it would appear
essential that the ‘leading’ marketing journals are prepared to publish material from countries other than
America and Canada.
Despite the importance of scholarly journals to the discipline, there are concerns about the quality of what
is published: For example, Hubbard offers examples of flawed studies that were published in peerreviewed journals as evidence that many published marketing studies are inaccurate and that “the integrity
of marketing’s empirical literature is potentially compromised”(1994 p. 361). Similarly, Sawyer cites
several journal editors to back up his assertion that “bad writing is a major problem for all journals in
marketing and consumer research” (1998 p. 49). Armstrong (2002) points out that, despite the quality
assurance provided by peer review, errors can be found in most published articles.
If the material published within the discipline does not always meet expected standards of research and
writing, it leads one to question what factors other than the intrinsic worth of the research and writing of a
manuscript influence whether it is published.
This paper will consider the background evidence of geographic biases in the top marketing journals
before highlighting the perceptions of leading New Zealand marketing academics with regard to
publishing in those top journals.
Background
Ten journals were selected for analysis based on being highly ranked by several recent studies (Easton &
Easton, 2003; Hult, Neese, & Bashaw, 1997; Koojarenprasit et al., 1988; Mort et al., 2004; Theoharakis
& Hirst, 2002): Journal of Marketing (JM), Journal of Consumer Research (JCR), Journal of Marketing
73
Research (JMR), Marketing Science (MS), Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science (JAMS), Journal
of Retailing (JR), International Journal of Research in Marketing (IJRM), Journal of Business Research
(JBR), Journal of Advertising (JA), Journal of Advertising Research (JAR).
Content analysis was carried out on the journals to investigate aspects of both the authors and the data
sources of articles within selected issues. As a current rather than historical account was sought, analysis
was based on two recent years of material (2002 to 2004).
The authorship of all articles was analysed and for articles that contained empirical data further analysis
was undertaken to investigate the geographic origin of the data source. The data source analysis was
based on the location of the respondents or study subjects - in the case of web-based studies, an
international data source was assumed if no information was provided on the geographic location of the
research subjects.
Origin of Authors and Data
Table 1: Content Analysis — Incidence of USA Authors
Articles with
Articles with ≥
Article
Journal issues analysed
no USA
1 USA authors
s
authors
JM
Vol.66:4 (2002) - 68:3
69
3
4.3% 66
95.7%
(2004)
JCR Vol. 29:2 (2002) - 31:1
100
13
13.0% 87
87.0%
(2004)
JMR Vol.39:2 (2002) - 41:2
78
7
9.0% 71
91.0%
(2004)
MS
Vol.21:3 (2002) - 23:2
57
4
7.0% 53
93.0%
(2004)
JAMS Vol.30:3 (2002) - 32:2
72
10
13.9% 62
86.1%
(2004)
JR
Vol.78:3 (2002) - 80:2
43
7
16.3% 36
83.7%
(2004)
IJRM Vol. 19:3 (2002) - 21:2
44
15
34.1% 29
65.9%
(2004
JBR
Vol.55:1 (2002) - 57:6
257
107
41.6% 150
58.4%
(2004)
JA
Vol.31:3 (2002) - 33:2
56
10
17.9% 46
82.1%
(2004)
JAR
Vol.42:3 (2002) - 44:2
96
18
18.8% 78
81.3%
(2004)
Articles with
all USA
authors
54
78.3%
74
74.0%
55
70.5%
40
70.2%
55
76.4%
30
69.8%
15
34.1%
113
44.0%
41
73.2%
70
72.9%
As seen in Table 1. JM had the highest proportion of USA-authored articles with 95.7% of articles
including at least one USA-based author, and a high proportion of articles (78.3%) contained no authors
from outside the USA. JCR, JMR, MS, JAMS, JA and JAR all also had high proportions of articles with
at least one USA author (83.1% to 93.0%). There were only two journals where the proportion of articles
with input from a USA author fell under 70%: IJRM (65.9%) and JBR (58.4%).
Overall, across all ten journals the average proportion of articles with at least one USA author was 84.4%.
In other words, less than 20% of all articles published in the top ten marketing journals in recent years
were written without the involvement of an author based in the USA.
74
The next stage of the content analysis was a review of the incidence of articles with data from the USA
(Table 2).
Table 2: Content Analysis — Incidence of USA Data
Articles with Specifically
Journal issues analysed
real world data includes USA
JM
Vol.66:4 (2002) - 68:3
57
49 86.0%
(2004)
JCR Vol. 29:2 (2002) – 31:1
88
71 80.7%
(2004)
JMR Vol.39:2 (2002) - 41:2
71
55 77.5%
(2004)
MS
Vol.21:3 (2002) - 23:2
39
29 74.4%
(2004)
JAMS Vol.30:3 (2002) - 32:2
46
35 76.1%
(2004)
JR
Vol.78:3 (2002) - 80:2
43
32 74.4%
(2004)
IJRM Vol. 19:3 (2002) - 21:2
32
12 37.5%
(2004
JBR
Vol.55:1 (2002) - 57:6
208
110 52.9%
(2004)
JA
Vol.31:3 (2002) - 33:2
52
38 73.1%
(2004)
JAR
Vol.42:3 (2002) - 44:2
75
50 66.7%
(2004)
Does not
include
Data source
unclear
7
12.3%
1
1.8%
14
15.9%
3
3.4%
10
14.1%
6
8.5%
10
25.6%
0
0.0%
8
17.4%
3
6.5%
7
16.3%
4
9.3%
19
59.4%
1
3.1%
95
45.7%
3
1.4%
13
25.0%
1
1.9%
20
26.7%
5
6.7%
JM had the lowest incidence of non-USA based empirical articles with only 12.3% of articles not
specifically including data from the USA, followed closely by JMR, JR, JAMS, and JCR with comparable
levels of articles without USA-based data, (ranging from 14.1% to 17.4%) , and the middle ground
occupied by JA (25.0%), MS (25.6%) and JAR (26.7%).
JBR and IJRM stood out by having a higher proportion of articles that did not specifically include data
from the USA (45.7% and 59.4% respectively). This was not surprising given the lower proportion of
USA authors in these journals uncovered in the previous analysis.
Locations of Editorial Board Members
A review of the geographic location of editorial board members revealed a strong North American
dominance. As shown in Table below, 85.3% of the 668 board members were based in USA. Only IJRM
has more than 20% of board members located outside USA, while at the other extreme, JCR, JR, JAMS,
JA, and JAR all have greater than 90% of board members based in USA.
The work of Kuhn and others offers explanation for why this pattern is found in marketing and
throughout science: Thomas Kuhn (1970) described science as involving communities of scientists who
subscribe to a particular paradigm, and these communities reject material that does not fit their
worldview. Foucault’s writing on legitimation has been interpreted in a similar vein, with groups of
experts controlling the production of knowledge through academic journals (Weiner, 1998). McNamee
75
and Willis assert that there is “considerable evidence” that an invisible college of academics “control, or
influence significantly” the core journals of a discipline (1994 p.397).
Table 3: Summary of Geographic Spread of Editorial Board Members
JCR JMR MS
JAMS JR
IJRM JBR
JM
JA
JAR
Overall
*
USA
88.7% 90.2% 84.9% 87.9% 91.4% 95.5% 49.4% 82.9% 96.6% 91.7% 85.3%
Canada
0.0% 1.4% 1.4% 4.0% 7.5% 1.5% 0.0% 6.3% 1.7% 0.0% 3.3%
Europe
7.8% 5.6% 8.2% 5.1% 1.1% 0.0% 34.9% 7.4% 0.0% 0.0% 7.0%
Australasia 0.9% 0.0% 1.4% 1.0% 0.0% 1.5% 4.8% 0.6% 1.7% 5.0% 3.3%
Asia
0.9% 2.1% 1.4% 0.0% 0.0% 1.5% 4.8% 1.1% 0.0% 0.0% 1.3%
Middle East 0.9% 0.0% 0.0% 1.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.6% 0.0% 0.0% 1.3%
South
0.9% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.3%
America
*Overall figures relate to the 668 individuals on the boards and vary from figures for the journals due to the people who serve on multiple boards.
The Kuhnian view of science can be applied to the marketing discipline as an explanation for the results
of the editorial board analysis. However, this study cannot draw conclusions regarding the degree to
which the network of editorial board members controls the research published within the marketing
discipline, nor can it resolve the question of whether such control does the discipline good or ill.
The analysis of the top journals provides evidence to corroborate the views on a USA dominance revealed
during the interviews. The data show clearly that the top journals contain a far greater proportion of
articles with USA authors and/or data than articles from the rest of the world combined.
JBR is an interesting example, with a North American dominated editorial board but among the lowest
rates of USA-based authors and/or data. The journal makes no claims in its editorial policies about
having an international focus (JBR Online, 2004), but its content suggests that there is research produced
internationally that is of a suitable standard for publication in a highly ranked journal. Five of the
journals with higher rates of USA input also make no claims regarding seeking international coverage of
the discipline: JA, JAR, JCR, JMR and JR..
On the other hand, the highly USA-dominated content of JM, MS and JAMS appears somewhat out of
step with their editorial policies: JM describes itself as: “…positioned as the premier, broad-based,
scholarly journal of the marketing discipline… …It is read internationally by marketers in education,
manufacturing, finance, health care, and other industries (JM Online, 2004). MS claims to enable readers
to “Discover the latest findings in the global marketplace” (MS Journal Homepage, 2004). The editorial
policies of JAMS purport to place a strong emphasis on an international focus, for example, “The Journal
of the Academy of Marketing Science is an international journal for the study and improvement of
marketing” and “comprehensively covers” areas including “international marketing” (AMS, 2003; JAMS
Online, 2004).
The title of IJRM signals that the journal is intended to be an international publication. However, for a
European journal, IJRM still has a large proportion of its board based in the USA, and more than two
thirds of its articles included one or more authors from the USA - a higher proportion than might be
expected from a European-based publication.
This study was merely an exploration to determine whether there is evidence to support the contentious
issues that emerged from the interviews’ results that follow. However, results of Czinkota’s (2000) study
support the validity of the findings of this smaller investigation. Czinkota (2000) investigated
76
international contributions to marketing journals and found that in the USA journals only 8.9% of articles
published over an eighteen-year period were written by non-USA affiliated authors. Looking at JM as an
example, in Czinkota’s study 4.7% of JM articles had no USA authors, a result very close to the 4.3%
found in this study.
Literature Review
A review of literature on academic publishing reveals several factors that can influence whether an article
is published. Perry, Carson and Gilmore (2003) and Day (1996) mention the benefits of citing material
from the journal being targeted: “Refer to other articles in the journal about your topic” (Day, 1996
p.656), and the importance of making a point of including citation of work by journal editors and
potential reviewers: “Get to know the work of the editor and those of his or her editorial board who have
written in your area, and show this knowledge by referring to their articles if those articles are at all
related to the planned paper” (Perry et al., 2003, p.656).
An author’s reputation is also suggested as an influence on publishing success (Kerr, Tolliver & Petree
1977; Peter & Olson, 1983; Urban, Wayland, & McDermott, 1992): Despite blind review, 30% of
reviewers and editors surveyed by Kerr et al. (1977) indicated that knowing the author has a justifiably
strong reputation in the area would increase the likelihood of acceptance of a manuscript. The idea that
published authors find it easier to publish again, is explained by Peter and Olson as the credibility of a
researcher having a “halo effect” (1983 p.113). They go on to suggest that unknown researchers (like
doctoral students) need to attract “celebrity scholars” to help market their research (Peter & Olson, 1983
p.113).
The notion that social contact and being part of an academic community are important for publishing
success was discussed under several separate themes in the literature. Aside from the notion of gaining
feedback and testing ideas (Bucholtz, 2004; Perry et al., 2003; Twedt, 1977), aspects of social contact
discussed included being up-to-date with developments in the field (Perry et al., 2003) and explaining and
selling ideas (Peter & Olson’s, 1983). Personal contact specifically with editors of journals was also
noted as a possible influence on publishing success (Czinkota, 2000; Moxley, 1992; Twedt, 1977): “It
helps to know the editors of professional journals” (Twedt, 1977 p.71).
The origin of the author was mentioned as a factor, specifically the idea that authors from outside the
USA face barriers to publication in USA journals. Homburg suggests that some reviewers see articles
that come from outside the USA as being of a lower quality, and that “many German colleagues…view
leading US-based marketing journals as somehow predisposed against manuscripts from outside the
United States” (2003, p.349). Easton and Easton describe the cause of the problem thus: “non-US
authors cannot compete on an equal basis since they face implicit and explicit barriers” based on distance
from the formal and informal institutions that surround the top USA based journals (2003 p.16). On the
same theme, Czinkota states that “in order to get published in US journals, the author needs to move to
the USA or work in the USA for some time” (2000 pp.1306-1307).
These literature sources suggest that dubious citation practices, being ‘known’ and getting to know the
right people, and a USA connection can all enhance the chances of an author being published in a top
marketing journal. However, none of the literature sources found were designed to offer a comprehensive
account of factors that influence publishing success. Further exploratory investigation was carried out by
soliciting the opinions solicited of an expert panel of senior Australasian marketing academics.
THE RESEARCH
The interview research was designed to answer the following question:
What factors (other than the quality of the research and writing) influence whether an article from New
Zealand is published in a top (American) academic marketing journal?
77
Method
Semi-structured interviews were selected as the primary means of data collection because they offer
participants greater opportunity to expand on their opinions and allow for clarification of responses
(Weiers, 1988). An interview protocol was developed to enhance the reliability of the data by providing
some standardisation of the process (Weiers, 1988).
Interviews were transcribed verbatim by the researcher and participants were sent a transcript to correct or
clarify to ensure that the interview content reflected their opinions. The checked transcripts were saved as
online documents within NUD-IST and coded into themes. Differences in interpretation of results can be
a major threat to reliability in qualitative research of this kind, particularly where responses are in the
form of comments coded into a limited range of categories (Henerson, Morris, & Fitz-Gibbon, 1987).
The coding approach used in the current study was rigorously applied and checked to reduce the
likelihood of variability.
Description of the Expert Panel
Armstrong (1997) suggests that a sample of between five and twenty people is appropriate for expert
opinion studies. An expert panel of fourteen senior academics was selected for this study. The fourteen
panel members were labeled from A to N and are referred to by these labels in the results to preserve their
anonymity.
The panel members together held 43 university degrees or diplomas from a variety of disciplines. The
number of years since participants received their first academic appointment ranged from seven to thirtyone years with an average of eighteen years and the average number of years since award of their
doctorate was 10.5 years. All panel members had held academic posts in NZ but experience in other
countries was also common: Six panel members had worked in the UK/Ireland, four had held academic
posts in the USA, and four had worked in continental Europe. Between them, the panel had published
307 peer-reviewed journal articles, and were involved in the editing or reviewing of an average of 6.6
peer-reviewed journals each.
Results
“I don’t think the final decision to publish is entirely based on the merits of the article” (Panel member H)
In line with the literature, several tactics for publishing in top (USA) marketing journals were suggested
by the interview panel, including making a point of citing relevant works from the journal being targeted,
and also citing the editor and potential reviewers:
Panel member G: “Sometimes it comes down to the simple things like…if you quoted research that the
editor has done, you’ve a chance of at least a first review…”.
Other submission tricks noted included a suggestion by Panel member F to leave an intentional flaw for
the reviewers to focus. Three panel members suggested that publishing success could be enhanced by
ruling out potentially negative reviewers by soliciting their feedback ahead of submission and
acknowledging them in the manuscript:
Panel member B: “If you don’t want someone to review your paper give them the paper for comment and
then acknowledge them for their comment so it won’t get sent to them under review”.
Two of the panel members described the ‘halo effect’ of author’s reputation that had been mentioned in
Peter and Olson’s (1983) work. The manner in which an author’s reputation makes it easier to be
published was described by one panel member:
Panel member H: “So if you have written a nice JM piece before and you come back with something
that’s maybe not quite so good but you’re known as being somebody whose done good work in the field
78
and is a thinker, it might go to a different reviewer who might think, “well that’s still worth putting out
there”.
The influence of image was also evident in the suggestion from two panel members that acknowledging
expert input on a manuscript will impress the editor and reviewers:
Panel member F: “… try to get important people in the field to review your article before you send it to
the journal and then you can cite them and thereby twist reviewers’ arms by saying ‘ah has already read it,
can’t be too bad then’ sort of thing”.
Contact with members of one’s academic community was generally accepted as important, with nine
panel members commenting on aspects of social contact. Like the literature sources, ideas on the form
and purpose of the social contact varied and explanations of the purpose of social contact ranged from
meeting potential collaborators, to getting noticed and promoting research:
Panel member C: “You’ve got to get to a conference where you might get noticed or you might meet
someone you could work with kind of a thing”.
One aspect of social contact, schmoozing, revealed some differences in opinion that cannot be easily
reconciled. Seven of the participants commented that getting to know influential people (such as, editors
and reviewers) increased the likelihood of being published.
Panel member G: “… if you’re networking with more senior academics who have been successful in a
particular field, then yes it does improve your chances of being published”.
Panel member I: “…building relationships with editors is really a good way of getting published”.
This practice of purposefully striking up relationships with important people was referred to as
schmoozing. Some of the panel members who believed that schmoozing aided in publishing success also
associated schmoozing with the notion that a clique of people control the top journals, as this comment
illustrates:
Panel member A: “I think that what gets you published is following the formula.
And unfortunately the formula is laid down in most of this
discipline by a very narrow clique of largely American
academics. And I think that has done a great deal of damage….
In the leading American journals I think there’s no doubt it’s a
matter of who you know rather than what you know.”
On the other hand, five panel members stated that schmoozing does not affect an author’s chances of
successful submission to a journal.
Panel member N: “…there are a very small number of people who can really do top
level research. And you see their names over and over again in
the top journals – it’s not because they have some automatic
pathway to the editor, sure they’re probably more known to the
editor, but that’s because they keep getting stuff in those
journals. And if they go to conferences etc… and so people who
think ‘Oh I can’t break into that group because I’m not known to
the editor, or I’m from New Zealand or I’m from Australia, they
don’t know about us in America’, they’re using that as an excuse
I think”.
79
It is interesting to note, however, that the groups of panel members presenting the opposing views on the
topic of schmoozing are not mutually exclusive — two participants expressed views on both sides of the
issue. The contradiction is clear in this statement:
Panel member D: “It’s not a game. Is it a game? No. I think you have to play the
game but I don’t think it’s a game. I mean, I hate to think that
my profession is a game. I don’t think it is, but it’s political, I
know someone as well who actually got accepted for a journal
that sent the paper in after me but who knows the editor and you
think well, if I knew the editor I’d be pulling his strings too”.
The discussions regarding influences on publishing success often turned towards demographic
characteristics of the authors. Seven of the panel members suggested that the geographic origin of the
author has an influence on whether an article is published. However, the nature of that influence was
expressed quite differently. For some panel members the influence was related to a bias such as
“patriotism” (Panel member F), for other panel members (primarily those with greater experience in
academia and publishing), the influence of nationality was due to the style of research conducted in
various countries, as illustrated by the comment below.
Panel member M: “Classic example of that was at the EMAC conference this year
– 70% of the Dutch papers got accepted and only 20% of the
Scandinavian papers got accepted. And that can’t only be
explained by quality - it can also be explained by different
styles of research. It can also be explained by who the
reviewers were and a lot of the Northern Europeans were
somewhat reluctant to be involved in reviewing processes, so
their perspectives and values in terms of type of research
wasn’t perhaps represented as clearly”
In many of the interviews, discussion turned specifically towards a perceived “North American
dominance” (Panel member M). On this topic the panel was again divided, with six panel members
suggesting that a coauthor or data from the USA increased the likelihood of publication in at least some
journals. On the other hand, five panel members expressed the view that USA input was not needed.
Panel members J and L appeared on both sides of the argument – they affirmed that USA data is needed
for publication in JM, but stated that it is not necessarily a requirement for other journals. Some panel
members cited examples of people who had successfully published in the USA without USA data or
coauthors:
Panel member N: “Well some people talk about issues like if you try to publish
in a US journal with New Zealand data stuff like that – they
get all hung up about that. I’ve published with New Zealand
data many times in top journals – it’s never been a problem.
So, if other people comment on that, I think that they’re
making excuses for themselves”.
However, examples were also proffered on the other side of the argument, for example Panel member K
described his own experience with one of the top journals:
Panel member K: “I’ve had papers rejected from JR because they didn’t have
American data. So they didn’t think it was of interest to
Americans. Maybe they should change it to the ‘Journal of
80
American Retailing’…The comments were basically exactly on
those lines ‘Replicate this with American data and.…it will be
published’. So that’s another influence”.
Discussion
The investigation of factors that influence whether an article is published in a top academic marketing
journal revealed agreement on several themes in both the literature and the interviews. There was
however a lack of consensus among the expert panel on two topics: There was disagreement over
whether schmoozing with the right people influenced publishing success, and whether including USA
authors and/or data increased the likelihood of publication in the top journals.
While there is clearly a very high proportion of articles with USA authors and/or data published in the top
journals, the nature of the relationship between the origin of the author and their success in being
published is not clear and cannot be established from the simple analyses carried out within this study.
However, several possible justifications warrant discussion.
The USA dominance could be explained by the fact that nine of the ten journals originate in that country.
However, the publicity of many suggests they seek to present an international perspective. Furthermore,
IJRM, which is a European-based journal, still contains a high level of USA-produced content.
Therefore, the country of origin of the journal does not fully explain the high degree of USA-based
content in the top ten journals.
Another possible explanation for the predominance of USA authors and data is that the readers of the
journals prefer the American-style content and the journal editors are simply ‘giving their customers what
they want’. Subscription data were not part of the current study, however, the fact that these ten journals
have been highly ranked by sources from UK and Australasia (for example, Easton & Easton, 2003; Mort
et al., 2004) suggests satisfaction with the USA-dominated content.
A further explanation for the bias towards USA-based content in the top journals is that the quality of
work produced in the USA is seen as superior to that produced elsewhere. The boards comprise mostly
USA academics so the manuscripts submitted are assessed by people immersed in the USA style of
research and writing. Pecotich, Pressly, and Roth’s (1996) finding that country of origin influences
assessments of quality for at least some offerings, can be related to this phenomena: If editorial board
members have a subconscious preference for material from their own country, they may well rationalize it
via an assessment that their own country’s style of research is superior.
The most plausible explanations for the high incidence of USA data and authors in the top journals are
therefore a real or assumed reader preference, and/or editorial board member preference for particular
research approaches, writing style, and/or subject matter.
The following factors were suggested by panel members as having a positive influence on publishing
success within the marketing discipline:
• Inclusion of citations from targeted journals and members of their editorial review boards.
• Ruling out negative reviewers by involving them ahead of submission.
• Inclusion of an intentional flaw for reviewers to find.
• The image of a well known author and/or mention of expert input on a manuscript.
• Social contact with the academic community to solicit feedback, stay up-to-date, identify
potential collaborators, and promote research.
81
•
•
Schmoozing or getting to know the right people – becoming connected to the small network of
editorial board members.
Matching the theoretical and methodological paradigms and style preferences of the editorial
board members (for example by including USA data and/or authors).
If the marketing discipline is to benefit from the richness available in countries other than USA, then
many of these ‘requirements’ to publish must be removed.
REFERENCES
AMA Task Force. 1988. The Development of Marketing Thought: Developing, Disseminating and
Utilizing Marketing Knowledge. Journal of Marketing 52 (October): 1-25.
AMS. 2003. JAMS Online. Academy of Marketing Science. Retrieved 20 September, 2004, from
http://www.j-ams.org/.
Armstrong, J. S. 1997. Peer Review for Journals: Evidence on Quality Control, Fairness, and Innovation.
Retrieved 20 July, 2004, from
http://www- marketing.wharton.upenn.edu/people/faculty/armstrong2.html. (Also published in
Science and Engineering Ethics. 3: 63-84).
Armstrong, J. S. 2002. Discovery and Communication of Important Marketing Findings: Evidence and
Proposals. Journal of Business Research. 56 (1): 69-84.
Baumgartner, H. and R. Pieters, 2003. The Structural Influence of Marketing Journals: A Citation
Analysis of the Discipline and Its Sub Areas Over Time. Journal of Marketing 67 (2): 123-140.
Bucholtz, M. 2004, 27 March 2004. Tips for Academic Publishing. Retrieved 10 June 2004, from
http://www.linguisitics.ucsb.edu/faculty/bucholtz/sociocultural/publishingtips.html.
Coe, R. K. and I. Weinstock, 1983. Evaluating Journal Publications of Marketing Professors: A Second
Look. Journal of Marketing Education 5 (Spring): 37-42.
Czinkota, M. R. 2000. International Information Cross-Fertilization in Marketing—an Empirical
Assessment. European Journal of Marketing 34 (11/12): 1305-1314.
Day, A. 1996. How to Get Research Published in Journals. Aldershot: Gower.
Easton, G. and D. Easton, 2003. Marketing Journals and the Research Assessment Exercise. Journal of
Marketing Management 19 (1-2): 5-24.
Ganesh, G. K., P. R. Chandy and G. V. Henderson, 1990. Awareness and Evaluation of Selected
Marketing Journals Inside and Outside the Discipline: An Empirical Study. Akron Business and
Economic Review 21 (4): 93-106.
Gross, A. G., J. E. Harmon and M. Reidy, 2002. Communicating Science: The Scientific Article from the
17th Century to the Present. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Henerson, M. E., L. L. Morris and C. T. Fitz-Gibbon, 1987. How to Measure Attitudes. Newbury Park,
CA: SAGE Publications Inc.
82
Homburg, C. 2003. Publishing Processes in the Academic Marketing Discipline in the United States: A
German Perspective. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 31 (3): 348.
Hubbard, R. 1994. The Dangers of Generalizing from Published Marketing Studies. Journal of the
Market Research Society 36 (July): 257-261.
Hult, G. T. M., W. T. Neese and R. E. Bashaw, 1997. Faculty Perceptions of Marketing Journals. Journal
of Marketing Education 19 (Spring): 37-52.
IJRM Online. 2004. Elsevier Ltd. Retrieved 20 September, 2004, from
http://authors.elsevier.com/JournalDetail.html?PubID=505550&Precis=DESC.
JA Online. 2004. American Academy of Advertising. Retrieved 20 September, 2004, from
http://www.bus.iastate.edu/JoA/.
JAMS Online. 2004. SAGE Publications Ltd. Retrieved 20 September, 2004, from
http://www.sagepub.co.uk/journal.aspx?pid=105692.
JAR Online. 2004. Cambridge University Press. Retrieved 15 December, 2004, from
http://uk.cambridge.org/journals/journal_catalogue.asp?mnemonic=jar.
JBR Online.2004. Elsevier. Retrieved 20 September, 2004, from
http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/505722/description#description.
JCR Online. 2004. University of Chicago Press - Journals Division. Retrieved 20 September, 2004, from
http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/JCR/home.html.
Jensen, T. D., J. Olson and M. Landry, 1995. Publication and Social Exchange: Trends in Four Major
Marketing Journals. Paper presented at the World Marketing Congress, July 6-10, 1995 (pp.
13/79-13/86). Melbourne, Australia.
JM Online. 2004. American Marketing Academy. Retrieved 20 September, 2004, from
http://www.marketingpower.com/live/content1053C362.php.
JMR Online. 2004. American Marketing Academy. Retrieved 20 September, 2004, from
http://www.marketingpower.com/content1054.php
JR Online. 2004. Elsevier Ltd. Retrieved 20 September, 2004, from
http://authors.elsevier.com/JournalDetail.html?PubID=620186&Precis=DESC.
Kerr, S., J. Tolliver and D. Petree, 1977. Manuscript Characteristics Which Influence Acceptance for
Management and Social Science Journals. Academy of Management Journal 20 (1): 132-141.
Klein, D. B. and E. Chang, 2004. The Social Science Citation Index: A Black Box - with an Ideological
Bias. Econ Journal Watch 1 (1): 134-165.
Koojarenprasit, N., A. Weinstein, W. C. Johnson and D. O. Remington, 1998. Marketing Journal
Rankings Revisited: Research Findings and Academic Implications. Marketing Education Review
8 (Spring): 95-102.
Kuhn, T. S. 1970. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
83
McNamee, C. J. and C. L. Willis, 1994. Stratification in Science: A Comparison of Publication Patterns
in Four Disciplines. Knowledge: Creation, Diffusion, Utilization 15 (4): 396-416.
Mort, G. S., J. R. McColl-Kennedy, G. Kiel and G. N. Soutar, 2004. Perceptions of Marketing Journals by
Senior Academics in Australia and New Zealand. Australasian Marketing Journal 12 (2): 51-61.
Moxley, J. M. 1992. Publish Don't Perish: The Scholars Guide to Academic Writing and Publishing.
Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
MS Journal Homepage. 2004, 13 September 2004. University of Florida. Retrieved 20 September, 2004,
from http://bear.cba.ufl.edu/centers/MKS/index.asp.
Pecotich, A. and J. E. Everett, 1989. An Extension of the Citation Analysis of Selected Marketing
Journals. International Journal of Research in Marketing 6 (3): 199-205.
Pecotich, A., M. Pressly and D. Roth, 1996. The Impact of Country of Origin in the Retail Service
Context. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 3 (4): 213-224.
Perry, C., D. Carson and A. Gilmore, 2003. Joining a Conversation: Writing for EJM's Editors, Reviewers
and Readers Requires Planning, Care and Persistence. European Journal of Marketing 37 (5/6):
652-667.
Peter, J. P. and J. C. Olson, 1983. Is Science Marketing? Journal of Marketing 47 (Fall): 111-125.
Robinson, L. M. and R. Adler, 1981. Measuring the Impact of Marketing Scholars and Institutions: An
Analysis of Citation Frequency. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 9 (2): 147-162.
Rotfeld, H. J. (1997a). Contribution Calculating and Credit Counting. Marketing Educator 16 (Spring): 2.
Rotfeld, H. J. (1997b). We Unequivocally Do Not Thank the @#$%*&^ Anonymous Reviewers.
Marketing Educator 16 (Fall): 6.
Sawyer, A. G. 1988. How to Write a Painful Marketing Manuscript: Prose and Cons. Journal of
Marketing Education 10 (3): 49-54.
Theoharakis, V. and A. Hirst, (2002). Perceptual Differences of Marketing Journals: A Worldwide
Perspective. Marketing Letters 13 (4): 389-402.
Twedt, D. W. 1977. A Marketing Strategy for Marketing Knowledge - or How to Publish and Prosper.
Journal of Marketing 41 (2): 69-72.
Urban, D. J., J. P. Wayland and D. R. McDermott, 1992. An Empirical Investigation of Research
Standards for Marketing Faculty at AACSB-Accredited Business Schools. Journal of Marketing
Education 14 (2): 53-68.
Weiers, R. M. 1988. Marketing Research. Englewood Cliffs, N J.: Prentice Hall.
Weiner, G. 1998. Scholarship, Disciplinary Hegemony and Power in Academic Publishing. Proceedings
of the European Conference for Educational Research, 17-20 September 1998. Retrieved 20
April, 2004, from http://www.leeds.ac.uk/educol/documents/000000795.html
84
Public Environmental Consumerism:
Recycling Behaviour and its Impact
on Intentions to Purchase Green Products
Clare D’Souza, La Trobe University
Mehdi Taghian, Deakin Business School
Peter Lamb, La Trobe University Albury Wodonga
Abstract
Recycling behaviour has been used as indicators of green behavior since research has indicated
that there is a positive relationship between environmental concern and ecologically responsible
behavior such as recycling. The objective of this study is to find out if there is a difference between
recyclers and non-recyclers and heavy and light recyclers with respect to their willingness to pay more
for green products despite the perception of green products being of a lower quality in comparison to
alternative products. The answers to this will help us to better understand environmentally conscious
consumerism of both groups of consumers i.e. the light and heavy recyclers. The results indicate that
willingness to pay somewhat higher prices and accepting somewhat lower quality of green products
appears to be, generally, independent of recycling and non-recycling, as well as heavy and light recycling
behaviour. It also addresses the implications of these findings for business managers.
Introduction
Household waste has become a critical issue in society and governments have implemented policy
measures to curb wastes by way of educating consumers to recycle and encouraging environmental
consumerism. On the other hand, businesses are also doing their best to contain environmental pollution
by producing products that are environmentally friendly to support environmental consumerism.
Environmental consumerism (green buying) is a type of environmentally conscious behavior, whereby
consumers purchase and consume products that are safe or friendly towards the environment (Mainieri,
Barnett, Valdero, Unipan, Oskamp 1997). Such examples would commonly include household items
manufactured with recyclable or reusable packaging or those containing environmental safe ingredients
such as phosphate free/biodegradable detergents.
The purpose of this paper is to report on the results of a study conducted to segment consumers based on
pro environmental behaviour, such as recycling, and to assess the different ways they view price and
quality attributes of green products. Since consumers make product choices, in part, based on price and
quality, these two issues can become critical in consumer’s intentions to purchase green products.
Since the 1990s there has been widespread growing public awareness of environmental degradation, with
marketing being blamed for the increase in consumption and household waste. This attention had been
magnified to an extent that environmentalism had been identified as potentially the ‘biggest business issue
of the 1990s’ (Kirkpatrick, 1990). Since then the green agenda was pursued in a number of diverse
considerations. From the marketing perspective, many businesses have directed organisational efforts
towards designing, promoting, pricing and distributing products that are environmentally friendly (Pride
and Ferrell, 1993).
85
At a macro level, the use of green products can make an important contribution to the safety of the
environment by helping to reduce environmental degradation. It is, therefore, helpful to investigate how
consumers evaluate green products. There has been research suggesting that consumers appear to be not
tolerant of lower quality and higher prices (D’Souza, Taghian and Lamb, 2004) but how does this affect
highly involved green consumers (those that are highly environmentally concerned) in comparison to
those that are not so involved? It is helpful to distinguish these market segments for a number of reasons.
Firstly, the highly involved green consumers are, by definition, the existing primary target market for
green product producers. Secondly, are these highly involved consumers prepared to trade off price and
quality for the sake of environmentally friendly products? Thirdly, if the highly involved consumers are
not prepared to compromise on price and quality, then managers should be thinking about strategic and
innovative ways to improve product quality and to introduce efficiency in production processes to reduce
costs and thereby prices and to communicate these aspects to the consumer.
ENVIRONMENTAL BEHAVIOUR AND SEGMENTATION
Past studies of environmental consumerism have concentrated on measuring the personal characteristics
of consumers with respect to social responsibility (e.g., Webster 1977). There is evidence to suggest that
environmental concern is related to various personal and behavioral characteristics (Van Liere and
Dunlap 1980), such as recycling, which is noted to be indicative of pro-environmental concerns (e.g.,
McGuinness, Jones and Cole 1977). In fact on some occasions, supermarket managers have reported that
sales of environmentally friendly products appear to be somewhat stronger in neighbourhoods with
recycling programs (Carson 1996). Additionally, the literature has also highlighted that modification of
purchase habits and adoption of recycling behavior has been used by researchers as being indicators of
consumers’ green behavior (Henion 1972; Kinnear, Taylor and Ahmed 1974).
There appears to be an understanding that recycling is an environmental behaviour and may be interpreted
as a personal characteristic of socially responsible consumers. Dunlap (1991) identified two types of
environmental behavior: those that emphasize individual responsibility and those that indicate political
action. Individual responsibility refers to ecologically related consumer choices, recycling, and energysaving behaviours, whereas, political behaviour can involve consumer boycotts and, contributing money
to, or volunteering for, environmental institutions.
Notably, individual responsibility type of environmental behavior has been widely researched (Dunlap
and Scarce, 1991; Dunlap, 1991). Previous studies of recycling behavior of Americans have found that
46% recycled bottles or cans and 26% recycled newspapers (Roper Organization, 1990). This survey
also found that environmentally conscious consumers who purchased products made from recycled or
biodegradable material and those that avoided excess packaging, ranged from 7% to 25% of the
respondents. Considering the magnitude of concerns expressed by the consumers and the political and
social emphasis given to preserving the environment these results may appear to be relatively poor,
considering that these results were obtained at the time when the green movement was gathering
momentum. In comparison, the Australian Bureau of Statistics (2002) states that household recycling
increased to 85% in the nineties and by 2000 this had risen to 97%. Most of the recycled items consisted
of paper, old clothing, plastic bags and glass.
It appears that encouragement to recycle has come from both businesses and governments. It has been
indicated that in Australia people in the state of Victoria are strong recyclers, with 92% of households
having access to curb side recycling and around 87% actively recycle (Public Views Victoria, 2001). Ecorecycle, research suggests that on average, each household in Victoria puts out about 21 kg of recyclable
materials in the year, which is up 14 kg from 2001-2002 (Eco-recycle Victoria, 2003).
86
In addition, to support mechanisms for consumers to recycle, there are also contextual factors that need to
be considered as predictors of recycling behavior (Oskamp et al. 1991). These factors include
convenience of behavior, knowledge of environmental issues, family composition, the influence of
neighbors' expectations and behavior, sense of personal efficacy, and degree of intrinsic satisfaction
associated with the behavior. This also suggests that those who recycle would vary in their behaviors. For
instance, those consumers that recycle regularly can be differentiated as heavy recyclers and those that
recycle occasionally or seldom as light recyclers. This differentiation can also lend itself to the number of
persons in a household. For example, the ABS (2002) estimated that recycling behavior in Australia is
different for different types of households. It has been indicated (ABS 2002) that one person households
recycle relatively less than other types of households. A prominent reason provided by 93% of Australian
households that did not fully recycle was because of less recyclable materials in their waste. Another
common reason was the absence of recycling facilities.
In general, 9% of Australian consumers rated environmental problems as the most important social issue
(ABS, 2002). Other studies have also investigated customers’ general environmental attitudes (e.g.,
Dunlap and Van Liere, 1978; Weigel and Weigel 1978). There appears to be evidence to suggest that
there is a positive relationship between environmental concerns and ecological responsible behaviour
such recycling (Arbuthnot and Lingg, 1975; Kellgren and Wood, 1986; Simmons and Widmar 1990).
To be a green consumer, it may be argued, “individuals require an understanding of the consequences of
their [green purchase] behaviours" (Bohlen et al., 1993, p. 417). There have been numerous consumer
based studies that have examined psychological correlates of environmental concern and environmental
behaviours, indicating that an internal locus of control is positively correlated with intent to purchase
ecologically packaged products (Schwepker and Cornwell, 1991).
Similarly, the intention to purchase green products is also positively correlated for post purchase
behaviours such as recycling (Shrum, Lowrey, and McCarty, 1994). This suggests that those consumers
who recycle may have a higher tendency and motivation to buy green products. In other words, if
recycling can be used to predict general environmental buying behaviors, then those consumers involved
in recycling behavior can be segmented accordingly.
More specifically, we are interested in whether recycling behaviour can be meaningfully used as a market
segmentation base. It has been indicated that regular recycling behavior reflects customers’ strong
environmental attitude, which, potentially, may be translated into, and is an indication of, their conscious
green purchase patterns (Oskamp 1995), while other studies support recycling as an environmental
behavior. There is strong evidence suggesting that consumers that demonstrate high level of
environmentalism behaviour will also be proactive recyclers. For instance, there is correlational evidence
to suggests that those who recycle are also more likely to indulge in other environmental behaviors, even
if the behaviors require larger commitment (such as using your own shopping bag) (Berger, 1997).
Howenstein (1993) concluded that provided there is strong motivation from consumers, there is certainly
considerable recycling potential in almost all-demographic groups. Vining and Ebreo (1992, 1994)
indicate that recyclers' behavior was motivated more by concern about the environment than by financial
incentives or other rewards. Supporting Vining and Ebreo (1992, 1994), the research work of Simmons
and Widmar (1990) found that recyclers were more likely to believe in environmental conservation and
personal responsibility for the condition of the environment but if it lacked knowledge about recycling
then positive attitudes may not result in recycling. Thus, while recycling can be established with all
demographic groups, environmental awareness and a sense of personal responsibility for the environment
are critical to a successful recycling program.
The findings by Mainieri et al. (1997) indicate that the number of materials that people recycled assisted
to predict their general environmental buying behaviors. Their results suggest the possibility that,
87
consumers become more alert through their conscious efforts to recycle, and because of this their
awareness levels increase towards the environmental impact of the materials and packaging they buy. In
addition, the existing literature reflects that recycling activities demonstrate more positive relationships
with those purchasing decisions that require extended time and effort (Schlegelmilch; Bohlen; and
Diamantopoulos 1996). Therefore, advocating recycling appears to be an indication of advocating pro
environmental behaviour. Based on the foregoing analysis pro environmental behaviour such as recycling
can profile a consumer into various categories of greenness. Consequently, as we are more interested in
factors that contribute to general levels of environmental consumerism, this post purchase behaviour of
recycling provides a common ground for ecological segmentation. Hence, recycling behavior for the
purpose of this research was determined by two critical variables based on the frequency of recycling and
the category of materials recycled by consumers on a regular and occasional basis, which characterise the
heavy and light recyclers respectively.
PRICE AND QUALITY ISSUES:
Although, there are other factors that can be considered as being important determinants of consumer
product preferences, price and quality are, arguably, powerful influences on consumers’ decision in
evaluating alternatives in the process of product selection.
Whilst quality and price, generally, contribute to the value that consumer seeks in purchasing a product
(Adcock 2000), the purchase of green products represents a dual value systems. The benefits that the
consumer receives in satisfying their needs/wants are complemented by the implications that this
consumption may have with respect to the environment (Adcock 2000). If a green product has higher
quality, then the consumer purchasing this product could also use this superior ‘quality’ as an attractive
attribute in their process of evaluating alternative product choices.
There is evidence to suggest that green consumers are price-sensitive in purchasing green products
(Mandese 1991). D’Astous et al. (1997) investigated the association between perceived purchase value of
a green product and consumers’ intention to buy. Their findings indicated that ecological features of a
product had a significant relationship to both purchase value and buying intentions and while ecological
features enhanced the desirability of the product, the essential choice criterion was product efficacy.
Therefore, performance and the overall value remain the critical buying considerations (Berger 1993).
Even when researchers have explicitly considered consumers’ actual purchase of green products,
customers preferences involved factors beyond the ecological status of the brands examined (Henion
1972). It may be, therefore, suggested that while ecological advantages of a product may positively
contribute towards strengthening consumers’ purchase intention, they would, arguably be considered as
additional attractions rather than determining factors.
Additionally, while there appears to be claims of concerns for the environment by some consumers those
concerns may not necessarily translate into recycling behavior or buying green products. For instance, one
study revealed that 74% of respondents supported environmental protection, even if it meant slower
economic growth, but few of those respondents actually engaged in recycling behaviours (46% recycled
bottles and cans; and 26% recycled newspapers). On the other hand, three fourths of those surveyed
claimed that they would pay more for "green" grocery products, but only 14% regularly bought goods
made from or packaged in recycled materials (Hume 1991). At the same time, it was found that
consumers were willing to to pay more for ecologically friendly products (Myburgh-Louw &
O'Shaughnessy 1994), and regarded environmental issues when shopping (e.g., checking if the product is
produced from recycled materials), and bought more green compatible products (e.g., biodegradable
diapers) (Laroche, Tomiuk, Bergeron, and Barbaro-Forleo 2002). This may, arguably, be broadly
interpreted as evidence that there are some consumers who are willing to support the environment by lip
service but not willing to act, whilst others are prepared to act and even go to the extent of paying more
88
for environmentally friendly products. It is, therefore, important to investigate how consumers that
recycle would react to paying premium prices for green products and would they compromise with
respect to quality standards.
HYPOTHESES
Based on the extant literature it can be conceptualised that those customers who engage in environmental
behavior such as recycling are more likely to engage in other types of green behavior such as green
purchases. If this is so, it would be interesting to investigate the degree to which consumers would
support the purchase of green products if they were somewhat more expensive and/or lower in quality in
comparison to non-green product. We therefore hypothesise that:
H1. There is a difference between recyclers and non-recyclers with respect to the
purchase of green products even if they were somewhat more expensive in
comparison to other alternatives
H2. There is a difference between recyclers and non-recyclers with respect
to the purchase of green products even if they were somewhat lower in
quality in comparison to other alternatives.
H3. There is a difference between the heavy and light recyclers with respect
to their purchase intention of green products if they were more expensive in
comparison to other products.
H4 There is a difference between the heavy and light recyclers with respect to
their purchase intention of green products if they were somewhat lower in
quality in comparison to other products.
METHODOLOGY:
In order to test the hypotheses a descriptive research was designed and implemented. The unit of analysis
was determined as the main buyer of food from supermarkets in the household irrespective of gender. It
was considered that the person responsible for buying from supermarkets would, potentially, represent the
perception of the other family members about green products in their purchase behavior. The sampling
frame used was the telephone directory of the state of Victoria (Australia) including both metropolitan
and regional areas. The data were collected using a structured questionnaire administered on the phone
over a weekend using Quantum Research (CATI) data collection services. A total of 155 questionnaires
were completed and were used for data analysis. The sample selection was based on random telephone
dialling and is expected to be representative of the supermarket shoppers’ population.
The research instrument used in this study was structured based on prior qualitative research and the
literature review. It was pre-tested and modified to include variables contributing to the customers’
information processing on seven different dimensions contributing to the formation of the overall
respondents’ perception about green products, recycling behaviour, and their purchase intentions. The
information provided the possibility of segmenting respondents into heavy and light recyclers based on
frequency of recycling and the types of products recycled. All measurements were subjective assessments
by the respondents using a seven-point Likert-type scale (Wrenn 1997).
The data were analyzed using descriptive measures, t-tests, and Chi-square analysis to compare the means
and assess the associations of purchase intentions with respect of all recyclers and non- recyclers as well
as heavy and light recyclers of different recyclable product categories.
89
RESULTS AND DISCUSSIONS
The selected sample’s main characteristics were female (79%), 35-54 year olds (49%), married (65%),
with children (47%), secondary school educated (60%), and employed (50%).
The commonly recycled materials such as newspapers, metal /aluminum cans, glass, plastic and
cardboard were classified into three categories of regularly, occasionally, and never. The regular recyclers
were characterised as heavy recyclers and occasional recyclers were characterised as light recyclers. The
sample studied showed that most respondents recycled products in some form or the other. The recycle
behavior appears to be a popular activity with respondents ranging from 76% for metal and aluminium
cans to 91% for glass and plastic. The recyclers in each product category falling into the “occasional” and
“never” categories were relatively small (Table 1). Those respondents that answered “never” to some
items such as cans and newspapers did recycle other items.
Table 1: Recycling by categories of recyclable products (multiple response)
Product
categories
Glass
Plastic
Newspaper
Cardboard
Metal
Number of
respondents
155
155
155
155
155
Regularly
%
91
91
88
78
76
Occasionally
%
5
4
5
11
14
Never
%
4
5
7
11
10
This high level of heavy (regular) recycling behavior may be attributed to the overall awareness of
consumers of environmental issues resulting in their concern and support for the minimization of waste. It
may also reflect the initiatives of the local governments in the state of Victoria (Australia) by providing
special collection recycling bins and weekly free collection from households. Consequently, recycling
requires no specific effort on the part of the consumer except for segmenting the recyclable materials in
different bins and placing the bins outside on specific weekdays for collection. Indeed given the facilities
provided it is concerning that still some customers do not recycle all products.
The t-test analyses indicated that there were no differences between the recycling behaviour (including all
recyclers) and non-recyclers with respect to different classification of recyclable materials (newspapers,
metal/aluminium cans, glass, plastic, and cardboard) and their intention to purchase green products if they
were somewhat more expensive and somewhat lower in quality with the exception of glass (t(149) = 3.32,
p <.01), and plastic (t(149) = 2.70, p <.01) in relation to purchase intention if more expensive. Therefore,
there was only partial support for H1 and H2.
Additionally, t-tests were conducted to measure if there were differences between the heavy (regular) and
light (occasional) recyclers of different product classification (newspapers, metal/aluminum cans, glass,
plastic, and cardboard) in terms of their intentions to purchase green products if they were somewhat
more expensive and/or somewhat lower in quality. The results indicated the heavy and light recyclers
only differed in terms of glass (t(141) = 2.51, p <.05), and plastic (t(141) = 2.51, p <.05). Therefore, H3
and H4 were also partially supported.
90
The results also indicated that the only association existing between recyclers and purchase intention was
in relation to plastic recyclers, who would also be willing to pay somewhat higher prices, for green
products χ 2 (1) = 4.53, p <.05.
The overall conclusion, therefore, is that willingness to pay somewhat higher prices and accepting
somewhat lower quality of green products appears to be, generally, independent of recycling and nonrecycling as well as heavy and light recycling behavior. This conclusion reconfirms the previous findings
that the current customers’ overall perception of green products is such that they will refrain from any
compromise on price or quality. Additionally, as reported by D’Souza, Taghian, and Lamb (2004)
customers’ expectation appears to be that all products need to be environmentally safe regardless of the
additional cost of compliance by manufacturers and that corporate strategies need to support and react
favorably to this expectation.
IMPLICATIONS
The implications of this research are two-fold. Firstly, recyclers and non-recyclers do not appear to be,
generally different in terms of their price and quality expectations, except for glass and plastic recyclers in
relation to somewhat higher prices, therefore, recyclers are unlikely to compromise on the overall product
value. At the same time, there is no difference between heavy and light recyclers on price and quality
expectation either, again with the exception of glass and plastic recyclers in relation to somewhat higher
prices. Consequently, there is no evidence to support that heavy recyclers would be more compromising
in terms of the overall product value for the sake of support for the environment. The other implication is
that businesses need to recognise that while green products, arguably, present a competitive advantage
they can only do so if prices and product quality are at the level of the alternative products.
The results demonstrate that at the market level price and quality play a very important role as
antecedents to green purchase behavior. These have direct implications for marketers intending to
introduce new green products or to reposition their current products to project green status. Those
managers that seek to increase market penetration of these products would be advised to consider making
their prices competitive and refrain from premium pricing strategy. In addition to quality issues,
marketers need to attempt to compete on similar bases as main stream products and should not
compromise on quality issues.
Most crucial of all is the environmental ethic of recycling. Participation in recycling is on the rise in
Australia, thus the issues of environmental impact have gained importance in the areas of research,
particularly as they relate to consumer behaviour.
Recycling not only saves natural resources but saves energy in manufacturing, due to decreases in
emissions, recycling reduces the green house effect and most of all it provides opportunities for business
in terms of changing their packaging materials and formats.
The findings of this investigation, potentially, have implications at a broader level. Those that recycled
were asked if they recycled newspapers, metal /aluminum cans, glass, plastic and cardboard. With the
exception of newspapers, other items, to a large extent, are used in the manufacturing of green products or
are being used in some shape or form as packaging materials. Interestingly, about 46% of respondents did
not agree that the word recycle needs to be used on packaging, as those respondents, normally, knew what
to recycle. Whist the other 54% did not care if the word recycling was used on the packaging as these
respondents recycled the above items anyway. There is some evidence to suggest that business strategy
needs to be aimed at focusing efforts on producing products using materials that can be recycled and
allowing for, where possible, packaging material that can be recycled.
91
In addition, refundable deposits should be made more clearly pronounced on packaging as refundable
deposit for the purchase of beverages in bottles or cans is, potentially, an attractive monetary incentive in
recycling, as it may contribute toward the price of future purchases. In other countries, the monetary
incentive of a refund has been responsible for massive increases in the percentage of returned containers,
to levels as high as 90%, thus considerably reducing the amount of roadside litter (Shireman, 1993).
In sum, we believe that potential for green markets exist if products are developed by adding superior
value, as one of the factors contributing to the failure of the environmental marketing movement can be
attributed to consumers’ higher prices and lower quality perceptions of green products.
CONCLUSIONS AND LIMITATIONS
This study was designed to accomplish several purposes. First, we attempted to segment the market
according to pro environmental behaviour such as recycling. Our research limited itself to only
behavioural-based segmentation. Further research needs to be directed toward studying the effect of other
segmentation variables. Recycling here was used as a context to study environmentally responsible
behaviors and intentions to buy green products with respect to price and quality issues. Our research
limited itself to only two product attributes, although, customer purchase intentions can be influenced by
a number of other moderating and mediating factors. Further research is recommended to include other
identifying green attributes that consumers use to make green purchases. If tested against other product
selection criteria, for example, product performance or convenience, there could be some level of
variation in these results.
The use of demographics and other socio-economic variables are suggested to identify which variables
are most effective in market segmentation bases for green products. Researchers interested in the
investigation of a more realistic results need to explore consumers’ recycling behaviour using actual
purchases rather than intentions to purchase behaviors. The strength of customers’ attitude with respect of
their expectation that all products should be environmentally safe may, arguably, suggest a backlash in
demand as customers may negatively react to non-green products in the presence of available, equally
priced and of acceptable quality green products.
Marketing’s impact on society has been detrimental to the environment over the last decade, with an
increase in solid wastes. Handling the vast amount of solid waste, particularly in developed countries, the
so-called ‘throw away society’, is, arguably, of some threat to a sustainable environment in the future.
In addition to support from local governments for promoting recycling, there are other institutions
assisting in developing strategies, such as the Ecorecycle (2003), to help Victorians move toward a more
sustainable production and consumption as a part of the development of the ‘towards zero waste’ strategy
and the South Australia’s Container Deposit Legislation that offers recycles monetary incentive for most
glass and plastic containers. Whilst consumers increased their desire for a better environment with a
willingness to alter their behaviour and purchasing habits (Davis, 1993), marketing efforts may have
fallen somewhat short. Since environmentalism is one of Australia’s dominant issues, solutions must be
found to remedy these potential problems. The foregoing discussion found some indication to help reduce
this problem, rather than experience marketing success with traditional products. Green products with
appropriate positioning can help to support environmental goals and can assist in providing a sustainable
future.
The findings of this study need to be considered cautiously allowing for the relatively small sample size.
Additionally, the findings are from the study of consumers in one state of Australia and may not
necessarily represent the characteristics of consumers in other parts of the country. Further research could
increase the sample size to the level that can facilitate the assessment of environmental consumerism by
92
sub-cultures and ethnic groups within the Australian population as well as assessment by demographic
factors and geographic locations.
REFERENCES:
Adcock, D. 2000. Marketing Strategies for Competitive Advantage. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley
and Sons Ltd.
Arbuthnot, J. and Lingg, S. 1975. A comparison of French and American Environmental
Behaviors, Knowledge, and Attitudes. International Journal of Psychology 10: 275-281.
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2002). Environment, Environmental Attitudes and
Behaviour in Australian Households Year Book
Berger, I. 1993. The Relationship between Environmental Attitudes and Behavior.
Canadian Journal of Marketing Research 12: 36-43.
Berger, I. 1997. The Demographics of Recycling and the Structure of Environmental
Behavior. Environment and Behaviour 29(4): 515-522.
Bohlen, G.M., Schlegelmilch, B.B. and Diamantopoulos, A. 1993. Measuring Ecological
Concern: A Multi-Construct Perspective. Journal of Marketing Management 9(4):
415-430.
Carson, P. 1996. The Greenbox Program. Presentation given at Queen's University. Cited in
Berger, I. 1997. The Demographics of Recycling and the Structure of Environmental
Behavior. Environment and Behavior 29(4): 515-522.
D’Astous, A., Sadrudin, Valence, Gilles and Francois Houde. 1997. Proceedings of the
European Marketing Academy Conference, University of Warwick, 287-301.
D’Souza, Clare, Mehdi, Taghian, and Peter Lamb. 2004. A Review of the Green
Customers’ Perception Formation about Environmentally Safe Products. In Wiley, James (Ed.),
Proceedings of the ANZMAC 2004 Conference (Full Paper Published on CD-ROM).
Wellington,
New Zealand, School of Marketing and International Business, Victoria University of Wellington, 29
November – 1 December, No page numbers.
Davis, J. 1993. Strategies for Environmental Advertising, Journal of Consumer Marketing
10(2); 19-36.
Dunlap, R.E. 1991. Public Opinion in the 1980's: Clear Consensus, Ambiguous Commitment.
Environment 33(8): 9-37.
Dunlap, R.E. and R. Scarce. 1991. The Polls - Poll Trends: Environmental Problems and
Protection. Public Opinion Quarterly 55(4): 651-672.
Dunlap, R. E., and Van Liere, K. D. 1978. The new Environmental Paradigm. Journal of
Environmental Education 9: 10-19.
Ecorecycle, 2003. Towards Zero Waste. [cited 18 March 2005]. Available from
www.econrecycle,vic.gov.
93
Henion, K. 1972. The Effect of Ecologically Relevant Information on Detergent Sales.
Journal of Marketing Research 9:10-14.
Howenstein, E. 1993. Market Segmentation for Recycling. Environment and Behavior 25,
86-102.
Hume, S. 1991. Consumer Doubletalk Makes Companies Wary. Advertising Age 62(46):
GR4.
Kinnear, T., Taylor, J., and Ahmed, S. A. 1974. Ecologically Concerned Consumers: Who
are They? Journal of Marketing 38 (April): 20-24.
Kirkpatrick, D. 1990. Environmentalism: The New Crusade, Fortune 121, (February): 4451.
Kellgren, C. A., and Wood, W. 1986. Access to Attitude-Relevant Information in Memory
as a Determinant of Attitude-Behavior Consistency. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology 22, 328-338.
Laroche, M. Tomiuk, M., Bergeron, J and Barbaro-Forleo, G. 2002. Cultural Differences in
Environmental Knowledge, Attitudes and Behaviours of Canadian Consumers, Canadian
Journal of Administrative Sciences 19(3): 267-284.
McGuinness, J., Jones, A. P., & Cole, S. G.1977. Altitudinal Correlates of Recycling
Behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology 62: 376-384.
Mandese, J. 1991. New Study Finds Green Confusion. Advertising Age 62(45): 1-56.
Mainieri, T., Barnett, E., Valdero, T., Unipan, J., Oskamp, S. 1997. Green Buying: The
Influence of Environmental Concern on Consumer Behavior. The Journal of Social
Psychology 137(2):189-205.
Myburgh-Louw, J. and O'Shaughnessy, N.J. 1994. Consumer Perception of Misleading and
Deceptive Claims on the Packaging of "Green" Fast Moving Consumer Goods. In R.
Achrol & A. Mitchell (Eds.), AMA Summer Educators' Conference Proceedings.Chicago:
American Marketing Association 344-353.
Oskamp, S. 1995. Resource Conservation and Recycling: Behavior and Policy. Journal of
Social Issues 51(4): 157-178.
Oskamp, S., M. Harrington, T. Edwards, D. Sherwood, S. Okuda and D. Swanson. 1991.
Factors Influencing Household Recycling Behavior. Environment and Behavior 23(4): 494519.
Pride, W. M. and Ferrell, O. C. 1993. Marketing 8th ed., Houghton Mifflin, Boston, M.A.
Public Views Victoria. 2001. Community Attitudes to Waste and Recycling, Report.
[cited 18 March 2005]. Available from www.ecorecycle.vic.au.
Roper Organization. 1990. The Environment: Public Attitudes and Individual Behavior.
New York: The Roper Organization.
94
Schlegelmilch, B., Bohlen, G. and Diamantopoulos, A. 1996. The Link between Green
Purchasing Decision and Measures of Environmental Consciousness. European
Journal of Marketing 30(5): 35-56.
Schwepker, C.H. and Cornwell, T. B. 1991. An Examination of Ecologically Concerned
Consumers and their Intention to Purchase Ecologically Packaged Products, Journal of
Public Policy and Marketing 10(Fall): 77-101.
Shrum, L. J., Lowrey, T. M. and McCarty, J. 1994. Recycling as a Mark-Sting Problem:
A Framework for Strategy Development. Psychology and Marketing 11 (July/August);
393-416.
Shireman, W. K. 1993. Solid Waste: To Recycle or to Bury California. In T. Palmer (Ed.).
California's Threatened Environment: Restoring the Dream
Washington, DC: Island
Press, 170-181.
Simmons, D., & Widmar, R. 1990. Motivations and Barriers to Recycling: Toward a
Strategy for Public Education. The Journal of Environmental Education 22(1), 1318.
Van Liere, K. D., and Dunlap, R. E. 1980. The Social Bases of Environmental Concern: A
Review of Hypotheses, Explanation and Empirical Evidence. Public Opinion
Quarterly 44: 43-59.
Vining, J., and Ebreo, A. 1992. Predicting Recycling Behavior from Global and Specific
Environmental Attitudes and Changes in Recycling Opportunities. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology 20, 1580-1607.
Vining, J., and Ebreo, A. 1994. What Makes a Recycler? A Comparison of Recyclers and
Non-recyclers. Environment and Behavior 22(1), 55-73.
Webster, F. E., Jr. 1977. Determining the Characteristics of the Socially Conscious
Consumer. Journal of Consumer Research 2:188-196.
Weigel, R., and Weigel, J. 1978. Environmental Concern: The Development of a Measure.
Environment and Behavior 10: 3-15.
Wren, B. 1997. The Market Orientation Construct: Measurement and Scaling Issues. Journal
of marketing theory and practice 5 (3): 31-54.
95
Are negative reactions to sexist appeals in alcohol
advertisements a function of feminism or gender?
Sandra C. Jones, University of Wollongong
Abstract
Anecdotal evidence suggests that the use of sexual appeals in alcohol advertising is increasing. It has
been shown that the use of sex appeals may result in a more negative attitude towards the brand,
particularly among female consumers. This study investigates the proposition that this is the effect of
feminist ideology rather than, or in addition to, biological gender. The results show that female
respondents have more negative attitudes towards alcohol advertisements utilizing overt (or demeaning)
sexual appeals than males and more positive attitudes towards alcohol advertisements utilizing feminist
(empowering) appeals than males; and that there is no consistent independent effect of self-reported
feminism.
INTRODUCTION
Sex in advertising
Studies in the 1980s found that sexual images in advertising had become more overt over time (Soley &
Kurzbard 1986), and that models were wearing more suggestive clothing (Soley & Reid 1988). This
trend continued into the 1990s and beyond, with eroticism and nudity in magazine advertising becoming
more prevalent and increasingly blatant (see, for example, Reichert et al. 1999). Eroticism and nudity
have increased for both male and female models (Kolbe & Albanese 1996). A recent study of U.S.
primetime television commercials (Lin 1998) found that 12% of models were partially-dressed or nude
and that three-quarters of “sex-object” appeals used female models.
Reichert et al. (1999) compared magazine ads from 1983 and 1993, and found that in 1993 both female
and male models were more likely to be dressed in a sexually explicit manner. Nevertheless, in both 1983
and 1993, female models were three times as likely as male models to be dressed in a sexually explicit
manner – with 40% of female models in 1993 dressed provocatively. They also found that ads in 1993
were far more likely to display “intimate” or “very intimate” physical contact between male and female
models (53% of ads containing a female and a male model, compared to 21% in 1982), and that this type
of ad appeared largely in gendered (i.e., men’s or women’s) magazines rather than general interest
magazines.
Gender differences in responses to sex appeals
It has been shown that the use of sex appeals may result in a more negative attitude towards the brand
(Simpson, Horton, & Brown 1996) and lower purchase intention (LaTour & Henthorne 1994) than nonsex appeals.
However, there appear to be gender differences in response to sex appeals. One study that included male
and female respondents (LaTour et al. 1990) found that males reported a more positive response to ads
with nude models and females a more negative response, which, as the investigators pointed out, would
result in little net effect if the target audience contained equal numbers of males and females.
96
Sex appeals in alcohol advertising
Anecdotal evidence suggest that the use of sexual appeals in alcohol advertising is increasing, in Australia
and many other countries, although there are no empirical studies of the proportion of alcohol ads which
use sex appeals. A U.S. study found that beer and malt liquor advertisements were often connected with
sex and power (Moore et al. 1988). Given that these two products are predominantly marketed to men,
such appeals are consistent with this hypothesised biological and social conditioning.
In Australia, section 2.3 of the Australian Association of National Advertisers (AANA) Code of Ethics
specifies that advertisements shall “treat sex, sexuality and nudity with sensitivity to the relevant audience
and, where appropriate, the relevant program time zone.” Of the 419 complaints lodged with the
Advertising Standards Board (ASB) between 1999 and 2001 under this section, only three (0.7%) were
upheld (ASB 1999a; 1999b; 2000a; 2000b; 2001a; 2001b). Of the 33 complaints about alcohol
advertisements lodged during this period, 14 (42%) were under section 2.3 – none of these were upheld.
Attitudes to sex in alcohol advertising
An extensive search of the literature on advertising appeals failed to identify any studies specifically
relating to reactions to the use of sexual appeals in alcohol advertisements, although there are several
experimental studies on the impact of sexual appeals in women’s fragrance advertisements (e.g., Rossiter
& Jones 2003).
A recently-published Australian study (Polonsky et al. 2001) investigated whether ‘feminist’ women
(classified according to scores on the Bem Sex Role Inventory) were more critical of sexist (i.e., gender
stereotyped) appeals in ads for beer that ‘non-feminist’ women. The study concluded that, while the
women felt that the ads portrayed women in a “less than flattering” manner, this did not have a negative
impact on purchase intentions. Further, they found that the women did not wish the ads to be modified in
order to target female consumers. However, this study left a number of unanswered questions relating to:
the inclusion of only female participants; the use of only beer advertisements which, as reported, women
saw as targeted only at men; the use of the BSRI, which measures gender role identity, as a proxy
measure of feminism; and the use of ads which were sexist in their implied attitudes towards women
rather than their portrayal of women and which were fairly mild compared to many current campaigns.
This study was designed to investigate the independent, and interactive, effects of biological gender and
feminism on both purchase intentions and ad liking for alcohol products that use visuals portraying
women in an overtly sexual (demeaning) fashion as opposed to empowering or neutral portrayals.
HYPOTHESES
As discussed above, there appear to be gender differences in response to sex appeals. It is argued,
therefore, that women will find overtly sexual (demeaning) portrayals of women more offensive than will
men, thus:
H1
females will have more negative attitudes to overtly sexual alcohol
advertisements than will males
Similarly, if we accept the argument that men and women are subconsciously programmed as discussed
above, it is likely that men will react negatively towards advertisements which present women as
powerful, independent entities (particularly where the appeal may be taken to imply that men are not
necessary for a women to be happy and successful). Thus:
H2
females will have more positive attitudes to feminist (empowering) alcohol
advertisements than will males
97
Given the rationale behind H1 and H2, advertisements which present women neither as subservient
(sexual objects) nor as dominant (empowered) should not specifically appeal to, or against, genderspecific conditioning. Thus:
H3
females and males will not differ in their attitudes toward
neutral alcohol advertisements
Feminism is defined by the Oxford dictionary as “advocacy of women’s rights and sexual equality.” This
is a distinctly different construct to gender role identification; it is an ideological perspective, as opposed
to a self-identity. Feminists, by definition, should have more negative evaluations of messages which
portray women in a demeaning fashion (independent of biological gender). Thus:
H4
participants who are higher on feminism, regardless of biological gender, will
have more negative attitudes to overtly sexual alcohol advertisements than will
participants who are lower on political feminism
H5
participants who are higher on feminism, regardless of biological gender, will
have more positive attitudes to feminist alcohol advertisements than will
participants who are lower on political feminism
H6
high-feminism and low-feminism respondents will not differ in their attitudes
toward neutral alcohol advertisements
METHOD
Participants
The participants were 316 undergraduate college students taking a first-year introductory marketing
course. The mean age of the participants was 19.9 years (range 17 to 56). 59% of the participants were
female, and 74% were born in Australia.
Measures
Feminism was measured using 10 items from the Attitudes toward Women Scale (Spence & Helmreich
1978). This scale was designed to “assess attitudes towards the rights, roles, and privileges women ought
to have (Bailey et al. 1992; p.315); and includes items such as “women should assume their rightful place
in business and all the professions along with men” and “a woman should be as free as a man to propose
marriage.” Respondents answered each item on a 4-point Likert scale (“agree strongly,” “agree mildly,”
“disagree mildly,” or “disagree strongly”), and responses were then converted to a 0 – 4 scale.
Stimuli
The stimuli were 9 print advertisements for alcohol brands. The ads were selected from a group of thirtyfive magazine advertisements from current campaigns. The initial criterion for selection was that the ad
must include a picture of at least one woman (either on her own or with other men or women). This left a
group of 16 suitable advertisements which were pretested with a separate group of 10 males and females
to classify them as either ‘sexist’, ‘empowering’, or ‘neutral’.
The sexist ads:
Ad 3 – blended whisky. It consists of a photograph of a middle-aged man in a suit and a young girl in a
cropped top, mini skirt and sneakers in a lift. The caption reads "he who hesitates is lost".
98
Ad 5 – scotch whisky. It consists of a photograph of a woman, from the chest down, wearing a very short
skirt stepping out of a car. The caption reads "yes, God is a man".
Ad 8 – liqueur. It consists of a photograph of a woman reclining in a chair with one strap of her dress
fallen down, her dress pulled up to the top of her thighs, and her hand between her legs. The caption
reads "after a couple of glasses you may find you've taken advantage of yourself."
The empowering ads:
Ad 2 – sparkling wine. It consists of a photograph of a woman and a man in a bath (the focus of the
picture is the woman and only the man’s arms and part of his face are visible); the woman is laughing and
holding a glass of champagne, with champagne splashing out of the top of glass. The caption reads
“what's an occasion anyway?”
Ad 4 – wine. It consists of a photograph of a woman in a bar wearing a bright yellow dress, surrounded
by three men in suits who are clearly interested in her (the man's faces are only partially visible). The
caption reads “yellow stands out.”
Ad 7 – sparkling wine (the same brand as Ad 2). It consists of a photograph of three women laughing and
drinking champagne while trying to take a photograph of themselves. The caption reads “what's an
occasion anyway?"
The neutral ads:
Ad 1 – sparkling wine. It consists of a photograph of a man and a woman, arm-in-arm, walking away
from the camera and past an outdoor café; the man, who is carrying a bottle of the wine, is looking back
over his shoulder and is smiling. The caption reads “unmistakably Italian.”
Ad 6 – liqueur. It consists of a photograph of a woman looking directly at the camera, and two glasses
containing ice cubes, with liqueur being poured from the bottle into one of the glasses. The caption reads
“where fire meets ice.”
Ad 9 – liqueur. The ad shows three people, two women and a man, smiling and laughing; each is
holding a glass containing ice cubes and liqueur. The caption reads “you’ll always remember your first
[brand name].”
Study design
Participants were shown, in a classroom setting, each of the nine ads (in the order in which they are
numbered above) on an overhead projector. They were asked, for each ad, to rate their liking for the ad
(How much do you like or dislike this ad?) on an 11-point scale, from minus five (absolutely hate it) to
plus five (absolutely love it); and their purchase intention (How likely is it that you will buy this brand
next time you buy alcohol?) on a 10-point scale, from 0% (definitely won't) to 100% (definitely will).
They then completed an unrelated task (a questionnaire on health status and health behaviour). Finally,
they completed the above-described measures of gender role identification and feminism.
RESULTS
Attitude toward the ads
The mean ad liking scores for the ads are shown in Table 1. As can be seen from the table, the mean
scores for all of the ads were not far from the neutral point on the scale, with eight of the nine mildly
favourable.
99
Table 1: Mean ad liking scores (all respondents combined)
Mean
ad 1 (neutral)
-.07
ad 2 (empowering)
.86
ad 3 (sexist)
1.38
ad 4 (empowering)
1.08
ad 5 (sexist)
.62
ad 6 (neutral)
.97
ad 7 (empowering)
.84
ad 8 (sexist)
.01
ad 9 (neutral)
.40
SD
2.02
1.95
2.41
1.20
2.83
2.48
2.41
2.44
2.10
Attitude by Biological Gender
As shown in Table 2, female participants reported significantly lower levels of ad liking for all three of
the sexist ads than did male respondents (H1 supported).
Table 2: Ad liking by gender (sexist ads)
Male
Female
ad 3
2.10
0.89
ad 5
2.08
-0.39
ad 8
1.16
-.76
T
-4.71
-8.60
-7.58
Sig
.000
.000
.000
As shown in table 3, female participants reported significantly higher levels of ad liking for two of the
three empowering ads than did male respondents, with the third being directionally consistent (H2 largely
supported). Interestingly, ad 7 (the three girls drinking champagne without a man present) received the
highest mean rating of all the ads among female participants and the lowest mean rating of all the ads
among male participants.
Table 3: Ad liking by gender (empowering ads)
Male
Female
ad 2
.43
1.15
ad 4
.90
1.21
ad 7
-.57
1.8
T
3.14
1.37
9.76
Sig
.001
Ns
.000
As shown in table 4, there were no differences in ad liking between males and females for two of the
three neutral ads, with the third being preferred by females (H3 largely supported). Interestingly, Ad 6
(the one which was preferred by females) was the only ad in this category that pictured a woman without
a man.
Table 4: Ad liking by gender (neutral ads)
Male
Female
ad 1
-.02
-.12
ad 6
.64
1.22
ad 9
.19
.55
100
T
-.44
2.05
1.49
sig
ns
.04
ns
Attitude by Feminism Score
The mean score on the feminism scale was 22.55 (range 6 to 30, with a possible range of 0 to 30). Not
surprisingly, the mean feminism score was slightly higher for females (24.21) than for males (20.24).
Respondents were then dichotomised into either low feminism (score at or below the mean, n = 131) or
high feminism (score above the mean, n = 172).2
As shown in Table 5, high-feminism respondents reported:
significantly lower levels of ad liking for all three of the sexist ads than did low-feminism respondents
(H4 supported);
significantly higher levels of ad liking for only one of the three empowering ads than did low-feminism
respondents, with the other two ads being directionally consistent (H5 partly supported); and
no differences in ad liking from low-feminism respondents for any of the three neutral ads (H6
supported).
However, when these results were analysed by gender, there was no independent effect of feminism on ad
liking (i.e., all apparent ‘feminism’ differences were simply caused by biological gender differences).
Table 5: Ad liking by feminism (all ads)
Low feminism
ad 3 (sexist)
1.78
ad 5 (sexist)
1.31
ad 8 (sexist)
0.59
ad 2 (empowering)
0.76
ad 4 (empowering)
0.99
ad 7 (empowering)
0.41
ad 1 (neutral)
0.05
ad 6 (neutral)
0.92
ad 9 (neutral)
0.19
High feminism
1.22
0.15
-0.45
0.99
1.12
1.20
-0.05
1.03
0.56
T
2.09
3.62
3.72
-1.04
-5.38
-2.88
0.43
-0.37
-1.50
Sig
.04
.000
.000
ns
ns
.004
ns
ns
ns
Purchase intention
The mean purchase intention scores for the ads are shown in Table 6. As can be seen from the table, the
mean PI scores for the brands ranged from 16.6% (ad 3 – a blended whisky) to 31.7% (ad 7 – a sparkling
wine).
Table 6: Mean purchase intention scores (all respondents combined)
ad 3 (sexist) – blended whisky
ad 5 (sexist) – scotch whisky
ad 4 (empowering) – sparkling wine
ad 9 (neutral) – liqueur
ad 8 (sexist) – liqueur
ad 6 (neutral) – liqueur
ad 1 (neutral) – wine
ad 2 (empowering) – wine
ad 7 (empowering) – sparkling wine
2
Mean
16.55
21.70
23.49
23.84
24.21
25.26
26.56
26.60
31.72
SD
21.53
23.14
25.18
25.75
24.97
25.96
27.45
25.89
28.35
14 respondents could not be categorized as they did not complete all of the items on the feminism scale.
101
Purchase Intention by Biological Gender
As shown in Table 7, female respondents reported significantly higher purchase intentions than male
respondents for six of the nine alcohol brands (four wines and two liqueurs), and males reported
significantly higher purchase intentions for two (a scotch whisky and a liqueur).
Table 7: Mean purchase intention scores by gender
ad 3 (sexist) – blended whisky
ad 5 (sexist) – scotch whisky
ad 8 (sexist) – liqueur
ad 2 (empowering) – wine
ad 4 (empowering) – sparkling wine
ad 7 (empowering) – sparkling wine
ad 1 (neutral) – wine
ad 6 (neutral) – liqueur
ad 9 (neutral) – liqueur
Male
25.27
30.44
27.83
12.81
16.82
11.82
12.81
24.92
21.47
Female
23.54
18.72
21.21
27.94
33.57
36.92
19.19
36.66
28.01
t
-.600
-3.981
-2.183
6.248
6.189
9.606
2.593
3.679
2.200
sig
ns
.000
.03
.000
.000
.000
.01
.000
.03
Purchase Intention by Feminism Score
High-feminism respondents reported significantly higher purchase intentions that low-feminism
respondents for ad 4, sparkling wine (28.9 vs 22.8, t = -2.03, p = .04), and significantly lower purchase
intentions for ad 5 (sexist), scotch whisky (19.7 vs 28.8, t = 3.15, p = .002). However, when these results
were analysed by gender, there was no independent effect of feminism on purchase intention.
DISCUSSION
As hypothesized, we found that female respondents reported more negative attitudes towards alcohol
advertisements utilizing overt (or demeaning) sexual appeals than did males; more positive attitudes
towards alcohol advertisements utilizing feminist (empowering) appeals than did males; and did not differ
from males in their evaluation of advertisements utilizing neutral appeals.
As hypothesized, we also found that female respondents reported higher purchase intention for brands
utilizing feminist (empowering) appeals and lower purchase intention for brands utilizing overtly sexual
appeals. The former finding may be confounded by the fact that these products, particularly the two
sparkling wines, are more likely to be purchased by females regardless of advertising appeal (an argument
supported by the fact that female respondents also reported higher purchase intentions for the three brands
utilizing neutral appeals, which were also products predominantly consumed by women). However, the
finding in relation to overtly sexual appeals is more conclusive – of the only two brands for which
females reported significantly lower purchase intentions (both utilizing sexual appeals), one was for a
scotch whisky (targeted at male consumers) but the other was for a brand of liqueur which is
predominantly consumed by women, and it is evident from the wording in the ad that it is targeted
towards women. This finding is consistent with previous research on the use of overtly sexual appeals in
women’s fragrance advertising, which also found reductions in purchase intentions (Rossiter & Jones
2003).
We found no consistent effect of feminism on ad liking independent of biological gender. However, these
findings need to be interpreted with caution given the small differences between male and female
respondents on this variable as measured.
There is a need to future studies to investigate the independent, or interactive, effects or sex role
identification, feminism and gender; and to identify the reasons behind women’s dislike of apparently
sexist advertising.
102
REFERENCES
ASB. 1999a. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board, January-June 1999, Sydney:
Advertising Standards Bureau.
ASB. 1999b. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board, July-December 1999, Sydney:
Advertising Standards Bureau.
ASB. 2000a. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board, January-June 2000, Sydney:
Advertising Standards Bureau.
ASB. 2000b. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board, July-December 2000, Sydney:
Advertising Standards Bureau.
ASB. 2001a. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board, January-June 2001, Sydney:
Advertising Standards Bureau.
ASB. 2001b. Case Reports of the Advertising Standards Board, July-December 2001, Sydney:
Advertising Standards Bureau.
Kolbe, R., & Albanese, P.J. 1996. Man to man: A content analysis of sole-male images in maleaudience magazines. Journal of Advertising 35: 1-20.
LaTour, M.S., & Henthorne, T.L. 1994. Ethical judgments of sexual appeals in print advertising. Journal
of Advertising 23: 81-90.
LaTour, M.S., Pitts, R.E., & Snook-Luther, D.C. 1990. Female nudity, arousal, and ad response: An
experimental investigation. Journal of Advertising 194: 51-62.
Lin, C.A. 1998. Uses of sex appeals in prime-time television commercials. Sex Roles: A Journal of
Research 385: 461-473.
Moore, T., Taylor, D. et al. 1998 African-American youth perceptions of alcohol advertisements: a
preliminary analysis, Challenge: A Journal of Research on African American Men 92:
67-82.
Polonsky, M.J., Ford, J., Evans, K., Harman, A., Hogan, S., Shelley, L. & Tarjan, L. 2001 Are
feminists more critical of the portrayal of women in Australian beer advertisements than nonfeminists? Journal of Marketing Communications 7: 245-256.
Reichert, T., Lambiase, J., Morgan, S., Carstarphen, M., & Zavoina, S. 1999. Cheesecake and
beefcake: No matter how you slice it, sexual explicitness in advertising continues to
increase. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly 761: 7-20.
Rossiter, J.R. & Jones, S.C. 2003 Sex versus romance: effectiveness of women’s fragrance ads,
Proceedings of the Proceedings of the European Marketing Academy Conference,
Glasgow, Scotland, May 20-22, 2003.
Simpson, P.M., Horton, S., & Brown, G. 1996. Male nudity in advertisements: A modified
replication and extension of gender and product effects. Journal of the Academy of
Marketing Science 24: 257-262.
Soley, L., & Kurzbard, G. 1986. Sex in advertising: A comparison of 1964 and 1984 magazine
advertisements. Journal of Advertising 153: 45-54.
Soley, L.C., & Reid, L.N. 1988. Taking it off: Are models in magazine ads wearing less? Journalism
Quarterly 65: 960-966.
Spence, J.T. & Helmreich, R.L. 1978. Masculinity and femininity: Their psychological dimensions,
correlates, and antecedents. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
103
____________________________________________________
A Dynamic Approach to International Codes of Ethics
Göran Svensson, Halmstad University, Sweden
Dr Greg Wood, Deakin University, Australia
Introduction
Corporate codes of ethics are one of several influences on business ethics within and across cultures. In a
model that goes beyond philosophically-based ethics, Stajkovic and Luthans (1997) use social-cognitive
theory as a springboard to identify factors influencing business ethics standards and conduct. They
propose that a person’s perception of ethical standards and subsequent conduct is influenced by
institutional factors (e.g. ethics legislation), personal factors (e.g. moral development), and organizational
factors (e.g. code of ethics). Within the cultural context, these key antecedent factors interact to influence
ethical standards (Stajkovic and Luthans, 1997). Generally, the focus of this paper is limited to the
content of code of ethics.
Research has so far not approached the contents of codes of ethics from a strategic point of view. For
example, research has not considered the classification in terms of standardization, replication,
individualization and customization of the meaning of corporate codes of ethics within and across
contextual business environments. Therefore, the objective of this initial research effort is to introduce
and describe a generic framework of classification to provide insights into the diverse strategic
approaches of corporate code of ethics content within and across contextual business environments. The
current study uses corporate code of ethics collected from Australian corporations as empirical
illustrations (i.e. a selection of sample extracts).
Frame of Reference – Corporate Codes of Ethics
In previous research, the content of corporate codes of ethics have been analyzed according to scales
derived from Cressey and Moore (1983), Mathews (1998), Lefebvre and Singh (1992), Wood (2000),
Svensson and Wood (2004) and Singh et al. 2005).
Langlois and Schlegelmilch (1990) compare European and American corporate codes of ethics. They
found differences between the European and American codes, the first being that the European codes
were more recently developed than the American. Moreover, they found that while most ethical issues
transcend national barriers there were differences in the issues addressed by American and European
(British, French and West German) codes. In particular they concluded that European companies
emphasize employee responsiveness to company activities, while firms in the United States stress
company responsiveness to employee requirements of fairness and equity. Further, the study found
differences among the codes of British, French and German companies.
Other researchers have also found differences in ethical beliefs across cultures. Becker and Fritzsche
(1987) in a study of American, French, and German managers sought their views on five vignettes on
coercion and control, conflict of interest, the physical environment, personal integrity and paternalism.
Generally, the American managers responded differently from the European managers, but there were
also differences between the responses of the French and German respondents. This finding supports
Stajkovic and Luthan’s (1997) contention that the analysis of business ethical standards and resulting
ethical behavioral conduct is grounded in the unique characteristics of each national culture. In their
social cognitive model of business ethics across cultures, they propose that institutional factors, personal
factors and organizational codes of ethics also influence ethical standards and conduct. Singh et al. (2005)
104
came to the conclusion that there is a stronger formalization of rules reflected in the Australian and
Canadian corporate codes of ethics, as compared to the Swedish codes, that reflect the different
uncertainty avoidance values in these countries. The Swedish codes are also less regulatory than the
Australian and Canadian codes in respect to the codes mentioning enforcement procedures.
In the studies of codes of ethics and ethical values across cultures, it has been realized that ethical beliefs
vary across cultures and that the emphasis on certain aspects of the content of a code of ethics may also
differ between cultures. These findings have been found across European cultures, yet many of these
corporations studied would be doing business in other parts of the world where cultural values and norms
may be quite divergent from the parent company or accepted European cultural mores. This interest led
the researchers to postulate the framework of strategic classification approaches to code content as
discussed in the next section of this paper. Were there discernible differences in the emphasis on certain
aspects of the codes to be studied and were these differences culturally specific? If these differences did
exist was there any evidence to suggest that the companies studied had tailored their codes in order to
account for these cultural differences? The research was undertaken in Australia.
Framework of Classification – Strategic Approaches
The framework of classification used in the current study differs from the previous ones by Cressey and
Moore (1983), Mathews (1998), Lefebvre and Singh (1992), Wood (2000), Svensson and Wood (2004)
and Singh et al. (2005). These research efforts focus on the content of codes of ethics. The research at
hand focuses on the contextual consideration of code prescription and intensity of codification in
Australian codes of ethics content.
The content of corporate codes of ethics is surrounded by the corporation’s business operations and their
different contextual business environments. The contexts surrounding the codes of ethics are highly
dynamic, therefore, it is important to be aware that corporate codes of ethics should be regarded as
dynamic artifacts, instead of static ones. Cressey and Moore (1983), Mathews (1998), Lefebvre and Singh
(1992), Wood (2000) and Singh et al. (2005) have studied the static content, whereas this study provides
an initial and important insight into the dynamic features of corporate codes of ethics.
The content of corporate codes of ethics should evolve and adapt as contextual changes occur in business
operations of a corporation’s business environments. The content refers to the code prescription and the
intensity of codification used in codes of ethics, while the context refers to the code of ethics business
environment. There are a number of potential strategic approaches to corporate codes of ethics. The code
of ethics content, in conjunction with the reigning code of ethics context, provides a fundament to
distinguish a set of strategic approaches of corporate codes of ethics. They may be derived from two
principal dimensions, namely the application of content and the context of application. The dynamic
features inherent and derived from these two dimensions (i.e. meaning of content and context) in
corporate codes of ethics may be divided into a selection of strategic approaches (see Table 1), all of
which apply to both the corporation’s ethical values and principles used in business conduct.
105
Application of
Content
Same
Context of
Application
Uniform
Flexible
Standardized
Individualized
Replicated
Customized
Different
Table 1: Strategic Approaches to Corporate Codes of Ethics.
On the one hand, the context dimension may be divided into two sub-categories: within the same and
across different contextual business environments. On the other hand, the content dimension may also be
divided into two sub-categories: uniform and flexible meaning and interpretation to the code of ethics
content. Four generic strategic approaches of corporate codes of ethics may be distinguished based upon
these four sub-categories:
Standardized Strategic Approach means that the corporation applies a uniform meaning and interpretation
to the code of ethics content within the same contextual business environment (e.g. all suppliers or
customers). This approach assumes the universal applicability of the meaning of the corporate code of
ethics content and that it is consistent within the same context. Furthermore, it relies on the fact that there
are no essential or evident differences between the ethics values and principles in the corporation’s
contextual business environment at hand that justify a flexible meaning or interpretation of the code of
ethics content, but a standardized meaning is appropriate and sufficient.
Replicated Strategic Approach means that the corporation applies a uniform meaning and interpretation
for the code of ethics content across different contextual business environments (e.g. industries and
marketplaces). This approach assumes the universal applicability of the meaning of the corporate code of
ethics content and that it is consistent across different contexts. Furthermore, it relies on the fact that the
potential or recognized differences between the ethics values and principles in the corporation’s different
contextual business environments do not require a flexible meaning or interpretation of the code of ethics
content, but replication is appropriate and sufficient.
Individualized Strategic Approach means that the corporation applies a flexible meaning and
interpretation of the code of ethics content within the same contextual business environment (e.g. an
industry or a marketplace). This approach assumes the need for the tailored interpretation of the corporate
code of ethics content (although the contextual business environment is the same) to customer or supplier
criteria. Furthermore, it relies on the fact that each cultural business environment is unique and requires
separate and close attention to the reigning ethics values and principles of that contextual business
environment and that the meaning and interpretation of the code of ethics content to some extent is
required to be individualized.
Customized Strategic Approach means that the corporation applies a flexible meaning and interpretation
of the code of ethics content across different contextual business environments (e.g. industries or
marketplaces). This approach assumes the need for an adaptive interpretation of the corporate code of
ethics content (e.g. customer or supplier criteria). Furthermore, it relies on the fact that the recognized
106
differences between the ethical values and principles across the corporation’s contextual business
environments to some extent require the meaning and interpretation of the code of ethics content to be
customized.
Methodology
The purpose of this study was to examine the dynamic features of the content of the codes and to
examine, according to a specific method, the contents of corporate codes of ethics in Australia. This
research reports on the diverse strategic approaches and summarizes the issues addressed in the
introduced framework of classification in the codes of ethics of the top five hundred corporations in
Australia.
A survey document was sent to the public relations managers of the top 500 Australian companies (based
on revenue) operating in the private sector (Shoebridge, 2000). Companies were asked to answer the set
of questions in the survey and to supply a copy of their code of ethics. The 479 packages delivered
elicited 173 responses, with 49 companies indicating various reasons for not participating in the study, 30
indicating that they did not have a code, 81 indicating that they did have a code (65 sent codes and
questionnaires) and another 13 companies who declined to fill in the questionnaire, but who provided
their code. The useable response rate was therefore 25.9%. This paper reports on those 78 companies that
possessed and sent their code of ethics for examination by the researchers. These were used to provide
sample extracts from Australian Codes of Ethics according to the framework of strategic approaches
introduced in Table 1.
Empirical Findings
Previously, it was argued that the meaning of the content of corporate codes of ethics may benefit from
being flexible and adaptable as contextual changes occur in corporations’ business operations and various
business environments. Different strategic approaches to corporate codes of ethics content may be
identified (see Table 1). The code of ethics content, in conjunction with the reigning code of ethics
context, provides a fundament to distinguish these strategic approaches. In Tables 2 to 5, a selection of
sample extracts illustrate the diverse strategic approaches identified in the examined code of ethics
contents in top corporations of Australia.
Selection of Extracts – Standardized Strategic Approach
“...reaffirms the general business principles that govern each of the companies which make up the
group of companies conducts it affairs...”
“…to observe all applicable laws…”
“…provides guidelines and information to assist in understanding ethical values and standards of
behavior that apply in all our daily business activities…”
“…stipulates the standards of behavior and conduct of all employees…”
“…you are expected to behave with fairness, respect and honesty in all your dealings within the
corporation be it with fellow workers, customers or suppliers…”
“…perform our work safely and in accordance with the law…”
“…apply to the whole of the company, its subsidiaries and business units, and to every employee…”
“…applies to all employees, from the most senior to the most junior staff…”
“…employee must comply with all state and federal laws and regulations…”
“…everyone is equal when it comes to observing the code…”
“…employees must comply with the letter and, where it is clear, the spirit of all laws and regulations
relating to their business conduct…”
Table 2: Standardized Strategic Approach – Sample Extracts from Australian Codes of Ethics.
107
The selection of sample extracts of standardized strategic approaches from the examined Australian codes
of ethics in Table 2 shows that corporations apply a uniform meaning to the code of ethics content within
the same contextual business environment. A major part of the Australian corporate codes of ethics that
were examined relies on content such as the sample extracts provided in Table 2. Apparently, the
examined corporations presume to a large extent the universal applicability of the content of their code of
ethics. They may also presume that the content is consistent within the same context. Furthermore, there
is a perception that there is no essential or evident differences between the ethics values and principles in
the corporation’s contextual business environment at hand that justify a flexible meaning of the code of
ethics content within the same context. Standardization of major parts of the code of ethics content is
acknowledged as appropriate and sufficient.
Selection of Extracts – Replicated Strategic Approach
“…we must resist any temptation to modify our standards regardless of the situation…”
“…it provides guidance in ethical issues…”
“…treat customers, the public and fellow employees with honesty, courtesy and respect…”
“…employees should conduct themselves in an ethical manner at all times…”
“…has adopted policies which commit it to meeting its responsibilities in areas where ethical or legal
issues arise…”
“…managers are expected to lead according to our standards of ethical conduct, in both words and
actions…”
“…expects all employees of the company and its subsidiaries to act in accordance with the highest
standards of personal and professional integrity in all aspects of their employment and to comply with
all applicable laws, regulations, and company policies…”
“…every individual in the company is required to act in accordance with both the letter and spirit of
these principles…”
“…expects the highest levels of personal conduct by all its employees, whatever their position…”
“…strikes a balance between different environments…”
“…guided by principles of non-discrimination and respect for human rights and individual
freedom…”
“…all employees are urged to recognize, value and exemplify ethical conduct…”
“…disassociate yourself from participation in any possibly illegal activity with competitors, confine
your communication to what is clearly legal and proper…”
“…recognizing the complexities of a business operating in different environments across the globe,
the company aspires to be regarded as a good corporate citizen wherever it conducts business and it is
committed to the core values of civic society…”
Table 3: Replicated Strategic Approach – Sample Extracts from Australian Codes of Ethics.
The selection of extracts of replicated strategic approaches from the examined Australian codes of ethics
in Table 3 shows that corporations also apply a uniform meaning to the code of ethics content across
different contextual business environments. A substantial part of the examined Australian corporate codes
of ethics relies on contents such as the sample extract provided in Table 3. Subsequently, the studied
corporations tend to believe in the universal applicability of their code of ethics content. They also tend to
believe that the content is consistent across different contexts. Furthermore, there is a belief that there are
no essential or evident differences between the ethics values and principles in the corporation’s contextual
business environments at hand that justify a flexible meaning of the code of ethics content. Replication of
major parts of the code of ethics content is acknowledged as appropriate and sufficient.
108
Selection of Extracts – Individualized Strategic Approach
“…the rules are not exhaustive…”
“…we need to maintain our commitment to community accepted standards…”
“…this means not only complying with the law, but abiding the highest principles of integrity, honor
and concern for one another…”
“…it is our policy to go beyond the letter of the law and comply with its spirit…”
“…supports and protects the dignity, well-being, and rights of those with whom it is directly
involved: its employees and their families, and the local communities which are neighbors of its
operations…”
“…goes beyond the legal minimums…”
“…it would be wonderful if the right thing to do were always perfectly clear. In the real world of
business, however, things are not obvious…”
“…can only be translated into reality by the daily behavior of each and every one of us…”
“…while specific examples are offered to illustrate various situations, it needs to be stressed that these
are not exhaustive…”
“…ethical behavior goes beyond complying with laws…”
“…we respect diversity within humanity including gender, spiritual values, sexual preference, age,
disability and culture and provide services to support this…”
“…we are creative and dynamic in our response to the challenges facing society…”
Table 4: Individualized Strategic Approach – Sample Extracts from Australian Codes of Ethics.
The selection of extracts of individualized strategic approaches from the examined Australian codes of
ethics in Table 4 shows that corporations apply a flexible meaning to the code of ethics content within the
same context. A minor part of the examined Australian corporate codes of ethics relies on contents such
as the sample extract provided in Table 4. Accordingly, the examined corporations see to some extent the
need for the tailoring of the corporate code of ethics content (although the contextual business
environment is the same). They also see that the content is not consistent within the same context.
Furthermore, they see that each cultural business environment may be unique and may require separate
and close attention to the reigning ethics values and principles of that contextual business environment.
Individualization of minor parts of the code of ethics content is to some extent seen to be needed.
Selection of Extracts – Customized Strategic Approach
“…wherever we operate, we work as closely as possible with our hosts, respecting laws and customs,
minimizing adverse impacts…”
“…we seek to make lasting contributions to local communities and to be sensitive to their culture and
way of life…”
“…wherever the group operates, good relations with its neighbors are fundamental…”
“…knowing that each local community is different, the policy is that every operation shall strive to
understand and interact constructively with its local communities and to assist their development…”
“…requires continuing and effective two way communications and realistic expectations on both
sides…”
“…local circumstances may place particular demands on the way consideration is given…”
“…the company will comply with all laws and regulations in each country where it operates, and will
apply the standards reflecting the company’s aspiration to best international practice…”
“…the group is a decentralized, diversified group of companies with widespread activities, and each
company has a wide freedom of action…”
“…employees who accept an international assignment are responsible for familiarizing themselves
109
and their accompanying family members with the norms, laws and customs of the country in which
they will be living and working…”
Table 5: Customized Strategic Approach – Sample Extracts from Australian Codes of Ethics.
The selection of extracts of customized strategic approaches from the examined Australian codes of ethics
in Table 5 shows that corporations apply a flexible meaning to the code of ethics content across different
contextual business environments. A few of the examined Australian corporate codes of ethics rely on
content such as the sample extract provided in Table 5. Presumably, the studied corporations
acknowledge the need for adaptation of the corporate code of ethics content. They also acknowledge that
the content is not consistent across different contextual business environments. Furthermore, they also see
that each cultural business environment is unique and requires separate and close attention to the reigning
ethical values and principles of that contextual business environment. Customization of minor parts of the
code of ethics content is to some extent seen as necessary and requisite.
Concluding Thoughts and Suggestions for Further Research
The corporate codes of ethics in Australia studied may, to a large extent, be categorized into standardized
and replicated strategic approaches. It is a rather unexpected finding that the contents of the analyzed
corporate codes of ethics predominantly rely on standardization and replication considering the fact that
there are a set of other strategic approaches of corporate codes of ethics available (see Table 1). Many of
the studied corporations run international or worldwide business operations that have to confront different
contextual business environments. It would have been reasonable to expect a larger part of the content of
each code to have relied upon a flexible meaning within and across business environments. It would also
have been expected to observe more explicit or pronounced adaptations in the corporate codes of ethics
content in the same way as other business operations are adapted to each specific marketplace and
surrounding society. Apparently, the operational and tactical levels of the studied corporations’ business
operations have not influenced the strategic level of current corporate codes of ethics artifacts. Therefore,
further case studies of best/good practice in terms of customized and individualized strategic approaches
of corporate codes of ethics content would make a fruitful contribution to current knowledge in the field.
It is important to remember that a code of ethics is not a rulebook that addresses every ethical issue that
might arise. It is neither a summary of all laws and policies that apply, nor is it a contract. Furthermore, it
does not replace good judgment, but gives guidance and direction to resources to assist making the right
decisions. Nevertheless, the ways that the content of the corporate codes of ethics are expressed may
foreshadow, to a larger or lesser extent, flexible meanings of application rather than uniform ones.
When the corporate uncertainty avoidance increases, then as a consequence, the degree of code
prescription and the intensity of codification may also increase (Singh et al., 2005). These parameters
reflect the corporate propensity to uncertainty avoidance in corporate codes of ethics in different national
cultures. The meaning of the content of these codes should therefore be seen as dynamic, not static,
reflecting the need for uncertainty avoidance. The contents of corporate code of ethics may also be
interpreted as reflecting an inherent flexibility or inflexibility of meaning towards employees in the
corporations’ various business operations and different cultural business environments. On the one hand,
the code of ethics may contain a high degree of code prescription and codification intensity (i.e. less
flexibility of the meaning of the code of ethics content). On the other hand, the code of ethics may contain
a low degree of prescription and codification intensity (i.e. more flexibility of the meaning of the code of
ethics content). These combinations define the corporation’s strategic approach of flexibility and
uniformity of the actual meaning of content towards employees, customers, suppliers and/or other
stakeholders.
110
The need to avoid uncertainty in the marketplace and surrounding society implies a potential
inappropriateness of homogenous corporate codes of ethics contents across national and cultural business
environments. It also indicates the importance of a flexible meaning and interpretation of the corporate
codes of ethics content for multinational corporations in their international and cross-cultural business
operations. The diverse national and cultural contexts that corporations encounter in the marketplace and
the surrounding society should be taken into consideration in determining the meaning and interpretation
of corporate codes of ethics content. Otherwise, these corporate codes of ethics may be seen to be
meaningless and may not foster corporate ethical values and principles on the tactical and operational
levels. The code may be seen as window dressing on a strategic level, disconnected from the reality of the
corporation’s business operations and different business environments. All in all, this research has
triggered and advanced the search for strategic approaches in corporate codes of ethics content, which
complements previous research efforts in the field.
REFERENCES
Becker, H. and Fritzsche, D. 1987. A comparison of the ethical behaviour of American, French, and
German managers. Columbia Journal of World Business. 22: 87-95.
Berenbeim, R. 2000. Global Ethics. Executive Excellence. 17(5): 7.
Butler, J.W. 1994. Hammarabi, Hippocrates and business ethics. Executive Speeches. 9(1): 67.
Cressey, D.R. and Moore, C.A. 1983. Managerial values and corporate codes of ethics. California
Management Review, 25(4): 53-77.
Fritzsche, D. J. 2000. Ethical climates and the ethical dimension of decision making. Journal of Business
Ethics. 24 (2): 125-140.
Langlois, C.C. and Schlegelmilch, B.B. 1990. Do corporate codes of ethics reflect national character?
Evidence from Europe and the United States. Journal of International Business Studies. (Fourth
Quarter): 519-536.
Lefebvre, M. and Singh, J. 1992. The content and focus of Canadian corporate codes of ethics. Journal of
Business Ethics. 11: 799-808.
Mathews, M.C. 1998. Strategic Intervention in Organizations: Resolving Ethical Dilemmas. Newbury
Park, California: Sage Publications, Inc.
Shoebridge, N. 2000. The Big List. Business Review Weekly Nov 17: 115-134.
Singh, J., Carasco, E., Wood, G., Svensson, G. and Callaghan, M. 2005. A Comparative Study of the
Contents of Corporate Codes of Ethics in Australia, Canada and Sweden. Journal of World
Business. 40: 91-109.
Stajkovic, A.D. and Luthans, F. 1997. Business ethics across cultures: A social cognitive model. Journal
of World Business. 32(1): 17-34.
Svensson, G. and Wood, G. 2004. Codes of Ethics Best Practice in the Swedish Public Sector: a
PUBSEC-Scale. International Journal of Public Sector Management. 17(2): 178-195.
Wood, G. 2000. A cross cultural comparison of the content of codes of ethics: USA, Canada and
Australia. Journal of Business Ethics. 25: 287-298.
111
Localize Global Codes of Ethics
Göran Svensson, Halmstad University, Sweden
Dr Greg Wood, Deakin University, Australia
Introduction and Objective
Corporate codes of ethics are one of several influences on corporate and business ethics within and across
contexts. In a model that goes beyond philosophically-based ethics, Stajkovic and Luthans (1997) use
social-cognitive theory as a springboard to identify factors influencing business ethics standards and
conduct. They propose that a person’s perception of ethical standards and subsequent conduct is
influenced by institutional factors (e.g. ethics legislation), personal factors (e.g. moral development), and
organizational factors (e.g. code of ethics). Within the cultural context, the key antecedent factors
triadically interact to influence ethical standards (Stajkovic and Luthans 1997). Generally, this paper is
restricted to corporate and business ethics. In particular, it is limited to the corporate code of ethics and
the ways in which an organization supports the implementation of their code.
Research has so far rarely approached the application of corporate code of ethics. The objective is to
develop an empirically based typological framework of corporate codes of ethics, and a structured
framework of applications.
Frame of Reference – Corporate Codes of Ethics
Langlois and Schlegelmilch (1990) define a corporate code of ethics as a statement setting down
corporate principles, ethics, rules of conduct, codes of practice or company philosophy concerning
responsibility to employees, shareholders, consumers, the environment, or any other aspects of society
external to the company. These documents vary in length, breadth of topics covered and extent to which
topics are covered. Berenbeim (2000) cites three trends as evidence of the growing importance of
corporate codes of ethics: the globalization of markets and the need for core principles that are universally
applicable, the acceptance of these codes as part of the corporate governance as illustrated by increased
participation of boards in their development and the improved ethical literacy of senior managers as
illustrated by the increasing sophistication of the codes. Foremost among the internal measures available
to corporations are corporate codes of ethics. With multinational corporations such codes govern their
operations worldwide and promote uniformity. However, what is covered by such codes and commitment
to enforcing them vary among corporations (Carasco and Singh 2003), multinational or not.
Despite the mixed findings of research studies on the effectiveness of corporate codes of ethics in
influencing behavior, studies have found that having a code of ethics does have a positive impact on the
ethical behavior of the employees of the organization. (Adams Tashchian and Stone 2001; Ferrell and
Skinner 1988; McCabe, Trevino and Butterfield 1996; Pierce and Henry 1996; Schwartz 2001; Somers
2001; Stohs and Brannick 1999; Wotruba Chonko and Loe 2001). Other studies have found this causal
relationship not to be conclusively the case (Clark and Leonard 1998; Ford, Gray and Landrum 1982;
Mathews 1998; McKendall, Demarr and Jones-Rikkers 2002).
Methodology
The current methodological approach differs from previous ones that have examined corporate and public
codes of ethics (e.g. Cressey and Moore 1983; Lefebvre and Singh 1996, 1992;Mathews 1998; Svensson
and Wood 2004a; and Wood 2000). The research is based upon a case study of McDonald’s Corporation.
The purpose was to analyze, according to a specific framework of content analysis the McDonald’s codes
112
of ethics. The study sought to identify the different areas of application and to summarize the areas found
in a framework of content analysis.
The data that underpins the case illustration has been collected as part of a larger research project of codes
of ethics across countries: Australia, Canada and Sweden. This paper draws upon and reports the findings
from one case of the larger research project that has been used in this instance to inform the codes of
ethics applications in corporate and business ethics settings. The McDonald’s code of ethics shapes the
case study at hand. The case illustration is based upon an inductive approach and qualitative methods. All
data was collected from publicly issued documents at the McDonald’s homepage. The content analysis of
the research efforts was based upon these data.
Corporate Code of Ethics – A Framework of Content Analysis
Corporate codes of ethics are surrounded by – and should be embedded in the business operations and the
different contextual business environments of corporations. The contextual business environment consists
of geographical and cultural settings such as: communities, regions, countries and continents. The
contexts surrounding the codes of ethics are dynamic and in some cases highly volatile. Therefore, the
contents of corporate codes of ethics have to be equally adaptable and permit flexibility to the reigning
context in each moment in which the company finds itself. Areas of application may be: management,
employee, customer, supplier, subcontractor, owner/operator, shareholder, competitor and community,
therefore, it is important that corporate codes of ethics should be seen as dynamic artifacts, rather than
static ones. They should not be seen as window-dressing, but they should support and govern corporate
business conduct where necessary. They should also address ethics values and principles. Cressey and
Moore (1983), Mathews (1998), Lefebvre and Singh (1992), Wood (2000) and Svensson and Wood
(2004a) have studied the static content of corporate codes of ethics, whereas this study provides an insight
into the dynamic features of a corporate code of ethics (i.e. context and content). For this purpose, a
framework of content analysis has been developed, which is described in this section.
Applications of Codes of Ethics
There are various standards that may be applied in corporate code of ethics and the business conduct
applicable to them. They may be derived from and illustrated by a set of principal dimensions, such as
application of content, context of application and timing of application. These dimensions are mutually
interdependent, which means that the change in one dimension may cause changes in the other two, and
vice versa.
Svensson and Wood (2003) conclude that business ethics is about what is perceived as acceptable or
unacceptable at a specific time and in a specific cultural setting. What was ethical yesterday may not be
ethical today, and what is ethical today may not be ethical tomorrow, therefore, the timing of application
in the business environment is often crucial to success. It applies also to ethics values and principles.
Ideally, the applications of ethics values and principles should be ahead of the stage in the marketplace to
which other companies have evolved or they should at least have achieved consensus (i.e. a par with the
evolutionary stage). The worst scenario occurs when corporate ethics values and principles are lagging
behind the evolutionary stage in the marketplace. Svensson and Wood (2004b) explore the common
grounds of proactive versus reactive business ethics and conclude that business ethics performance
requires the ongoing re-connection with reality by corporations.
The application of content (i.e. ethics values and principles) may be the same or similar on some
occasions, while it may be different or diverse in others. The context of application may take place within
or across contexts. Two principal parameters of codes govern the timing of application. These parameters
are the application of content and context of application, namely the degree of code prescription and the
intensity of codification of the organization’s ethics values and principles.
113
A Typological Framework of Codes of Ethics
Based upon the potential outcomes of the application of content (i.e. same versus different) and the
context of application (i.e. same versus different) a matrix of classification may be outlined (see Table 1).
It serves as a descriptive typology to classify corporate codes of ethics and their ethics values and
principles.
Application of
Content
Same
Context of
Application
Same
Different
Local/National
International
Global
Glocal
Different
Table 1: A Typological Framework of Codes of Ethics.
The content of corporate codes of ethics should evolve and should be adapted to the contextual changes
that occur in the corporation’s business environments. There is a selection of generic descriptive criteria
for corporate codes of ethics. The code of ethics content, in conjunction with the reigning code of ethics
context, provides a fundament to distinguish drivers of corporate codes of ethics. They may be derived
from two principal dimensions, namely the application of content and the context of application. The
dynamic features inherent and derived from these two dimensions (i.e. content and context) in corporate
codes of ethics may be divided into a typology, all of which applies to both the corporation’s ethics values
and the ethics principles used in corporate codes of ethics. On the one hand, the context dimension may
be divided into two sub-categories: same and different. On the other hand, the content dimension may
also be divided into two sub-categories: same and different. Four generic types of corporate codes of
ethics may be distinguished based upon these four sub-categories:
Local/National Standards of Business Conduct refers to the idea that the corporation uses the same code
of ethics within the same contextual business environment (e.g. the industry and all suppliers/customers).
It presumes that the corporate code of ethics is applicable and consistent within the same context. It also
assumes that there are no essential or evident differences of the ethics values and the ethics principles in
the corporation’s contextual business environments at hand that would justify a different code of ethics.
International Standards of Business Conduct refers to the way that the corporation uses different
adaptations of codes of ethics within the same contextual business environment (e.g. the industry or the
marketplace in different countries). It presumes the need for an adaptation of the code of ethics (e.g. to
customer or supplier criteria in each country’s marketplace). It also assumes that the recognized
differences between the ethics values and principles within the corporation’s same contextual business
environment to some extent require different or at the least modified codes of ethics.
Global Standards of Business Conduct refers to the idea that the corporation uses the same code of ethics
across different contextual business environments (e.g. industries and marketplaces in different countries
and continents). It presumes that the code of ethics is applicable and consistent across different contexts.
Furthermore, it assumes that the ethics values and principles in the corporation’s different contextual
business environments do not require different codes of ethics.
114
Glocal Standards of Business Conduct refers to the contention that the corporation uses different codes of
ethics across different contextual business environments (e.g. industries and marketplaces in different
countries and continents). It presumes the need for the tailoring of the code of ethics to customer or
supplier criteria. It also assumes that each contextual business environment is unique. Furthermore, each
of these environments requires separate and close attention to the reigning ethics values and principles of
that contextual business environment and that the code of ethics content to an extent requires continuous
adaptation.
It is also important to remember that the timing of application of the use codes of ethics may not be
foreseen or neglected in corporate business and market analysis of ethics values and principles.
Empirical Illustration: The Case of McDonalds
McDonald’s standards of business conduct may be described by using the dimensions: application of
content, context of application and timing of application. The outcome of the content analysis is
summarized in Figure 1.
Application of Content
– Underlying Level
Application of Content
– Overall Level
Timing of
Application
Local
Context of
Application
Confidential
Information
Code of
Conduct
Atmosphere
Global
Employment
Practices
Management
Customer
Insider
Information
Employee
Environment
Policy
Ethical
ShareOwner/
Operator Dependable holder
Animal
Energy
Truthful
Welfare
SubSupplier
contractor Additional
Natural
Resources
Resources
Competitor
Community
Gift, Favors &
Business
Record
Entertainment
National
Quality and
Sharing
Safety
Information
International
Figure 1: The McDonald’s Applications of Code of Ethics.
115
Concluding Thoughts
The case study of McDonald’s has indicated the potential inappropriateness of the homogeneity of
corporate codes of ethics across national and cultural business environments. This research has also
indicated the importance of a flexible approach to corporate codes of ethics content for intercontinental
corporations and their international or worldwide business operations. The diverse national and cultural
contexts that corporations will encounter should be brought into consideration in the content of corporate
codes of ethics, otherwise, the corporate codes of ethics may be considered to be meaningless and may
not contribute to accomplish the corporation’s ethics values and ethics principles on an operational,
tactical and strategic level. The code may only become a kind of ‘window dressing’ on a strategic level
that fails to connect with the reality in the corporation’s business operations and business environments
around the world. At worst, the inherent flaws of xenophobic corporate codes of ethics may create severe
ethical dilemmas in the marketplace and society if they are used generically across cultures.
The contents of corporate codes of ethics are surrounded by the corporations’ business operations and
their different contextual business environments. The contexts surrounding these codes of ethics are
highly dynamic. Therefore, it is important to be aware that corporate codes of ethics should be regarded
as dynamic artifacts, instead of static ones. Cressey and Moore (1983), Mathews (1998), Lefebvre and
Singh (1992) and Wood (2000) have studied the static content, whereas this study provides an initial and
important insight into the dynamic features of corporate codes of ethics (i.e. context and content).
The world often bemoans ‘business imperialism’ from first world countries upon each other, but more so
from first world countries upon developing nations. To foist one’s ethical perspectives upon other cultures
without regard for their historic, ethical perspectives is far worse, because it devalues the societal values
and beliefs of these cultures. McDonald’s appears to be cognizant of these needs to accommodate the
values of others. Whilst it is expected that the cultural values of the home country of the company will be
dominant, there is a distinct difference between dominant values and beliefs and dominating other’s
values and beliefs. A glocal approach to codes of ethics and the interpretation of these codes at a local
level can only but assist to personalize the values inherent in the corporation’s business values. Not only
does the home country culture share its views with others, but a glocal approach allows the home country
to grow richer in its views as a result of interaction with the beliefs and values of other cultures. A
discussion of and a sharing of one’s ethical values and beliefs across cultures can only but enhance the
perspectives of all parties and one hopes enhance the business and social outcomes for all stakeholders.
REFERENCES
Adams, J., Taschian, A., and Stone, T. 2001. Codes of ethics as signals for ethical behavior. Journal of
Business Ethics, 29:3,199-211.
Berenbeim, R. 2000. Global Ethics. Executive Excellence, 17:5, 7.
Carasco, E.F. and Singh, J.B. 2003. The content and focus of the codes of ethics of the world’s largest
transnational corporations. Business and Society Review, 108:1, 71-94.
Clark, M.A. and Leonard, S.L. 1998. Can corporate codes of ethics influence behavior? Journal of
Business Ethics 17:6, 619-630.
Cressey, D.R. and Moore, C.A. 1983. Managerial values and corporate codes of ethics. California
Management Review, 25:4, 53-77.
Ferrell, O.C. and Skinner, S.J. 1988. Ethical behaviour and bureaucratic structure in marketing research
organizations. Journal of Marketing Research, 25: 103-109.
116
Ford, R., Gray, B. and Landrum,R. 1982. Do organizational codes of conduct really affect employees’
behaviour? Management Review, 72: 53-54.
Langlois, C.C. and Schlegelmilch, B.B. 1990. Do corporate codes of ethics reflect national character?
Evidence from Europe and the United States. Journal of International Business Studies, Fourth
Quarter, 519-536.
Lefebvre, M. and Singh, J. 1992. The content and focus of Canadian corporate codes of
ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 11: 799-808.
Lefebvre, M. and Singh, J. 1996. A comparison of the contents and foci of Canadian and American
corporate codes of ethics. International Journal of Management ,13:2, 156-171.
Mathews, M.C. 1998. Strategic Intervention in Organizations: Resolving Ethical Dilemmas. Newbury
Park, California: Sage Publications, Inc.
McCabe, D.L., Trevino, L.K. and Butterfield, K.D. 1996. The influence of collegiate and corporate codes
of conduct on ethics-related behaviour in the workplace. Business Ethics Quarterly, 6:4, 461-476.
McKendall,M., DeMarr,B. and Jones-Rikkers, C. 2002. Ethical compliance programs and corporate
illegality: Testing the assumptions of the corporate sentencing guidelines. Journal of Business
Ethics, 37: 367-383.
Pierce, M.A. and Henry, J.W. 1996. Computer ethics: The role of personal, informal, and formal codes.
Journal of Business Ethics, 15: 425-437.
Schwartz, M. 2001. The nature of the relationship between corporate codes of ethics and behaviour.
Journal of Business Ethics, 32: 247-262.
Somers, M. J. 2001. Ethical codes and organizational context: A study of the relationship between codes
of conduct, employee behavior and organizational values. Journal of Business Ethics, 30:2, 185195.
Stajkovic, A.D. and Luthans, F. 1997. Business ethics across cultures: A social cognitive model. Journal
of World Business, 32:1, 17-34.
Stohs, J.H. and Brannick, T. 1999. Codes of conduct: Predictors of Irish managers ethical reasoning.
Journal of Business Ethics, 22: 311-326.
Svensson, G. and Wood, G. 2004a, “Codes of Ethics Best Practice in the Swedish Public Sector: a
PUBSEC-Scale”, International Journal of Public Sector Management, 17:2, 178-195.
Svensson, G. and Wood, G. 2004b, “Proactive versus Reactive Business Ethics Performance: A
Conceptual Framework of Profile Analysis and Case Illustrations”, Corporate Governance: The
International Journal of Business in Society, 4:2, 18-33.
Svensson, G. and Wood, G. 2003, “The Dynamics of Business Ethics: a Function of Time and Culture –
Cases and Models”, Management Decision, 41:4, 350-361.
Wood, G. 2000. A cross cultural comparison of the content of codes of ethics: USA, Canada and
Australia. Journal of Business Ethics, 25: 287-298.
Wotruba, T.R., Chonko, L.B. and Loe, T.W. 2001. The impact of ethics code familiarity on manager
behaviour. Journal of Business Ethics, 33: 59-69.
117
____________________________________________________
Consumer’s Moral Sensitivity: Building a Scale to Measure
Moral Sensitivity of Consumers and Linking it to the
Purchasing Involvement Construct
Samar M. Baqer, University of Texas at Arlington
______________________________________________________________________________
Abstract
In this study, we are trying to validate a scale for the construct of moral sensitivity. We relied on
literature from different disciplines including: marketing, education, psychology, and philosophy in order
to be able to find a clear definition for this construct. We also discuss the importance of moral sensitivity
for a better understanding of the purchasing behavior of customers and we argue that it should be
considered before approaching customers with any marketing activity. Structured equation modeling was
used to validate the scale and OLS regression was used for checking the predictability of our model. The
major findings of this research piece is that customers’ morals do interfere in their purchasing decision
and that moral sensitivity should be considered for predicting the purchasing involvement of current and
prospective customers.
Introduction
Although consumers are the major participants in business and buyer/seller relationships, the research of
consumer’s ethics accounts for only 5% of the literature of marketing ethics and moral behavior (Vitell,
Singhapakdi, and Thomas 2001, Fukukawa 2003, Vitell 2003). When we discuss consumer’s ethics in
this paper, we mean both of the cognitive development and the social learning aspects that affect the
consumer’s decision making process. In order to simplify the research process, we will adopt Ferrell,
Gresham and Fraedrich’s (1989) proposed synthesis model that will combine micro as well as macro
constructs.
Consumer’s moral sensitivity is among the neglected areas in research consumer ethics. The concept of
moral sensitivity appeared previously in the literature of psychology for the purpose of training
professionals (Rest 1984; Volker 1984; Bredemeier & Shields 1984; Myyry & Helkama 2002). Also,
when it was discussed in marketing literature, it didn’t, appropriately, address the underlying importance
of understanding the deep ethical thoughts of customers and how these values and morals affect
customer’s decision making. One could not ignore the micro-macro dilemma discussed by McCarthy
(1981, P. 11) that explains the fight between the self-interest and the interest of the society (Dixon 1982).
A customer would usually care about his/her own interest but at the same time might agonize over the fact
that the purchasing decision might be bad for the society as a whole (Dixon1982). We feel that it would
be interesting if we can develop a scale that could be used for understanding the moral sensitivity of
customers toward making specific purchases when they are tied to an ethical dilemma. We believe that
ethical decision making is not an easy process and it requires careful analyses.
Previous literature discussed customers’ perception of marketing processes as unethical (Laczniak 1981,
Andreasen 1982). Also, there were many discussions regarding customers’ ethical evaluation of
marketing campaigns relying on the type of products involved, especially products that involve social
controversies (Laczniak 1981). Socially responsible customers will consider the consequences of their
purchasing decisions. When consumers are faced with an ethical problem they need to be aware of its
118
existence and they must believe that their behavior is going to contribute to resolving this problem
(Singhapakdi, Ansusorn and Scott J. Vitell 1990).
Based on that, it would be very essential for companies to be aware to customers’ beliefs and cultural
symbols, and consider them before launching any type of marketing activities because it may affect the
customer’s purchasing decision (Ringlod 1998; Hackley 1999). One of the best ways to save the
company’s reputation and ethical standards is to promote consumer interests with regards to the ethical
standards (Fukukawa 2003). The customer’s perceived view of the firm’s unfairness is very essential
since it could ruin the reputation of the firm (Fukukawa 2003). When customers think, based on their
moral opinions, that a company is violating moral values, they tend to avoid purchasing products from
that company and it becomes very hard to build a long term relationship with them (Baron 1999). In
addition to that, customers could spread bad word of mouth about that company and they could ask for
regulations and laws to ban its goods (Baron 1999). Eventually, companies need to tell their customers
that they care about them and that they are trying to respect their values and beliefs.
In this paper we will be discussing the differences between morals and ethics. After that, we discuss, in
detail, the construct of moral sensitivity and introduce four major dimensions that represent this construct.
The main purpose of this paper was to develop a new scale to measure a construct of interest. We used
structural equation modeling technique to develop a scale for measuring the construct of moral sensitivity
and to assess the construct validation of it through assessing convergent validity, discriminant validity,
and nomological validity. Finally, we will be investigating the predictability of our model by using
multiple regression.
Literature Review and Hypotheses
Definitions from the Glossary of Ethics Terms
It has been noticed in literature that morality and ethics are two different constructs, although they were
known to be the same thing. In order to understand the difference between these two constructs, we need
to include some definitions that can explain the two constructs better.
We found that Taylor’s (1975) definition of ethics is one of the simplest ways of explaining the difference
between the two terms: “ethics…inquiry into the nature and grounds of morality where the term morality
is taken to mean moral judgments, standards, and rules of conduct.” (Taylor 1975, p.1). Morals can be
considered as the rules that help people in making decisions and being motivated to behave correctly
(MacDonald 2002). Also, we can use morals as tools to judge other people and evaluate situations. In
contrast, ethics are values that are engraved inside of us and we have the choice whether to use and
impose it on other people or not. In other words, ethics are basically the values that we believe in and they
become morals as soon as we impose them on others as part of the interaction process (MacDonald 2002).
Another simple distinction was mentioned in the Payton Papers’ website: “Ethics is the science of morals
and morals are the practice of ethics”. One could be behaving morally without knowing the ethical
aspect of the situation that he/she is facing. Morality is an aspect of a situation while thinking about that
situation and how to behave is the ethical part of the decision making process. Ethics could be a cognitive
process of evaluating situations and considering consequences of judgments and decisions while morals is
a process of following the rules resulted in the cognitive process (PaytonPapers.org 2000).
Moral Development
The theories of moral development have always suffered the lack of evaluation due to the difficulty of
specific and consistent definitions (Lovecky 1997; Erwin 2000).
Lawrence Kohlberg could be considered the father of the moral development theories. He was a moral
philosopher and his main interest was in the area of children’s moral development. He built his famous
stages of moral development based on the story of God and ancient Israel. In his book “Theories of
119
development”, W. C. Crain has discussed Kohlberg’s stages of moral reasoning and the following is a
brief discussion of those stages:
I)
Premoral or Preconventional Stages: (self-interest) Stage 1: Punishment and Obedience:
The basic idea behind it is that the physical consequences of any action are considered as
determinants of its level of goodness or badness. Stage 2: Instrumental Exchange: You do to
people what you expect them to do for you. It is like any exchange process in the market place.
II)
Conventional Morality: Stage 3: Interpersonal Conformity: People usually seek the approval of
others. If you belong to a group, you direct your actions toward whatever pleases or helps the
other members of that group. Stage 4: Law and Order: People are usually supposed to follow
rules and orders of authority.
III)
Postconventional or Principled Morality: Stage 5: Prior Right and Social Contract:
People protect their rights first and then they protect the society rights. Stage 6: Universal Ethical
Principals: If a person can reach this stage, he will be able to act based on universal principles
that imply the importance of human rights and equalities. (Cory (1986), and Crain (1985))
Kohlberg’s theory of moral development (1984) focuses on the process of decision making and how
would a person reach a conclusion in a particular situation based on his/her judgment of what is
considered fair (Lovecky 1997; Kohlberg 1984). The major criticisms to Kohlberg are based on the fact
that he didn’t relate to individuals’ actions and their thoughts about what’s wrong and what’s right. Also,
he has been criticized because of his model’s cognitive rigidity and his use of justice as the core of
morality (Dierchx, Roelens, & Gastmans). That’s when Hunt and Vitell step in and fill in the gaps of
moral development process. Hunt and Vitell (1986) argued that ethical judgments and intentions of
customers should be used as predictors of their behavior in a situation with an ethical dilemma. They
discussed two types of evaluations: deontological which focuses on the consumers’ actions and
teleological which focuses on the consequences of these actions (Hunt and Vitell 1986, Vitell, 2003).
Sometimes consumers would consider both types of evaluations in order to make their ethical decisions
(Hunt and Vitell 1986, Vitell 2003).
Moral Decision Making
Rest (1984) identified four important components of moral behavior that have been used in the literature:
1) moral sensitivity, 2) moral judgment, 3) moral motivation, and 4) moral character (Myyry & Helkama
2002). Moral Judgment has been discussed in the literature heavily while moral sensitivity didn’t get the
same luck (Myyry & Helkama 2002). Nevertheless, Kohlberg’s model, in its simplest forms, can be
highly related to the decision making process. People with different moral reasoning levels tend to use
morals to evaluate a situation and take a decision with different degrees. These different degrees of
reliance on morals in the decision making process is what I want to measure in my study. Mudrack (2003)
has argued that the reasons for making a decision are more important than the decision itself. When
someone makes a decision he will be automatically trying to increase his benefits or rewards and at the
same time avoiding any type of physical or emotional punishment. This agrees with the first two stages of
Kohlberg’s model (Mudrack 2003).
One of the proposed models in the literature is Jones (1999) conceptualization of the construct of moral
intensity and identified six dimensions that represent this construct: 1) magnitude of consequences, 2)
social consensus, 3) probability effect, 4) temporal immediacy, 5) proximity, and 6) concentration effect
(Singhapakdi, et al. 1996; Jones 1999). Singhapakdi et al. (1996) developed a scale that represents these
dimensions of the moral intensity construct (Singhapakdi et al. 1999). They were able to identify two
factors to capture the effect of the previous six dimensions: 1) perceived potential harm and 2) perceived
120
social pressure (Singhapakdi et al. 1996; Singhapakdi et al. 1999). We believe that this construct shares
some similarities with the concept of moral sensitivity which is represented by this paper.
Kohlberg (1969) argued that individuals will respond differently to similar situations with an ethical
center based on their stages of cognitive moral development (Ferrell, Gresham and Fraedrich 1989).
Ferrell and Gresham (1985) introduced a framework that showed that interaction effect between type of
ethical situation and other factors including the characteristics of the individual and the chance to engage
in an unethical behavior. Some of the individual characteristics include: education, values and intentions
(Ferrell, Gresham and Fraedrich 1989). On the same token, many people have a number of protected
values that they are not willing to sacrifice for the sake of gaining economic benefits (Baron & Spranca
1997; Baron 1999). That’s why these people refrain from participating in a purchasing process that could
lead into violating their protected values. They might also pay extra money for other products that do not
conflict with personal values and morals (Baron 1999). This cognitive effort that they go through is
manifested by their level of involvement in the purchasing process. We will refer to the variation in the
reliance on morality as the moral sensitivity of a person. Particularly, we are interested in the consumer’s
moral sensitivity and its relation to his/her purchasing decisions. We anticipate that the degree of the
affective involvement in the purchasing decision is affected by the degree of moral sensitivity. The
following are more explanations of the concepts that we are using in our study.
Moral Sensitivity: Is one of the major aspects of moral decision making. Rest (1984) argued that when a
person realized that his/her specific behavior could affect other people negatively, this person will be
considered as a morally sensitive one. In addition, Bredemeier and Shields (1984) discussed other aspects
of moral sensitivity when they defined three factors that could affect it: 1) contextual factors: which are
factors in the environment that influence behavior, 2) personal competency factors: which are cognitive
and affective factors variables that influence moral behavior, and 3) ego processing factors which
represent coordination between psychological processes affecting the moral behavior (Erwin 2000).
Sometimes people behave immorally just because they are not sensitive to the moral aspect of this
particular situation (MacDonald 2002). Some people will find it difficult to see the moral aspect of a
particular situation because they are not aware of its moral nature and/or their beliefs do not categorize
that particular situation as one that raises a moral dilemma. Of course, people differ in the way they set
their priorities in life. For example, some people would respect morals and principles and defend them on
the expense of financial gains.
We believe that the following variables can be considered as dimensions for the construct of moral
sensitivity. Those dimensions were discussed in the literature separately and in different occasions. We
decided to choose these particular dimensions based on their logical links to the construct of our interest.
Moral Awareness: We can describe it as moral knowing or moral understanding (Woodstock 1990).
People tend to know and understand morals with practice because it becomes like a skill that they develop
over time. Being aware of the moral situation is an important factor that affects the person’s moral
judgments. Among those judgments is the decision to purchase a certain product which could have ethical
or unethical attributes. When the customer is aware of the ethical problem associated with his decision
process, he will start to become more involved with the purchasing process and try to look for solutions
and alternatives in order to resolve the ethical problem (Hunt and Vitell 1986). Consumers will use their
beliefs in order to determine the rightness or the wrongness of the behavior or the decision that they are
going to choose.
This leads to our first hypothesis:
H1: Customers who are aware of the moral aspects of the situations/problems that they face tend
to be more involved in the purchasing process.
121
Cognitive Development: We live in a dynamic world and people, at one point of time, may choose to alter
some aspects of their behavior to adapt to and participate in the world’s continuous changes. Some factors
can lead a person to change his/her behavior are: to become a better person, to be accepted socially, to fit
in a certain group of people or certain society, to follow a new belief, etc. As we can see, morals play an
important role in guiding people through those behavioral changes and the reason that follows is the
simple definition of morals as our guide in deciding what’s wrong and what’s right (Woodstock 1990).
One could argue that there are some societies that blindly follow their principles without understanding
them appropriately, but the cultural differences are not discussed in this paper and we assume that we are
talking about free societies where people feel free to choose their own principles and behave according to
them. Social agreements are among the important factors that shape the consumer’s ethical decision
making. In the consumer ethics research we care about the cognitive approaches that are used for
understanding moral judgments and internal reasoning (i.e. Kohlberg’s model 1969) (Fukukawa 2003).
Although, it was argued in the literature that the social learning approach helps in investigating the
influence of external factors on moral judgments as well (Fukukawa 2003). In addition, we should not
neglect the fact that consumers consider what bad or good consequences would result from their decisions
(Hunt and Vitell 1986). All of these factors force the customer to become more involved in the purchasing
decision since he/she has to consider the other external and social aspect of this decision.
H2: Customers who don’t usually change their morals and principles tend to be affectively
involved in the purchasing process.
Moral Commitment: Having principles and rules does not mean that people will behave according to
their morals most of the time unless they choose to commit themselves to those moral principles, and we
stress again that those morals are chosen freely (Gray 1996). People can become creative and imaginative
in practicing their commitment to moral principles because they don’t see this commitment as an
obligation or a burden (Gray 1996). Some times that type of commitment comes from religious beliefs.
The power of religion on cultures comes from its social authority. This authority by itself can force people
to follow certain rules and to try not breaking them even for the sake of gaining other benefits
(Mittelstaedt 2002).
Based on that, we can see the differences between people when it comes to their values and their
principles. These differences in beliefs will also affect the level of the customers’ involvement in the
purchasing process.
H3: Customers who are committed to their morals and principles tend to have more affective
purchasing involvement.
Recognition of Social Principles: Many people tend to care about their self image and how people look
at them. When it comes to taking a moral decision, discussing the situation (or moral question) with other
people, maybe professional, becomes important because people do not want to carry the responsibility
and guilt of taking this decision alone (MacDonald 2002). As a participant in the market exchange
process, consumers tend to have some concerns about their purchasing decisions (Shaw & Shiu 2002).
There are three determinants of the ethical judgments: 1) the consumer’s motivation to make the
judgment and whether it was because he is expecting any benefit from it or he is just being passive, 2) the
legal aspects of the situation, and 3) the degree of harm that could be resulted from his/her judgment
(Vitell 1997). We want our actions to be justified and to be reasonable to others.
We were trying to follow Loveckey (1997) definition of a person with moral reasoning as a person who
values moral decisions and always thinks of the greatest good for people (Lovecky 1997, p.91). Also,
empathy and compassion would receive smaller weights compared to justice in the process of moral
decision making (Kohlberg 1984, Lovecky 1997). Customers try to avoid the regret of purchasing a
122
product by predicting the affect of that decision on their image of people and their self-recognition. One
could argue that all of this cognitive effort will tend to increase the customers’ involvement in the
purchasing process.
H4: Customers who care about how they feel about themselves and how other people think of
them tend to be more involved in the purchasing process.
We are measuring the construct of moral sensitivity through the previous four dimensions: moral
awareness, moral commitment, cognitive development, and recognition of social principles. Please refer
to appendix A.
Methodology
Scale Development
Based on the previous literature review, We have decided to measure the construct of Moral Sensitivity
through four dimensions: 1) awareness of the moral issue, 2) commitment to one’s morals, 3) self and
social recognition, and 4) cognitive development. To represent these dimensions, we generated 26 items
(7-point Likert scale). We had more items at the initial stages of our study (more than 7 items per
dimension) and we used peer and expert reviews to decide on the items that should be retained. Some of
our items needed to be reverse-coded. It was very important to maintain face validity and clarity of items
in order to make it easier for subjects to understand our survey. For the purpose of assessing nomological
validity of the moral sensitivity construct we had to choose a construct that has been measured previously
(reliable). We have chosen the 7-point Likert scale of purchasing involvement (affective) developed by
Ratchford (1987) and administered by Putrevu and Lord (1994). This scale contains three items and it
measures the degree to which customers consider the purchasing decision as being influenced by one’s
feelings. Please refer to the entire questionnaire in appendix B.
Subjects
We used 200 questionnaires in undergraduate business classes and 166 of the questionnaires were
completed. Students were promised to receive extra credits for their participation in our study. In addition,
students were asked to answer questions about the process of decision-making based on the provided
scenario. We needed to come up with a scenario that could raise a moral dilemma. Among the issues that
were discussed in previous literature is the dilemma of child labor in poor countries. The whole debate
was that children need to work in these countries (under poor an unhealthy conditions) in order to obtain
basic life needs. We wanted to use a scenario that could trigger affective and cognitive principles and
moral standards at the same time.
Please refer to the consent form and the questionnaire in appendix B.
Results
In our study, we used the structural equation modeling method to conduct Confirmatory Factor Analyses.
As discussed earlier, we have hypothesized the representation of our items to the moral sensitivity
construct.
Measurement Model for the Construct of Moral Sensitivity
We started with the construct of moral sensitivity because we wanted to make sure that the items we have
generated are appropriately presenting the construct’s dimensions. In addition, we needed to prove that
our model fits the data collected. To assess unidimensionality, convergent validity, and construct validity,
we had to depend on different criteria. Going through LISREL outputs we had to check: 1) LISREL
estimates to make sure that the parameters are positive and significant, 2) the goodness of fit index to
check our model’s fit, 3) summary statistics of standardized residuals to assess unidimensionality and
convergent validity, 4) modification indices to assess convergent validity, and 5) completely standardized
solutions to check the factor loadings. We depended on these criteria to determine whether an item should
123
be dropped. The items that were supposed to measure the dimension of cognitive development had very
low factor loadings and they were loading highly on other dimensions. In addition, the same items had
negative parameters’ estimates. Based on that, we decided to drop that dimension and keep the other
three. After we dropped the cognitive development dimension, our chi-square dropped significantly and
our p-value was higher than before (it was zero initially). Some other items were dropped as well because
the previous criteria were consistent in pointing out that those items were problematic ones (problems
with unidimensionality). Most of the dropped items were the same ones that had to be reverse-coded. We
expected the reverse coded items to have low loadings because of the negative form of the question that
could trigger biased response. Should you need to refer to LISREL outputs for this stage, please check
those outputs attached to our paper. The 10 remaining items have provided us with an adequate fit of our
model to the data. Please refer to appendices C and D. Overall; we can say that my model is reasonably
fitting the data based on: a) non-significant chi-square value (50.23), b) goodness of fit index of .94, and
c) adjusted goodness of fit index of .91.
To assess convergent validity we relied on the factor loadings. Please refer to appendix C. We are
comfortable with the values that we had and we think that they could be considered significant since
almost all of them exceed 0.5. Hence, we assessed convergent validity successfully. We did my best not
to lose the face validity of our items as a price for better convergent validity.
We used the average variance extracted method (AVE) to assess discriminant validity. Based on the
values listed in appendix E, we can say that the remaining three dimensions are different from each other
but they still measure the construct of moral sensitivity.
The Measurement Model for Moral Sensitivity and Purchasing Involvement
In this stage, we had to add purchasing involvement to our model. Putrevu and Lord reported a coefficient
alpha of 0.72 for that scale but there was no enough evidence for a strong validity. Going through the
same steps, we had to assess convergent validity and discriminant validity of our items in the presence of
the new dimension. We found out that one of the new items has a very low loading on the purchasing
involvement dimension and we decided to drop that item. As shown in appendices C and D, we can see
that we have enough evidence for convergent validity (significant factor loadings) and discriminant
validity (AVE > .5 and > phi squared). The four dimensions in this model are distinct.
Reliability
We checked for the reliability of each of our dimensions using SPSS. As shown in appendix F, we do
have high reliability (more than 0.70) and we can say that the generated items are measuring what they
are supposed to measure.
The Structural Model
As we pointed out in our hypotheses, purchasing involvement is positively influenced by the four
dimensions of the moral sensitivity construct. Dropping one of the dimensions does not mean that our
hypothesis doesn’t hold. Rather, we think that with better measurement and better representation of the
items we could confirm its positive impact on the purchasing involvement construct. In this model,
purchasing involvement is the dependent variable and the three remaining dimensions are independent
variables. Appendix H has a figure that explains the relationships between the variables of this model. By
looking at LISREL estimates (GAMMA γ) we can confirm the positive relationship between purchasing
involvement and both awareness and commitment (Significant t-values). The hypothesized positive
relationship between purchasing involvement and self/social recognition was not supported (significant
negative t-value = -2.47). Please refer to appendix G. Using different items in the future to measure the
dimension of self and social recognition could confirm our hypothesis about the positive relationship
between this dimension and purchasing involvement. The correlations between the independent variables
(Φ) were significant. LISREL outputs are attached to this paper for further explanations. We maintained
124
an acceptable chi-square value with high goodness of fit index at p = 0.0051. Please refer to appendix D.
As a result, we can say that we have a reasonable nomological validity to support our hypotheses.
Checking the predictability of the model
We used multiple regression in order to check for the predictability of the model that we have proposed
earlier. We saved the factor scores of the factors that we have retained earlier into a regression equation
using SPSS. Then, we ran multiple regression with purchasing involvement being the dependent variable
and moral commitment, awareness, and recognition of social principles being the independent variables.
In addition, we checked for the assumptions of multiple regression. From the normal probability plot
(appendix J), we can see that we have a normal distribution. Also, the assumptions of linearity and equal
variance were met in our model (appendix L) and we assumed the assumption of the independence of
error-terms. As we expected, the three selected factors (dimensions) of moral sensitivity could
significantly predict the purchasing intention of customers (appendix I). For summary statistics and Rsquare values, please refer to Appendix I.
Discussion and Limitations
The ethical problems in marketing have been growing over the past decades. Part of the ethical problems
concerns the customers themselves and there are big differences between consumer ethics and marketers’
ethics (Vitell 1997). Customers usually have several ways of dealing with ethical concerns and dilemmas
and eventually those ways affect customers’ purchasing behavior (Shaw & Shiu 2000). In our study we
were trying to develop a scale to measure the construct of moral sensitivity of customers. We wanted to
link the construct of moral sensitivity to the customers’ purchasing involvement. We found that there is
indeed a significant relationship between the two constructs. A customer, who puts a lot of cognitive
effort and social concerns into his decision, will tend to be more involved in the purchasing process.
Although we ended up deleting many of our items, we can still say that we have reasonable and reliable
items to measure the construct of interest. Using a three-items scale to assess the nomological validity
didn’t help much in showing the hypothesized relationships. Furthermore, dropping an entire dimension
affected the other items in our scale and maybe because of that we needed to delete more items to reach
satisfactory values of chi-square and GFI. Embedding some of the dropped items in other dimensions
may lead to a better model especially if those items were loading highly on the other latent variables. We
used this approach with items from the cognitive development dimension but it didn’t improve our model
and we considered it as additional evidence that those items were not suitable for our model. In addition,
we understand that our scenario might not be the best one to use due the affect of cultural and religion
background of the respondents. We knew that what might be acceptable for young Americans is not
necessarily acceptable for other students who are from less developed parts of the world or for students
with different religious beliefs. Those differences were discussed in many interesting cross-cultural
investigations (USA, Australia, Middle East, Asia, London, GCC countries, etc...) regarding the issue of
consumer ethics and moral judgments in different parts of the world (Vitell 2003, Swaidan et al 2004).
Other studies considered gender and age differences. As an example, some of these studies showed that
younger consumers tend to be less ethical than the older ones (Vitell, Lumpkin, and Rawwas 1991).
The predictability of our model was not very high as we expected but it is significant. We think that
maybe with more support from the literature, we could define more dimensions for measuring moral
sensitivity.
Suggestions for Future Research
Our study was very broad and it didn’t include any effects of different ages, different cultures/religions
and determinants of ethical beliefs and attitudes. One could not ignore the important role that religion
plays in shaping the moral beliefs of people. This role becomes more important when companies try to
expand business internationally (Mittelstaedt 2002). The effect of different religions in different parts of
125
the world could be very essential in the purchasing process (Mittelstaedt 2002). We suggest future focus
on the religion effects on the customers’ responses to marketing messages.
Also, in the field of marketing, researchers may want to investigate whether consumers are willing to pay
more prices to get products with more ethical attributes. In addition, further research is needed to measure
the cognitive development dimension. We suggest the use of different items to represent and measure this
dimension and investigate its influence on the purchasing involvement. Also, checking for gender effects
and exploring its influence on customers’ moral decision making could be a good topic for future
research.
In conclusion we can say that consumers now are more concerned with the impacts of their decisions on
the society and/or the environment. It is important for companies not to forget that their social
responsibility and to understand the ethical and moral concerns of their customers.
It is also critical to understand the degree to which consumers are affectively involved in the purchasing
process. Customers have morals and values that they want to protect and they use them for evaluating
purchasing situations. Companies must consider this aspect before designing or launching any marketing
activity.
Appendix A
Moral Sensitivity Construct with Four Dimensions
Moral sensitivity
Moral
Awareness
126
Moral
Commitment
Self/Social
Recognition
Cognitive
Development
Appendix B
Questionnaire
PLEASE SIGN AND RETURN WITH THE SURVEY.
Consent Form
I understand that this research involves Customer Decision Making.
I will be provided with information regarding buying sweaters and then will be asked to respond to
questions regarding how I make a decision. This task will take me on average 10 minutes to complete. I
have the opportunity to telephone the researcher with any questions that I may have.
No discomfort is anticipated except for possible boredom with the task.
The major benefits I will receive from participation in this research are increased knowledge of Consumer
Decision Making and increased familiarity with marketing research methods.
I understand that my answers will be held strictly confidential. Responses will only be presented in
aggregate form.
This research is under the supervision of Dr. Susan Kleiser. Dr. Kleiser’s office is room 629 in the
Business Building at the University of Texas at Arlington. Her phone number is (817) 272-2228. Please
feel free to contact Dr. Kleiser if you have any questions.
I hereby consent to participate in this research
and understand the above procedure.
Signature
________________________________________
Date
127
Thank you for participating in our research project. In the next few pages you will be asked to answer
some questions regarding the following scenario. Your response will be held in the strictest confidence
and will not be associated with your information.
Your favorite clothing store introduced a new line of, hand-made, Cashmere Sweaters and you’ve heard
in the news that this particular line of sweaters was made in one of the third world countries. You’ve also
heard that in such countries they let young children make these sweaters and they pay them less than the
minimum wage. You went to that store and you really liked these fine Cashmere sweaters.
(I) Given the above, please answer the following questions on a scale from 1 to 7
Strongly Disagree
1
Disagree
2
Somewhat Disagree
3
Neural Somewhat Agree Agree
4
5
6
1) ____ I know that by buying the cashmere sweaters I will be raising a moral issue
2) ____ I think that my family will be disappointed if I purchase the sweaters
3) ____ I will purchase one of the sweaters just because I like it (r)
4) ____ Purchasing the sweaters does not contradict my personal principles (r )
5) ____ I am comfortable telling my family that I am going to purchase one of those
sweaters. (r)
6) ____ I know the principle that I will be breaking when I buy one of those sweaters
7) ____ Before purchasing any product, I try to make sure that it follows my personal
principles
8) ____ When I take any decision, I give my morals and principles high priority
9) ____ Even if I think that purchasing one of the sweaters is morally wrong, I will still
buy it (r )
10)____ I don’t care of buying a product is morally right or wrong (r )
11) ____ I base all my decisions on my moral judgments
12)____ I behave morally most of the time
13)____ I believe that it is my responsibility to make sure that buying a product
is morally right
128
Strongly Agree
7
Strongly Disagree
1
Disagree
2
Somewhat Disagree
3
Neural Somewhat Agree Agree
4
5
6
Strongly Agree
7
14)____ I will feel good when I purchase one of those sweaters ( r)
15)____ I will be socially comfortable by buying one of those sweaters (r )
16)____ I will feel guilty when I buy one of those sweaters
17)____ I will feel ashamed if I buy one of those sweaters
18)____ I will buy one of those sweaters because they match my personal style (r )
19)____ I care about what people think of me when I buy one of those sweaters
20)____ I strongly believe in the principles that my family believe in
21)____ While growing up, I developed my own morals
22)____ My personal morals are somewhat different than what my parents taught me
23)____ When I live with people for a long time, I adapt to some of their principles
24)____ I am going to teach my children the same principles that my parents taught me
25)____ I believe that morals can change over time
26)____ Buying one of the sweaters follows my own principles
(II) On a scale from 1 to 7, please indicate your opinion about the purchasing decision making process in
general
27)____ Purchasing Decisions express one’s personality
28)____ Purchasing Decisions are based on a lot of feeling
29)____ Purchasing Decisions are based on looks, taste, touch, smell, or sounds
That completes our research. Thank you for your time and cooperation
Note: Any item followed by (r) was reverse coded
129
Appendix C
Final Moral Sensitivity Scale Items and Factor Loadings
Loading
Awareness
I think that my family will be disappointed if I purchase the sweaters
0.70
I will purchase one of the sweaters just because I like it
0.81
Purchasing the sweaters does not contradict my personal principles
0.72
I am comfortable telling my family that I am going to purchase one of those
sweaters.
0.90
Commitment
Before purchasing any product, I try to make sure that it follows my personal
principles
0.81
When I take any decision, I give my morals and principles high priority
0.48
I base all my decisions on my moral judgments
0.59
I believe that it is my responsibility to make sure that buying a product
is morally right
0.83
Self-Recognition
I will feel guilty when I buy one of those sweaters
0.90
I will feel ashamed if I buy one of those sweaters
0.96
Purchasing Involvement
Purchasing Decisions express one’s personality
0.75
Purchasing Decisions are based on a lot of feeling
0.76
Appendix D
Assessment of Fit
Measurement Model for Moral Sensitivity
Chi-Square (32 Degrees of freedom)
Goodness of Fit Index (GFI)
Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index (AGFI)
Root Mean Square Residual (RMR)
Root Mean Square Error (RMSEA)
130
50.23 (P = 0.021)
0.94
0.91
0.14
0.055
Structure Equation Model (Moral Sensitivity and Purchasing Involvement)
Chi-Square (48 Degrees of freedom)
Goodness of Fit Index (GFI)
Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index (AGFI)
Root Mean Square Residual (RMR)
RMSEA
76.91 (P = 0.0051)
0.93
0.89
0.15
0. 058
Appendix E
Discriminant Validity
Φ2
Average Variance Extracted
Awareness * Commitment
0.6239
0.48
Awareness * Self-Recognition
0.6239
0.865
Commitment * Self-Recognition
0.48
0.865
Awareness * Purchasing Involvement
0.6239
0.57
Commitment * Purchasing Involvement
0.48
0.57
Self-Recognition * Purchasing Involvement
0.865
0.57
0.194
0.504
0.1521
0.0441
0.1296
0.0
Appendix F
Reliability
Coefficient Alpha
Awareness
Commitment
Self-Recognition
Purchasing Involvement
0.8637
0.7807
0.9269
0.7259
Appendix G
Nomological Validity (P-value < 0.05)
Parameter
Standardized Solution
γ11
0.31
γ12
0.37
γ13
-0.36
131
Appendix H
Final model after running SEM
X1
X2
Awareness
ξ1
ζ
γ11
X3
Y1
X4
X5
Commitment
γ12
ξ2
X6
Purchasing
Involvement
η1
γ13
X7
Y2
X8
X9
Self & Social
Recognition
ξ3
X10
Appendix I
Summary of the model’s statistics after running multiple regression
Factor
Awareness
Commitment
Self and social
recognition
Beta
Sig.
0.182 0.014
0.284
0
-0.134
0.07
R2
Adjusted-R2
F
Sig.
0.131
0.115
8.175
132
0
Appendix J
Graphs for the assumption checks
Linearity
Normal P-P Plot of Regression Stand
Dependent Variable: REGR factor sc
1.00
Expected Cum Prob
.75
.50
.25
0.00
0.00
.25
.50
.75
1.00
Observed Cum Prob
Normality
133
Histogram
Dependent Variable: REGR factor score
30
Frequency
20
10
Std. Dev = .99
Mean = 0.00
N = 166.00
0
25
2.
75
1.
25
1.
5
.7
5
.2
5
-.2
5
-.7
5
.2
-1
5
.7
-1
5
.2
-2
5
.7
-2
Regression Standardized Residual
Equal variance
Partial Regression Plot
REGR factor score 1 for analysis
2
Dependent Variable: REGR factor score 1 for
2
1
0
-1
-2
-3
-3
-2
-1
0
REGR factor score 1 for analysis
134
1
2
1
3
4
1 fo
REFERENCES
Andreasen, Alan R. (1982), “Judging Marketing in the 1980s,” Journal of Macromarketing (2) Spring, 713.
Antil, H, John (1984), “Socially Responsible Consumers: Profile and Implications for Public Policy,”
Journal of Macromarketing, (4) Fall, 18-39.
Auges, Burkee, Devinney, and Louviere (2003), “What Will Consumers Pay fpr Social Product
Features?” Journal of Business Ethics (42), 281-304.
Baron, J. & Spranca, M. (1997), “Protected Values,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, Vol. 70, 1-16.
Baron, J. (1999), “Consumer Attitude about Personal and Political Action,” Journal of Consumer
Psychology, Vol. 8(3), 261-275.
Bloom, Alfred H. (2001), “Two Dimensions pf Moral Reasoning: Social Principledness and Social
Humanism in Cross-Cultural Perspective,” The Journal of Social Psychology 101.
Bredemeier, B.J. & Shields, D.L. (1994), “Applied ethics and Moral Reasoning in Sport,” Moral
Development in the Professions, 173-187.
Cory, Rachel M. (1986): “Kohlberg’s Stages of Moral Development”, www.agalelia.com
Crain, W. C. (1985): “Theories of Development” Prentice-Hall pp. 118-136.
Dierchx, Roelens, and Gatsmans (1998), “An adjusted Version of Kohlberg’s Moral Theory, Discussion
of its Validity for Research in Nursing Ethics,” Journal of Advanced Nursing, 27.
Dixon, D. F. (1982), “The Ethical Component of Marketing: An Eighteenth-Century View,” Journal of
Macromarketing (2) Spring, 38-46.
Erwin, J.W. (2000), ”Supervisor Moral Sensitivity,” Counselor Education and Supervision, Vol. 40(2),
115-128
Ferrell, O.C. and Larry Gresham (1985) "A Contingency Framework for Understanding Ethical Decision
Making in Marketing," Journal of Marketing, 49(Summer), 87-96.
Ferrell, O. C., Larry G. Gresham, and John Fraedrich (1989), “A Synthesis of Ethical Decision Models
for Marketing,” Journal of Macromarketing, (9) Fall, 55-64.
Fukukawa Kyoko (2003), “A theoretical Review of Business and Consumer Ethics Research: Normative
and descriptive Approaches,” The marketing Review (3), 381-401
Gray, Mel (1996), “Moral Theory for Social Work,” The Journal of Social Work, Volume 32(4).
Hackley, E. C. (1999), “The Meaning of Ethics in and of Advertising,” Business Ethics: A European
View, Vol. 8(1), 37-42.
135
Hunt, S.D. & Vitell, S.J. (1986), “A general Theory of Marketing Ethics,” Journal of Macromarketing,
Vol. 8 (Spring), 5-16.
John D. Mittelstaedt (2002), “A Framework for Understanding the Relationship between Religions and
Markets,” Journal of Macromarketing, Vol.22 No.1 (June), 6-18.
Jones, T.M. (1991), “Ethical Decision Making by Individuals in Organizations, an Issue-Contingent
Model,” Academy of Management Review, Vol. 16 (April), 366-395.
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D.
A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347-480). Chicago: Rand
McNally.
Laczniak, Gene., Robert F. Lusch, and William A. Strang (1981), “Ethical Marketing Perceptions of
Economic Goods and Social Problems,” Journal of Macromarketing (1) Spring, 49-57.
Lovecky, V.D.(1997), “Identity Development in Gifted Children, Moral Sensitivity,” Roeper Review,
Vol. 20(Dec), 90-94.
McCarthy, E. (1981), “Basic Marketing,” Homewood, Illinois: Ritchard D. Irwin
MacDonald, Chris (2002), “Moral Decision Making- An Analysis,” www.ethicsweb.ca. (2002).
MacDonald, Chris (2002), “A guide to Moral Decision Making,” www.ethicsweb.ca.(2002)
Mittelstaedt, John D. (2002), “A Framework for Understanding the Relationships between Religions and
Markets,” Journal of Macromarketing, (22) June, 6-18.
Mudrack, Peter E. (2003), “The Untapped Relevance of Moral Development Theory in the Study of
Business Ethics,” Journal of Business Ethics (42), 225-236.
Myyry, L. & Helkama, K. (2002), “The Role of Value Priorities and Professional Ethics Training in
Moral Sensitivity,” Journal of Moral Education, Vol. 31(1), 35-50
Putrevu and Lord (1994), “Comparative and Noncomparative Advertising, Attitudinal Affects Under
Cognitive and Affective Involvement Conditions,” JA 23, (June), 77-90.
Ratchford, Brian T. (1987), “New Insights About the FBC Grid,” JAR, 27 (4), 24-38.
Rest. J.R. (1984), “Research on Moral Development, Implications for Training Counseling
Psychologists,” The Counseling Psychologist, (12), 19-29.
Ringlod, D. J. (1998), “A comment on the Political Council for Social Communications’ Ethics in
Advertising,” Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, Vol. 17(2), 332-335.
Shaw and Shiu (2000), “The Impact of Ethics in Consumer Choice, a Multivariate Modeling Case Study,”
ANZMAC Visionary Marketing for the 21st Century.
136
Shaw and Shiu (2002), “An Assessment of Ethical Obligation and Self-Identity in Ethical Consumer
Decision-Making, a Structural Equation Modeling Approach,” International Journal of
Consumer Studies (26), December.
Singhapakdi, Ansusorn and Scott J. Vitell (1990), “Marketing Ethics: Factors Influencing Perceptions of
Ethical Problems and Alternatives,” Journal of Macromarketing, (10) Spring, 4-18.
Singhapakdi, A.; Vitell, S.J. & Kraft, K.L. (1996), “Moral Intensity and Ethical Decision Making of
Marketing Professionals,” Journal of Business Research, Vol. 36 (July), 245-255.
Singhapakdi, A.; Rawwas, M.; Marta, J. & Ahmed, M. (1999), “A Cross-Cultural Study of Consumer
Perceptions about Marketing Ethics,” Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 16(3), 257-272.
Singhapakdi, Vitell, Rae, and Kurtz (1999), “Ethics Gap, Comparing Marketers with Consumers in
Important Determinants of Ethical Decision-Making,” Journal of Business Ethics (21) 317-328.
Swaidan, Z., Mohammad Y. Rawwas, and Jamal Al-Khatib (2004), “Consumer Ethics: Moral Ideologies
and Ethical Beliefs of Micro-Culture in the US,” International Business Review (13) 749-761.
Taylor, P.W. (1975), “Principles of Ethics, An Introduction,” Dickenson, Encino, CA.
Vitell, S. J., J. R. Lumpkin, and M. Y. A. Rawwas (1991), “Consumer Ethics: An Investigation of Ethical
Beliefs of Elderly Consumers,” Journal of Business Ethics (May), 365-375.
Vitell and Ho (1997), “Ethical Decision Making in Marketing, A synthesis and Evaluation of Scales
Measuring the Various Components of Decision Making in Ethical Situations,” Journal of
Business Ethics (16), 699-717.
Vitell, Scott J. (2003): “Consumer Ethics Research, Review, Synthesis and Suggestions for the Future,”
Journal of Business Ethics, 43: 33-47.
Vitell, S. J., Anusorn Singhapadki, and James Thomas (2001), “Consumer Ethics, an Application and
Empirical Testing of the Hunt-Vitell theory of Ethics,” Journal of Consumer Marketing,” Vol.
18, NO. 2, 153-178.
Volker, J.M. (1984): Counseling Experience, Moral Judgment, Awareness of Consequences and Moral
Sensitivity in Counseling Practice. Ann Arbor, MI: IMI Dissertation Services.
Woodstock Business Conference: Moral Decision Making (PART I), (1999).
Website Links
http://www.fccc.edu/ethics/Glossary_of_Ethics_Terms.html
(2004)
WWW.PAYTONPAPERS.ORG (2000)
Acknowledgements
Dr. Susan Kleiser, Marketing
Dr. Wendy Casper, Management
The University of Texas at Arlington
137
Compulsive Buying and Credit Card Use Among Young
Adults
Tânia Modesto Veludo-de-Oliveira, University of São Paulo, Brazil
Ana Akemi Ikeda, University of São Paulo. Brazil
Rubens da Costa Santos, Fundação Getúlio Vargas
Abstract
The objective of this article is to analyze aspects of compulsive buying behavior among young adults.
Bibliographical and document research studies as well as a survey with Brazilian university students
were conducted. The instrument of data collection was based on Faber and O’Guinn (1992) and Roberts
and Jones (2001) scales of which 408 questionnaires were considered for analysis. Results suggest that
young adults who have propensity for compulsive buying are heavy credit card users.
Introduction
The debating of compulsive buying on young adults´ behavior is not yet consolidated. Studies, such as
those indicated by O’Guinn and Faber (1989), D’Astous, Maltais and Roberge (1990) and Schlosser et al.
(1994) show that this group has high propensity to develop an uncontrollable urge to spend. People with
this profile are at risk of taking this bad habit developed during adolescence throughout their lifetime. The
use of credit cards, due to the convenience that it provides when shopping can affect young consumers´
behavior.
Faber and O’Guinn (1992) and O’Guinn and Faber (1989) found estimates that compulsive buying affects
between 1.1% to 5.9% of the USA population. In spite of be considered a “global phenomenon” (Kwak,
Zinkhan, and Crask 2003, 161), the literature provides little background in relation to the occurrence of
this kind of behavior in other countries. This paper advances our knowledge by looking at a different
culture and how it may be similar/ different to the USA, where most of the research has been conducted.
Therefore, the overall objective of this study is to analyze aspects of compulsive buying behavior among
young adults in Brazil, considering credit card use.
As regards to specific objectives, using the Faber and O’Guinn (1992) scale, we sought to identify: (i) the
score of young adults classified as compulsive buyers, (ii) minimum and maximum levels of compulsivity
in the behavior of surveyed young adults, and (iii) the groups with high and low propensity to compulsive
buying. Using Roberts and Jones (2001) scale, we intend to assess the intensity in credit card use by the
surveyed young adults. Finally, as a result of the analysis of the two mentioned scales, we intend to assess
the behavior related to credit card use of young adults who have more propensity to compulsivity,
including those classified as compulsive buyers. Both scales are explained in the research method topic.
THE CONCEPT OF COMPULSIVE BUYING
One behavior is considered compulsive when it derives from compelled and impelled impulses, becoming
inappropriate or harmful to an individual (Ullman and Krasner 1969). Impulse takes on the nature of
compulsion when it becomes a powerful urge which is irresistible (Rook 1987) and can occur with
consumers whose feelings are out of control (Woodruffe-Burton, Eclles, and Elliott 2002). Based on a
study of consumption impulse formation, Dholakia (2000) states that the existence of explicit rules acts as
an impulse control strategy, guiding and controlling behaviors associated to addiction or to compulsive
consumption. McElroy, Phillips and Keck (1994b) argue that compulsive buying has been almost
138
exclusively understood as an impulse-control disorder and that “has both cognitive and behavioural
components, each potentially causing impairment” (Black 2001, 18).
Compulsive buying has been subject for research studies in the field of psychiatry, psychology and
marketing. This topic was originally described by psychiatry in the early 1900s by Kraepelin (1915) and
Bleuler (1924). Kraepelin (1915) defined it as a pathological impulse and Bleuler (1924) as a reactive
impulse. Despite seminal studies on the theme conducted in the beginning of the 20th century, “it was not
until the late 1980’s that compulsive buying began to receive much attention” (Magee 1994, 590). In
marketing, specifically in the consumer behavior area, the preliminary studies focusing on this topic
emerged around the 1980s (Faber and O’Guinn 1988; Moschis and Churchill 1978; O’Guinn and Faber
1989; Valence, D’Astous, and Fortier 1988), approximately 60 years after a clinical approach to the
theme was presented.
McElroy, Phillips and Keck (1994b) define compulsive buying as the uncontrollable, markedly
distressing, time-consuming and/or resulting in family, social, vocational, and/or financial difficulties.
Overall, it is possible to define it as “chronic, repetitive purchasing that becomes a primary response to
negative events or feelings” (O’Guinn and Faber 1989, 155) – definition adopted in this article.
Compulsive buying behavior manifests itself in different levels of intensity, in which its maximum level
is characterized by addicted buyers. Regarding this issue, Edwards (1993) suggests that the
characterization of buying behavior lies on a continuum that includes normal impulsive consumer
behavior at one extreme and deviant addictive behavior at the other.
The Compulsive Buyer Behavior
Frost et al. (1998) consider compulsive buying as an obsessive-compulsive behavior which in turn, can be
associated to other disorders such as alcoholism (Glatt and Cook 1987; Valence, D’Astous, and Fortier
1988), kleptomania (Mc Elroy et al. 1991), gambling (Schlosser et al. 1994) and eating disorders (Faber et
al. 1995). However, such behavior was not included in the last issue of DSM – Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders, American Psychiatric Association, 1994 –, as Miltenberger et al. (2003)
explain. Hence, it should be highlighted that “although some cases of uncontrolled buying can be
described as a disorder, most buying impulses are normal” (Lejoyex et al. 1996, 1525) and, for this
reason, compulsive buying should be understood in all levels.
Faber and Christenson (1996) suggest that compulsive buying behavior is used as a compensatory
response to a frustration or depression, as an attempt to manage mood and even overcome unhappiness.
They comment that compulsive buyers try to counteract feelings of low self-esteem through the emotional
lift and momentary euphoria provided by compulsive shopping. Likewise, negative emotions are
experienced by this group the same way or even more frequent prior to than during the shopping
experience. This means that “negative emotions, especially sadness, loneliness, anger, frustration, and
irritation increase uncontrolled buyers’ propensity to buy” (Lejoyeux et al. 1996, 1526) and that generally
the purchasing activity is associated to positive emotions: while buying the individual feels happy,
powerful, competent, relaxed or superior (Rook 1987). However, the possession of the product is not
capable of regulating some inner sense of emotional emptiness derived from negative feelings and can
intensify it because the tension, temporarily mitigated by a purchase, is rapidly transferred to pangs of
guilt (Christenson et al. 1994; Lejoyeux et al. 1996; Valence, D’Astous, and Fortier 1988).
Elliott´s research (1994) confirms that the tendency to addictive consumption is related to insecurities,
childhood traumas, the fact the person feels uninteresting, depression and lack of emotional support in
relationships. Scherhorn, Reish and Raab (1990) emphasize the low level of self-esteem and high level of
depression in compulsive buyers. Corroborating this view, Edwards´work (1993) showed that the
compulsive buyer presents a higher level of anxiety than the non-compulsive buyer and that s/he makes
purchases especially during stressful periods. Nataraajan and Goff (1991) explain that this can be a way
139
that shy or people preoccupied with their family let off their aggression. Faber and Christenson (1996)
point out the desire for stimulation, the high propensity for fantasies of imagined wealth and the desire to
be recognized as characteristics of these buyers who can be influenced by psychological, sociological and
biochemical factors.
Roberts (1998) state that peer pressure, television viewing, frequency of shopping, as well as credit card
use and its easy access can influence compulsive buyer´s behavior. “Credit cards, cash machines, instant
credit, home shopping networks, and advertising facilitate and incite the purchasing of items on impulse”
(Lejoyeux et al. 1996, 1525), where culture, commercial environment, and advertising are socio-cultural
factors that contribute to compulsive shopping (Valence, D’Astous, and Fortier 1988). It is noteworthy
that “the compulsive group was shown to react more favorably than the other group to image-oriented
ads” (D’Astous, Maltais, and Roberge, 1990, 306).
Bloch, Ridgeway and Dawson (1994) analyzed the impacts of shopping malls on consumer behavior and
noticed that some negative influences are present. The shopping mall reinforces materialistic values by
focusing in only one site the display of many products associated to “good life”. In their reports, some
consumers tell that they feel that products sometimes seem to be luring them, provoking buyers to take
possession of the product (Rook 1987). For this reason, the shopping mall environment tends to promote
relapses on susceptible compulsive buyers, the same way a casino does on a compulsive gambler (Block,
Ridgeway, and Dawson 1994).
Shoham and Brencic (2003) advert that consumers´ tendency to make unplanned purchases, and their
tendency to buy products not on shopping lists, serve to predict compulsive tendencies. Gender is also
pointed out as an antecedent of this behavior, where women are the main victims. This finding regarding
gender is consistent with the works of D’Astous, Maltais and Roberge (1990), Faber and O’Guinn (1992),
McElroy et al. (1994a), Black (1996), Woodruffe (1997), Hirschman and Stern (2001), and WoodruffeBurton, Eccles and Elliott (2002).
Black (1996), Elliott, Eccles and Gournay (1996), Boedecker, Morgan and Stoltman (1999), and Roberts
and Jones (2001) highlight indebtness, personal bankruptcies, legal problems, personal stress, credit card
debts, and relationship problems as frequent consequences of compulsive buying. Christenson et al.
(1994) present some estimates regarding this issue: the aggravation leads to huge debt amounts (58.3%),
failure to pay debts (41.7%), legal and financial consequences (8.3%), and criminal problems (8.3%).
The Compulsive Buyer and Credit Card Use
Some research has emphasized the relationship between compulsive buyer and credit card use. Faber and
O’Guinn (1988), for example, believe that some people with credit problems present buying patterns
similar to compulsive buyers. The authors point out that when respondents referred to credit card usage,
they naturally associated it with feelings of empowerment and importance (13.6%) and spending more
than they could afford (27.3%).
Roberts (1998) believes that, when compared to cash, the credit card promotes spending: the money
involved in credit card transactions is abstract or unreal, that is, intangible for the individual who ends up
overspending. The compulsive buyer is more likely to own more credit cards than other consumers (Black
2001; Christenson et al. 1994; O’Guinn and Faber 1989). D’Astous (1990), in turn, argues: the fact that
credit cards eliminate the immediate need for money to buy something likely accelerate the development
of compulsive buying.
Emphasizing the young adult segment, Warwick and Mansfield (2000) surveyed 381 undergraduates and
graduates, in order to check their knowledge regarding credit card use. Most of the respondents seemed to
140
ignore the interest rates charged by credit cards, and approximately half of the respondents were unaware
of their own credit card limit.
There is evidence of the relationship between compulsive buying behavior among young adults and credit
card use in a study conducted by Roberts and Jones (2001). Researchers sought to establish in a model the
relationship among (i) compulsive buying, (ii) buyer’s attitudes relating to money, and (iii) credit card
use. College students’ behavior was analyzed and it was concluded that the credit card acts as a mediating
variable between buyer’s attitudes and compulsive buying.
RESEARCH METHOD
In order to complement the theoretical review, a survey was conducted with students who were credit
card users during 2003 and 2004. As a matter of convenience, the empirical research collected
information from a non-probabilistic sample in São Paulo City (Brazil). The questionnaires were
administered in Business classrooms from different colleges. Of the 590 questionnaires answered, 408
were considered for analysis. The mean age of the sample was 23.6 years old, 165 or 40.4% were female
and 243 or 59.6% were male.
The questionnaire consisted of two parts. The first part was based on Faber and O’Guinn (1992) scale,
also known as “Diagnostic Screener for Compulsive Buying” (DSCB). The DSCB “is now considered an
important tool by researchers in identifying and diagnosing compulsive buying” (Black 2001, 20), and
since its development it has been utilized in different investigations (Faber and Christenson 1996; Faber
et al. 1995; Kwak, Zinkhan, and Crask 2003; Lyi, Lee, and Kwak 1997a,b; Miltenberger et al. 2003;
Rindfleisch, Burroughs, and Denton 1997; Roberts and Jones 2001; Roberts and Sepulveda 1999; Rook
and Fisher 1995). The DSCB comprises 7 questions based on a Likert-type scale with 5 points. In
question 1a, the respondent has to choose among the alternatives: strongly agree (1), somewhat agree (2),
neither agree nor disagree (3), somewhat disagree (4) and strongly disagree (5). In questions 2a to 2f, the
options are: very often (1), often (2), sometimes (3), rarely (4) and never (5). DSCB questions are
described as follows (Faber and O’Guinn 1992):
Q.1a) If you have any money left at the end of the pay period, I just have to spend it.
Q.2a) Felt others would be horrified if they knew of my spending habits.
Q.2b) Bought things even though I couldn’t afford them.
Q.2c) Wrote a check when I knew I didn’t have enough money in the bank to cover it.
Q.2d) Bought myself something in order to make myself feel better.
Q.2f) Felt anxious or nervous on days I didn’t go shopping.
Q. 7) Made only the minimum payments on my credit card.
The answers (and respective scores) are calculated using the equation proposed by the DSCB: -9.69 +
(Q.1a x 0.33) + (Q.2a x 0.34) + (Q.2b x 0.50) + (Q.2c x 0.47) + (Q.2d x 0.33) + (Q.2e x 0.38) + (Q.2f x
0.31) (for more details regarding the use of the equation, please see Faber and O’Guinn 1992). A subject
who scores less than -1.34 is classified as a compulsive buyer. The maximum score, namely, the highest
level of compulsive buying that can be obtained in the DSCB is -7.03. Analogously, the minimum level
of compulsive buying that can be obtained in the DSCB is 3.61.
This article also analyzes credit card use (second part of the questionnaire), based on Roberts and Jones
(2001) scale. This scale consists of a series of 12 questions. The respondent should select his/her level of
agreement with the sentences, choosing one of the following alternatives: strongly agree (1), somewhat
agree (2), neither agree nor disagree (3), somewhat disagree (4) and strongly disagree (5). The items on
credit card use are (Roberts and Jones 2001):
141
Q.1) My credit cards are usually at their maximum credit limit.
Q.2) I frequently use available credit on one credit to make a payment on another credit
card.
Q.3) I always pay off my credit cards at the end of each month. (R)
Q.4) I worry how I will pay off my credit card debt.
Q.5) I often make only the minimum payment on my credit card bills.
Q.6) I am less concerned with the price of a product when I use a credit card.
Q.7) I am more impulsive when I shop with credit cards.
Q.8) I spend more when I use a credit card.
Q.9) I am seldom delinquent in making payments on my credit cards. (R)
Q.10) I rarely go over my available credit limit. (R)
Q.11) I seldom take cash advances on my credit cards. (R)
Q.12) I have too many credit cards.
Someone scoring high on this scale could be viewed as using credit cards irresponsibly; Q.3, Q.9, Q.10
and Q.11 were reverse (R) scored to reflect this coding scheme.
Considering the level of compulsive buying obtained by the respondents on Faber and O’Guinn (1992)
scale, the sample was split into 4 parts (quartiles), each one containing 102 respondents with similar
scores. Of the 408 questionnaires, the answers from the 102 young adults included in the quartile with a
score closer to the compulsive buying limit was analyzed more carefully. In the group of these 102 young
adults, 24 were identified as compulsive buyers, since they scored less than -1.34 on the scale.
Respondents’ considerations over the use of credit card were analyzed in a comparative way. The results
of the survey were analyzed and compared to analyses from bibliographical and document research
studies, subsidizing the final considerations and parallels with literature.
RESULTS
In this study, 408 answers from young students were analyzed as well as credit card users. Nonetheless,
among them, more focused attention was given to the 102 young adults who belonged to the group that
had the score closer to compulsivity limit, according to Faber and O’Guinn (1992) scale, where the 24
students classified as compulsive buyers are included.
Figures 1 and 2 illustrate the sample distribution of compulsive buying behavior and the level of intensity
in credit card use among college students.
142
250
200
150
100
-1.34
50
-7.03
n um ber o f st uden t s
FIGURE 1
Compulsive Buying Behavior Among College Students (Faber and O’Guinn 1992 scale)
1.68
0.66
2.47
3.61
0
maximum level of compuls ivity
minimum level of compuls ivity
Figure 1 shows a high concentration of respondents on the right hand side of the normal curve, indicating
that students’ behavior is predominantly non-compulsive.
The -7.03 value corresponds to the extreme compulsive buying behavior, and 3.61 corresponds to the
lowest level of compulsive buying; the median found was 1.68. This means that the 1.68 score splits the
sample into two equal parts and that 204 respondents are in the range of 0.66 to 2.47. Young adults with
less propensity for compulsive buying are in the 2.47 quartile (ranging from 2.47 to 3.61), totaling 102
students.
Eleven students scored 3.61, namely, the lowest level of compulsive buying on the scale. College students
that are found in the 0.66 quartile (ranging from 0.66 to –7.03) present the highest indications of
compulsive buying, totaling 102 people. Young adults classified as compulsive buyers, consequently who
scored less than –1.34, are found in this quartile.
Compulsive behavior was identified in twenty four students with the following scores: -1.35, -1.38, -1.39,
-1.42, -1.49, -1.55, -1.56 (this one with three students), -1.59, -1.67, -1.93, -1.95, -1.97, -2.05, -2.55, 2.66, -2.69, -2.72, -2.81, -2.86, -3.52, -3.61 and -4.35.
Roberts and Jones (2001) scale is based on a Likert scale, where 1 represents the highest level in credit
card use and 5 the lowest. In their investigation, the authors utilized the average to assess results obtained
(despite being an ordinal scale), finding a 2.4 average. In the present investigation, a 4.11 average was
found, which means that in comparison to the North-American student sample, Brazilian students show a
less intense and more controlled use in credit cards. It is noteworthy that, comparing to North-American
reality, Brazilian culture on credit card is in relative infancy, since this service is still in its emergent
stage. In Brazil, not all commercial establishments are structurally organized to accept credit cards and
only recently people are having access to it.
Figure 2 shows results from the research and indicates a predominance of students that use credit card
responsibly and even sparingly. Of the total 408 respondents, twenty three scored 5, disclosing the lowest
level of intensity in card use. These participants are practically not affected by the benefits this instrument
provides when shopping. None of the respondents scored 1, which refers to the maximum level of
intensity of credit card use; the score that corresponds to the highest level of the sample is 2.25, obtained
by a young consumer whose behavior is more compulsive-oriented (ranging from 0.66 to -7.03 in Figure
1). The median found was 4.16, that splits the sample into two equal parts, where the 3.75 quartile
(ranging from 3.75 to 1) refers to the group that are heavy credit card users.
143
250
200
150
100
n um ber o f st uden t s
FIGURE 2
Level of intensity in credit card use by college students (Roberts and Jones 2001 scale)
4.16
3.75
4.58
50
1
2.25
5
0
maximum
intens ity of card
minimum
intens ity of card
Some parallels among the results presented in Figures 1 and 2 merit to be highlighted. Of the 102 students
that are in the 0.66 quartile of graphic 1 (Faber and O’Guinn 1992 scale), 61 are also in the 3.75 quartile
of graphic 2 (Roberts and Jones 2001 scale). This means that 61 of the 102 young adults who have more
propensity for compulsive buying belong to the group that are heavy credit card users. This observation
reinforces the researchers´ (Black 2001; Christenson et al. 1994; D’Astous 1990; Faber and O’Guinn
1988; O’Guinn and Faber 1989; Roberts 1998; Roberts and Jones 2001) idea that associate compulsive
buying to heavy use of credit cards. Analogously, it was noted that 19 of the 24 compulsive buyers are
heavy credit card users (they are in the 3.74 quartile in Figure 2), where one of the compulsive students
declared to own 4 cards and other three declared to own 3 cards. Further information is shown on Table 1.
TABLE 1
Parallel among college students´ buying behavior regarding compulsive buying
Students whose behavior
Students whose behavior
is more compulsiveis not compulsive-oriented
oriented
(3 quartiles)
(1 quartile)
306
102
Surveyed young adults
(75% of a total of 408
(25% of a total of 408
young adults)
young adults)
Gender (Female)
107
58
Gender (Male)
199
44
Average age (years)
24.12
22.37
Average number of credit
1.280
1.509
cards per capita
Pay their own credit card
201
81
bill
Co-pay card debt with
71
7
parents
Parents pay credit card debt 34
14
Young adults that are heavy
36
61
credit card users
* Sub-sample of previous column
144
Students classified as
compulsive buyers*
24
(5.88% of a total of 408
young adults)
12
12
21.50
1.708
22
1
1
19
FINAL CONSIDERATIONS
The credit card has become popular in numerous segments of the population, and consequently, bringing
about changes in the behavior of individuals that so far didn´t have access to this service. It is noted that,
at the same time that credit card expands, there is an increasing number of debtors and indebted people,
namely, people who buy more than they can afford. The uncontrollable use of credit cards has been
pointed out as a problem of young people who in order to revenge on the lack of parental care, go for an
expensive shopping spree (Lejoyeux et al. 1996) or due to their inexperience, are easily lured and end up
with mounting credit card debts.
The credit card, as Roberts and Jones (2001) conclude, act as mediator between compulsive buying and
buyer´s attitudes. This article´s survey reinforces the authors´ argument, since it is possible to note that
young people with scores closer to the level of compulsive buying are, in majority, the ones that declare
being heavy credit card users.
The credit card provides a higher level of comfort in shopping. It is possible that young people with a
compulsive shopping profile will buy without taking into account the economic or utilitarian value of
their purchase. They probably use shopping and spending to try to feel better and to reduce anxiety as
O’Guinn and Faber (1989), Edwards (1993), Faber and Christenson (1996), Lejoyeux et al. (1996) and
Woodruffe (1997) note. Perhaps these young people have found in the shopping activity, along with
credit card use, a way to mask their difficulties.
The organizations that are related to this problem, such as credit card issuers, the card industry that
benefits from them, banks and other consumer protection and defense agencies, as well as educators,
family and users themselves, should discuss responsibilities of all parties involved. Indeed, many or every
link in the chain play relevant and specific roles in guiding and counseling all participants, either
intermediary or final users.
The results of this exploratory research enable some recommendations to be made in order to stimulate
reflection upon this issue and the responsibility of every link in the chain. We do not intend to have the
solution of the issue that already gave rise to numerous debates, but rather bring to surface social
controversy involving entrepreneurs, families and credit card users. Hence, we believe that each one of
these agents and institutions should design communication activities, in order to increase awareness of
users regarding the responsible use of credit card, presenting the numerous advantages that they provide
and warning about associated risks and situations researched in this study. Furthermore, these agents can
promote in an integrated way, credit card education programs, control policies and counseling services
targeted at credit card users who present typical symptoms of compulsive levels in all levels.
As recommendations for future research studies on this topic, we suggest the development of
investigations of institutions so as to continually assess their posture in the face of compulsive buying.
Additionally, it would be interesting to promote a better understanding of the impact of cultural factors on
the compulsive buyer. Finally, research studies based on probabilistic sampling would be welcome so as
to extend the initial contributions on this study.
145
REFERENCES
Black, D. 2001. Compulsive buying disorder: definition, assessment, epidemiology and clinical
management. CNS Drugs 15 (1): 17-27.
Black, D. 1924. Compulsive buying: a review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 57: 50-54.
Bleuler, E. Textbook of psychiatry. New York: MacMillan.
Bloch, P.; Ridgeway, N.; Dawson, S. 1994. The shopping mall as consumer habitat. Journal of Retailing
70 (1): 23-24.
Boedecker, K.; Morgan, F.; Stoltman, J. 1999. Excessive consumption: marketing and legal perspectives.
American Business Law Journal 36 (Winter): 301-325.
Christenson, G.; Faber, R.; Zwann, M.; Raymond, N.; Specker, S.; Ekern, M.; Mackenzie, T.; Crosby, R.;
Crow, S.; Eckert, E.; Mussell, M.; Mitchell, J. 1994. Compulsive buying: descriptive
characteristics and psychiatric comorbidity. Journal of Clinical Psychiatric 55: 5-11.
D’Astous, A. 1990. An inquire into the compulsive side of ‘normal’ consumers. Journal of Consumer
Policy 13: 15-31.
D’Astous, A.; Maltais, J.; Roberge, C. 1990. Compulsive buying tendencies of adolescents consumers.
Advances in Consumer Research 17: 306-312.
Dholakia, U. M. 2000. Temptation and resistance: an integrated model of consumption impulse formation
and enactment. Psychology & Marketing 17 (11): 955-968.
Edwards, E. 1993. Development of a new scale for measuring compulsive buying behavior. Financial
Counseling and Planning 4: 67-84.
Elliott, R.; Eccles, S.; Gournay, K. 1996. Revenge, existencial choice and, addictive consumption.
Psychology & Marketing 13 (December): 753-768.
Elliott, R. 1994. Addictive consumption: function and fragmentations in postmodernity. Journal of
Consumer Policy 17 (2): 159-179.
Faber, R.; Christenson, G. 1996. In the mood to buy: differences in the mood states experienced by
compulsive buyers and other consumers. Psychology & Marketing 13 (December): 803-819.
Faber, R.; Christenson, G.; Zwaan, M.; Mitchell, J. 1995. Two forms to compulsive consumption:
comorbidity of compulsive buying and binge eating. Journal of Consumer Research 22 (3): 296304.
Faber. R.; O’Guinn, T. 1992. A clinical screener for compulsive buyer. Journal of Consumer Research 19
(December): 459-469.
Faber. R.; O’Guinn, T. 1988. Compulsive consumption and credit abuse. Journal of Consumer Policy 11
(March): 97-109.
146
Frost, R.; Kim, H.; Morris, C.; Bloss, C.; Murray-Close, M.; Steketee, G. 1998. Hoarding, compulsive
buying and reasons for saving. Behaviour Research and Therapy 36: 657-664.
Glatt, M.; Cook, C. 1987. Pathological spending as a form of psychological dependence. British Journal
of Addiction 82 (November): 1257-1258.
Hirschman, E.; Stern, B. 2001. Do consumers’ genes influence their behavior? Findings on novelty
seeking and compulsive consumption. Advances in Consumer Research 28: 403-410.
Kwak, H.; Zinkhan, G.; Crask, M. 2003. Diagnostic Screener for Compulsive Buying: applications to the
USA and South Korea. The Journal of Consumer Affairs 37 (Summer): 161-169.
Kraepelin, E. 1915. Psychiatrie. 8th ed. Leipzig: Verlag Von Johann Ambrosius Barth.
Lejoyeux, M.; Ades, J.; Tassain, V.; Solomon, J. 1996. Phenomenology and psychopathology of
uncontrolled buying. Journal of Psychiatric Association 153 (12): 1524-1529.
Lyi, D.; Lee, S.; Kwak, H. 1997a. Demographic trait and shopping attitude: impulsive buying and
compulsive buying. Korean Journal of Advertising Research 36 (Fall): 127-137.
Lyi, D.; Lee, S.; Kwak, H. 1997b. Trait aspects of impulsive buying behavior. Korean Journal of
Advertising Research 34 (Spring): 213-241.
Magee, A. 1994. Compulsive buying tendency as a predictor of attitudes and perceptions. Advances in
Consumer Research 21: 590-594.
Mc Elroy, S.; Pope, H.; Hudson, J.; Keck, P.; Smith, J.; White, K. 1991. Kleptomania: a report of twenty
cases. American Journal of Psychiatry 148 (May): 652-657.
Mc Elroy, S.; Keck, P.; Pope, H.; Smith, J.; Strakowski, S. 1994a. Compulsive buying: a report of 20
cases. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 55: 242-248.
Mc Elroy, S.; Phillips, K.; Keck, P. 1994b. Obsessive compulsive spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry 55: 33-51.
Miltenberger, R.; Redlin, J.; Crosby, R.; Stickney, M.; Mitchell, J.; Wonderlich, S.; Faber, R.; Smyth, J.
2003. Direct and retrospective assessment of factors contributing to compulsive buying. Journal
of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 34: 1-9.
Moschis, G.; Churchill, G. 1978. Consumer socialization: a theoretical and empirical analysis. Journal of
Marketing (Summer): 40-48.
Nataraajan, R.; Goff, B. 1991. Compulsive buying: toward a reconceptualization. Journal of Social
Behavior and Personality 6: 307-328.
O’Guinn, T.; Faber, R. 1989. Compulsive buying: a phenominological exploration. Journal of Consumer
Research 16 (September): 147-157.
Rindfleisch, A.; Burroughs, J.; Denton, F. 1997. Family structure, materialism, and compulsive
consumption. Journal of Consumer Research 23 (March): 312-325.
147
Roberts, J.; Jones, E. 2001. Money attitudes, credit card use, and compulsive buying among American
college students. The Journal of Consumer Affairs 35 (Winter): 213-241.
Roberts, J.; Sepulveda, C. 1999. Money attitudes and compulsive buying: an exploratory investigation of
the emerging consumer culture in Mexico. Journal of International Consumer Marketing 11 (4):
53-74.
Roberts, J. 1998. Compulsive buying among college students: an investigation of its antecedents,
consequences, and implications for public policy. Journal of Consumer Affairs 32 (Winter): 295308.
Rook, D.; Fisher, R. 1995. Normative influences on impulsive buying behavior. Journal of Consumer
Research. 22 (December): 305-313.
Rook, D. 1987. The buying impulse. Journal of Consumer Research 14: 189-199.
Scherhorn, G.; Reisch, L.; Raab, G. 1990. Addictive buying in West Germany: an empirical study.
Journal of Consumer Policy 13 (December): 355-387.
Schlosser, S.; Black, D.; Repertinger, S; Freet, D. 1994. Compulsive buying: demography,
phenomenology, and comorbidity in 46 subjects. General Hospital Psychiatry 16: 205-212.
Shoham, A.; Brencic, M. 2003. Compulsive buying behavior. Journal of Consumer Marketing 20 (2) 127138.
Ullman, L.; Krasner, L. 1969. A psychological approach to abnormal behavior. Englewoods Cliffs, N. J.:
Prentice Hall.
Valence, G.; D’Astous, A.; Fortier, L. 1988. Compulsive buying: concept and measurement. Journal of
Consumer Policy 11: 419-433.
Warwick, J.; Mansfield, P. 2000. Credit card consumers: college students’ knowledge and attitude.
Journal of Consumer Marketing 17 (7): 617-626.
Woodruffe-Button, H.; Eccles, S.; Elliott, R. 2002. Towards a theory of shopping: a holistic framework.
Journal of Consumer Behaviour 1 (February): 256-266.
Woodruffe, H. 1997. Compensatory consumption: why women go shopping when they’re fed up and
other stories. Marketing Intelligence & Planning 15 (7): 325-334.
148
__________________________________________________
Incentives for Growth, 1005 and 2005
James M. Carman, University of California, Berkeley
Abstract
This paper is a comparative analysis of the causes of the rapid economic development in Western Europe
in the 11th and 12th centuries and the conditions promoting successful economic growth in the transition
and developing world of today. While some technological factors facilitating growth today did not exist in
the middle ages, many of the conditions for growth are the same in both period., Social, political, and
cultural institutional barriers that interfere with positive individual incentives needed to be overcome in
both periods.
Introduction
This paper presents a relatively small portion of a monograph length piece entitled “An inquiry into the
causes of rapid economic development in Western Europe, 1000 to 1200”. The reader may rightly feel
shorted by this partial glimpse, but given the focus of this group and the time available, I hope you will
gain some insights
The approach of the monograph was to investigate a large number of hypothesized causes of rapid growth
to see which might be rejected and which supported by the available evidence. Obviously, this is not a
new question for medieval historians and economic historians. The time period of 1000 or 1050 to 1300
or 1347 was a period of rapid population, economic, social, political, technological and cultural growth in
Western Europe. We have chosen to focus on the first two hundred years because therein must lie the
causes of such a prosperous period.
At the CHARM history conference, I presented a paper based on the monograph that highlighted the
impacts of trade on economic development. For this audience I want to relate this work on the 11th and
12th centuries to my work on transition economies that I have previously discussed at this conference.
Therefore, I am going to skip all of the historical description and cut right to the conclusions. Then the
paper will focus on a comparison of the conditions for successful economic development in this middle
ages period and in the present period.
Edward Carr (1961, p. 87) said, “The study of history is a study of causes.” But then later in the same
book he says causality is a matter of interpretation and therefore inevitably bound up with value
judgment. History must serve the present, and so, too, must our view of the causes of historical events.
For the purposes of this summary, causes are identified as sufficient, necessary and facilitating.
Facilitating conditions are catalysts, i.e., they increase the growth rate, but the process may well have
proceeded without them. Of course, some of the hypothesized causes we have identified factors that were
effects, rather than causes of growth, and factors that had no positive impact on growth.
Findings
A listing of the hypothesized causes for rapid development derived from the literature that were
considered in our study and our conclusions appear in table 1.
149
Table 1. Summary of Findings of Hypothesized Causes
Sufficient
Necessary
Facilitating
Not A Cause
Artistic Culture
Guilds
Hanse
Education
The Church
Charlemagne
Vikings*
Muslim Trade
Byzantium
Weather
Agricultural Production
Population Growth
Technology
Urbanization; Trade fairs
Opening trade routes in Europe
Feudal system
Individual freedom
Stable communal government
Italian cities
International trade
Efficient transaction instruments
Money & banking
systems
Federalism
War
Peace
*In Scandinavia only
_____________________
_____________________________
__________________
In this summary the findings are separated into sufficient, necessary, and facilitating causes along with
those that were not a cause at all. There are a number of things that merit mention in this table. First, note
that there is not one cause sufficient to have stimulated the rapid growth in our period of interest. Multiple
and interrelated events were necessary. Second, the table is static and thus cannot reflect the dynamics
that lie behind a more interesting set of causal relationships. It is these dynamic paths that will really
provide insights into the causes of this growth. The more informative story needs to be told as follows.
Agricultural output increased creating surplus food stock in most years. This growth in agricultural
production came about through land clearing that allowed increasing returns from labor. Technological
advances in farming facilitated an increase in labor productivity. However, it played a minor role
compared to the increased labor supply. The increased labor supply permitted the land clearing. This
increase in labor supply stems from the reduced need to defend oneself and one’s neighbors from raiders
150
and other warfare. There was a need for fewer knights and citizen soldiers as well as more time to tend
the fields rather than hiding from enemies. The people realized there were benefits to rearing large
families who could engage in activities that raised the standard of living for the family and for the
community. Peace needs to be viewed as a necessary condition for economic development.
This increase in population and foodstuffs led to a supply sufficient to increase a division of labor which
led to increased productivity and even more agricultural surplus. Specialization took the form of activities
other than those required for farming and housekeeping. It led to specialization in trade and crafts. This
degree of specialization would probably not have occurred if slavery were the method of acquiring labor.
The freedom of the individual to move to an urban area, develop a craft or trade, or become a traveling
merchant would not have been possible under indentured servitude. To summarize these last two
paragraphs: increased population → increasing returns → land clearing → agricultural surplus →
division of labor → increased productivity → more surplus.
The habitation of the tradesmen and craftsmen in urban areas rather than on the demesne, in itself, led to
an increase in productivity and efficiency. Thus urbanization is a facilitating result of this process.
Perhaps a more important facilitating factor, institutionalized by Charlemagne, in this dynamic process
was the feudal system itself. It was an effective social and economic organizational form on the demesne,
and it was flexible enough to allow for the formation of urban areas, trade fairs, the opening of trade
within Western Europe. It allowed for degrees of individual freedom and thus led to the development of a
true middle class.
One characteristic of the demesne organization in the countryside and successful urban governance was
governance with a manageable span of control. This becomes particularly clear in the Italian cities. While
there were varying levels of popular participative government among these cities, governance was stable
and manageable in scope. The evolution of the feudal governance system to the manorial system
beginning in the 11th century permitted a more efficient and manageable governance in the countryside.
“The classic manor persisted as long as the initial condition of chaos, abundant land, differential military
endowments, and scarce labor prevailed.” (North and Thomas 1973, p. 21) Except for small duchies such
as Flanders, viable federal governments did not come into being until many centuries later. The role of
international trade, conducted in greatest volume by the Italian cities, was probably a necessary
ingredient for growth rather than just facilitating.
Without certain raw materials and skills from the east, Western Europe may have developed during these
two centuries, but the growth would not have been as great or accomplished so much. So, the causal path
now becomes: increased population → increasing returns → land clearing → agricultural surplus →
division of labor → increased productivity → more surplus → markets and trade→ urbanization.
Other facilitating factors during this period were favorable weather conditions, the availability of labor to
pursue religious vocations and the growth of religious orders, more educated people, and an increase in
cultural achievements. Some of these advances had their roots in the Carolingian period. In addition, it is
important to give the Byzantine Empire credit for preserving Greek and Roman knowledge, enriching it
with Muslim knowledge, and transmitting it through the Italian cities right up to the end of the 12th
century. Note, we are concluding that none of these factors were exogenous factors that led to some kind
of quick-start trigger of rapid growth. They are either facilitating factors or the results of the economic
growth.
Another such factor is an efficient body of enforceable commercial law. The demand for trade caused
merchants to develop The Law Merchant, or more accurately to improve upon Muslim and Roman
commercial law. Indeed, I considered developing this topic for presentation at this conference. Increasing
transaction efficiency is a topic of interest to macromarketing scholars. These developments were the
result of growth that certainly increased the rate of growth. It can be classified as a facilitating condition.
While one could argue, as North and Thomas did, that it is a necessary factor particularly for international
trade, the fact that monetary systems were so weak during our period refutes commercial law as a
151
necessary factor. Another way to express this conclusion is that the resources for growth, in this case
labor and land, had to precede efficiencies in exchange of goods.
There were some positive effects of Viking raiding for Scandinavia, and the Crusades, but surely Europe
would have been better off without war. War cannot be listed as a cause of growth. Peace, on the other
hand, increased the productivity of labor and the worth of children. Peace, not war, is a necessary
condition for economic development. Most of the economic writing on this subject places the emphasis
on population growth for rapid growth. The contribution offered here is that it was peace and the ability to
clear land that triggered this increase in birth rates.
Learning from History
Are there lessons for today’s world to be learned from this period of growth in the middle ages? The
author has been studying transition economies in the 1980’s and 90’s in order to identify those ingredients
necessary for a successful transition to prosperity in the modern world. In this process, we have
previously presented a set of “precondition” we believe to be necessary for successful transition to a
healthy level of development. A comparison of that list with the list of necessary and facilitating
conditions in table 1 is useful. This comparison is summarized in table 2.
The last six items in the 11th century column are all exogenous factors important only for historical
purposes. The last factor in the 20th century column is based on an exogenous technology not available in
the middle ages. Perhaps of more interest are the two primary causes of 11th century growth, population
growth and agricultural production. In the present day, we are more interested in population control and
unequal distribution of an adequate worldwide food supply. However, we only have to go back to the 19th
century to observe these same two factors of growth in the western United States. In particular, the
“manifest destiny” migration of US farmers into the Oregon territory during the 1840 insured that the
Northwest would become part of the United States and a very developed part of our country.
The only other factor without a counterpart in the other period is the monetary and banking system. It is
not that money and banking systems would not have been useful in the middle ages; it is that trade was
able to invent ways to function without them. In the modern world, doing without efficient money and
banking systems would severely cripple trade flows and economic growth. All the other factors have
counterparts in both periods.
The centrality of trade as a driver of economic growth in the middle ages reinforces the arguments of
today for continuing our efforts to promote globalization and the reduction in trade barriers – as well as
the positive impact the California-Oregon coastal trade had on Oregon prosperity in the 19th century.
Public policies promoting exports out-weigh policies to protect domestic industries by limited imports.
Also, the improvement in transportation infrastructure and efficiency in logistics is an important
facilitator of growth in both periods.
The development of efficient transaction instruments in The Law Merchant certainly facilitated the
necessary condition of international trade. While we find it less important than peace, North and Thomas
were certainly correct in emphasizing its importance. The comparative analysis simply points out how
much more detailed these transactional institutional factors have become in the modern economy.
Political reforms also emerge as high priority factors for the promotion of growth, albeit with a slightly
different emphasis. First in both periods, world peace is a paramount condition for development and
growth. Second, stable and efficient government at local, national, and international levels was key in
preventing social and cultural barriers to growth from developing.
Applying this requirement to the globalization debate of today, what we see, in really all countries both
developed and undeveloped, is globalization does not lead to obvious win-win results. In a soon to be
published book, Douglas North says that institutions must provide a reduction in uncertainty. He believes
some institutions reinforce non-rational beliefs, such as fear of specialization of knowledge and
152
impersonal exchanges, which increase personal uncertainty and thus provide negative incentives for
growth. One may not have to accept this whole argument in order to see that what we have not done is to
find institutional arrangements for reducing the uncertainty including short and intermediate social and
economic disruptions caused by globalization and international trade.
Such institutional arrangements are inexorably entwined with the social and cultural factors associated
with individual freedom. Individual freedom is a necessary condition shown in both tables. Elements of it
are in the evolution of the feudal system, in trade fairs and urbanization in Italy and Western Europe, and
in the expansion of educational opportunities as well. All these phenomena provided incentives for
entrepreneurship and the development of a non-hereditary middle class. In his book on “the elusive quest
for growth,” William Easterly (2001) places incentives for individual human behaviors as his key
necessary condition for economic development in today’s undeveloped world. Our analysis of the 11th
and 12th centuries certainly supports this conclusion. Can we transform these rather simple middle ages
lessons into more complex nuances for the 21st century?
REFERENCES
Carr, Edward H. 1961. What is History? New York: Knopf.
Cipolla, Carlo M., ed. 1972. The Fontana Economic History of Europe, Vol 1: The Middle Ages. New
York: Harper and Row.
Durant, Will. 1950. The Age of Faith. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Easterly, William. 2001. The Elusive Quest for Growth: Economists’ Adventures and Misadventures in
the Tropics. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.
Greif, Avner. 1994. “On the Political Foundations of the Late Medieval Commercial Revolution: Genoa
During the Twelfth and Thirteenth Centuries.” Journal of Economic History, 54 (June), 271-287.
Lopez, Robert S. 1971. The Commercial Revolution of the Middle Ages, 950-1350. Princeton: Princeton
University Press.
North, Douglass C. and Robert P. Thomas. 1973. The Rise of the Western World: A New Economic
History. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
153
Table 2. Comparison of 11/12TH and 20TH Century Conditions for Growth
11/12th Century
Agricultural
Production (N)
Population Growth (N)
Trade fairs (F)
Italian cities (N)
Technology (F)
International trade (N)
Efficient transaction
instruments (F)
Individual freedom
(N)
Feudal system (F)
Peace (N)
Stable communal
government (F)
Opening trade routes
in Europe (F)
Education (F)
20th Century
Trade Reforms:
reduce trade barriers
encourage exports
encourage FDI and inflows of technology and human capital
Institutional Reforms:
legal system
commercial code
protection of property rights
intellectual property
bankruptcy laws
establishment of legal persons worker and employee rights
Marketing and business services:
information systems
public accounting
insurance
advertising
electronics payments systems
Privatization:
of state enterprises, monopolies and trusts
Political Reforms:
law & order reforms inside and outside government
government efficiency reforms
Logistical Infrastructure:
investments in improvement of port, airport, road and rail
infrastructure
Education and training systems
Banking System Reforms:
independent central bank
regulation of bank operations to ensure transparency and sound
lending practices
separation of bank from enterprise management
Communication Infrastructure:
privatization, deregulation and foreign investment into
telecommunication
The Church (F)
Charlemagne (F)
Vikings (F)
Muslim Trade (F)
Byzantium (F)
Weather(F)
Source of Col. 2: Carman and Dominguez, 2001 (N) Necessary (F) Facilitating
154
The Impact of Trade Globalization on Quality of Life
M. Joseph Sirgy, Virginia Tech
Dong-Jin Lee, Yonsei University, Korea
Chad Miller, Virginia Tech
James E. Littlefield, Virginia Tech
Abstract
The purpose of the paper is to develop a conceptual model to explain the impact of trade globalization
(i.e., the free flow of goods and services in the form of imports and exports) on a country’s quality of life
(QOL). In contrast to many scholars in marketing, business, and economics, we maintain that the QOL
impact of trade globalization is both positive and negative. In this paper, we show the complex mediating
effects between trade globalization and QOL.
Introduction
There has been much discussion on the topic of globalization of the world economy (e.g., Held et al.
1999; Levitt 1983; Sirgy et al 2004; Yip 1989). Globalization reflects a state of affairs in which a country
becomes more interconnected with the rest of the world (e.g., Bordo 2002; Levitt 1983). The drivers of
globalization include elimination of trade barriers and free flow of products and services across countries
(e.g., Hill 1997; Levitt 1983; Yip 1989).
Some argue that trade globalization has a negative impact on quality of life (QOL). They argue that trade
globalization eliminates many jobs, especially in the manufacturing sector. For example, it has been
argued that net loss of jobs under NAFTA between 1993 and 2000 in the US was 766,030 (Scott 2001).
“Hyper-globalists” argue that globalization is creating a new epoch of human history in which nationstates and governments, in general, are powerless to improve the QOL of their citizens (e.g., Ohmae
1995; Petras 1999; Soros 2000). They claim that global capitalism is now a great threat to the "open
society." Thus, there are many who paint a gloomy picture for a truly global world.
In contrast, many others argue that trade globalization has a positive influence on QOL (e.g., Thorbecke
and Eigen-Zucchi 2002). These pro-globalists view trade liberalization and increased market integration
as an opportunity to increase productivity and wages, thus improving the QOL of workers (Zoellick
2001). For example, it has been argued that US exports to NAFTA support 2.6 million new jobs (Council
of Economic Advisors 2002). It also has been argued that the negative impact of globalization, such as
elimination of manufacturing jobs, has been dramatically overstated (Thorbecke and Eigen-Zucchi 2002).
Only about 10-20% of manufacturing job losses in the United States is due to plant relocation to other
countries (Fligstein 2001). The decline in the manufacturing industry is not caused by globalization; it is
mainly driven by technological changes (Krugman 1996) and productivity changes (Rhodes 2004).
The Purpose of the Paper
Globalization, in general, refers to the free flow of goods and services, people, capital, and information
among countries (Bordo 2002; Held, et al 1999; Levitt 1983; Petras 1999). In a previous, we analyzed the
effects of overall globalization on QOL (Sirgy et al 2004). In contrast, our focus in this paper is on trade
globalization, i.e., the free flow of goods and services in the form of imports and exports. We do not
attempt to conceptualize the QOL impact of other forms of globalization such as the free flow of people,
155
capital, and information. We make an attempt to conceptualize the QOL impact of imports and exports of
goods and services. Furthermore, in the context of trade globalization, we strictly focus on imports and
exports, not other market entry methods such as international manufacturing operations, international
sales subsidiaries, international strategic alliances as in joint ventures and licensing, etc. These
international business market entry methods are the beyond the scope of our study. Although this model
does include intrafirm exporting and importing, provided that transfer pricing is at “arms-length” as
required by most governments. To reiterate, we focus on developing a conceptual model to capture the
QOL impact of trade globalization in the form of imports and exports of goods and services. Despite
much controversy, we have a limited understanding regarding how imports and exports affect QOL. The
literature lacks specific guidelines on how to measure trade globalization of a country (e.g., Held,
McGrew, Goldblatt, and Perraton 1999; Petras 1999).
The purpose of this paper is to develop a conceptual model describing possible impacts of trade
globalization on the QOL of a country. In other words, we show how trade globalization can create
positive and negative QOL effects (immediate and future effects).
How Does Trade Globalization Affect the QOL?
Our conceptual model is shown in figures 1-4. As shown in the figures, we show how imports and
exports impact the QOL of a country through economic, consumer, social, and health well-being of the
country residents. Specifically, we show the positive and short-term (immediate) impact of a country’s
exports on the QOL of the country’s residents (see Figure 1).
The short-term (immediate) effects refers to the QOL effects within three years while the long-term
(future) effects means QOL effects that occur after more than three years. The long-term effects take a
longer time as the mediators requires structural changes (e.g., increase in industry competitiveness) or
reflects reactions from the exchange partner countries (e.g., trade retaliations).
We show the negative impact long-term (or future) impact of a country’s exports on the QOL of the
country’s residents in Figure 2. The positive long-term (future) influence of a country’s imports is shown
in Figure 3. The negative short-term (immediate) influence of a country’s imports is shown in Figure 4.
We then develop specific theoretical propositions articulating and explaining the links shown in figures 14.
Refer to Figures 1-4 on pages 165-168.
The QOL Impact of a Country’s Exports
In regard to the QOL implications of increased exports, we posit that the effects are both positive and
negative and are both short-and long-term. The positive short-term influence of a country’s exports on
the QOL of the exporting country is shown in Figure 1. The negative long-term influence of a county’s
exports on the QOL of the exporting country is shown in Figure 2.
The Positive Impact of Exports on Economic Well Being: A country’s exports serve to enhance the
economic well being of the country’s residents by creating more jobs in the exporting sector (Figure 1).
Exports contribute to economic growth (Jaffee 1985) and increase purchasing power of the exporting
country (Dornbusch et al. 1998; Mullen 1993; Thorbecke and Eigen-Zucchi 2002). This assertion is
consistent with neoclassical trade theory that predicts free trade results in positive economic growth for
both parties. According to the comparative advantage theory of international trade, both importers and
exporters benefit from trade by specializing in production of goods and services they have comparative
advantages over other countries (Bradley 1995, pp. 36-37; Keegan 1995, pp. 378-381). That is, trading
countries obtain needed foreign currency through free trade by specializing in the production of
156
commodities needed by other countries. Specialization should reflect abundant resources in specific
industrial sectors. For example, countries that have abundant oil resources specialize in the oil industry
and export oil to other countries in exchange for foreign currency.
New jobs created by export activities provide workers with greater job opportunities and increased
income of workers in the exporting country. Evidence shows that exporters tend to be relatively more
efficient and pay higher wages than non-exporters (Council of Economic Advisors 2002). Increases in job
opportunities, disposable income, and standard of living all contribute to increases in economic well being
(Dornbusch et al. 1998).
The Positive Impact of Exports on Consumer Well Being: Increases in exports also serve to enhance
consumer well being (Figure 1). Exports provide workers with more job opportunities, higher income,
and a higher standard of living (Jaffee 1985; Thorbecke and Eigen-Zucchi 2002). Higher income and
more job opportunities generated by exports contribute to satisfaction of basic consumption needs
(Mullen 1993). Greater purchasing power from higher levels of disposable income allows consumers to
purchase goods and services to meet not only their basic needs but also their higher-order needs. This
demand for non-necessity items serves to provide the necessary market incentives to supply these goods
and services, thus further enhancing consumer well being.
Furthermore, we argue that increases in exports has a positive short-term (immediate) influence on
consumer well being of country residents mediated through increases in public sector spending, which is
used to upgrade the country’s infrastructure and government services. This comes about as a direct result
of increases in tax revenues, which in turn are positively affected by increases in jobs.
The Positive Impact of Exports on Social and Health Well Being: Export sales increase corporate
profits and personal income through job creation, resulting in increased tax revenues. Tax revenues are
used to provide a variety of public services, from transportation and healthcare to recreation and culture.
These public services impact the QOL of the country's residents by enhancing their social and health well
being (see Figure 1).
Increased tax revenues (e.g., increased corporate income tax and personal income tax) can be used to
provide better public services in the areas of education, healthcare, public safety, leisure, among others.
Government spending in these areas enhances social and health well being by providing needed public
services (Easton 2001). In contrast, government budget cuts due to reduced tax revenues adversely affect
social and health well being (Henderson 2001). Thus, one can argue that increased tax revenues helps
government increase spending on public sector services. Enhanced public services should significantly
affect social and health well being (Peterson and Malhotra 1997).
The Negative Impact of Exports on Economic Well Being: Figure 2 depicts the negative long-term
(future) impact of a country’s exports on the QOL of the country’s residents. Exports adversely affect
competing firms in the countries of import. Thus, competing firms lose sales and profits, causing them to
lay off employees. Decreases in sales of competing firms in the importing countries do not contribute
only to increases in unemployment but also to decreases in tax revenues. High unemployment in the
importing countries perhaps causes workers in those countries to feel resentment toward the country of
export. Decreased sales and profits of competing firms also may result in feelings of resentment by the
business community in the importing countries. Reduced tax revenue also may cause government
officials in the countries of import to feel resentment against the exporting country (cf. Scheve and
Slaughter 2001). For example, Latin American agriculture workers resent imports of subsidized US
agriculture products, and this resentment is holding up expanded free trade agreements such as FTAA.
Mexican farmers rail against the more efficient, albeit heavily subsidized, North American agribusinesses. Of course, trade retaliation is likely to occur when export volume reaches a high threshold at
later stages of the relationship. In the early stages of a trade relationship, imports are less likely to
157
provoke resentment from the importing country. Resentment builds up over time with continuous and
increasing levels of imports.
Because workers and labor unions and the business community are all negatively affected by imports,
they may take retaliatory measures in the form of consumer boycotts and buy-domestic campaigns
(Mullen 1993; Sternquist and Phillips 1991). Research has shown that buy-domestic campaigns are
prevalent in countries in which residents express a high level of concern about job losses, empathy
towards displaced workers, and feelings of patriotism (Granzin and Olsen 1995). Furthermore, reacting
to feelings of anger and resentment of displaced workers, the business community as well as the
governments of the importing countries may retaliate against the country of exports by imposing high
tariffs, quotas, and/or other non-tariff barriers.
These retaliatory measures are likely to affect the QOL of the country’s residents. Specifically, the
country’s economic well being is likely to be adversely affected by the importing countries’ retaliatory
measures by the eventual decrease of exports, which in turn would cause loss of jobs and tax revenues.
The Negative Impact of Exports on Consumer Well Being: Increases in exports also may have a
negative future impact on the well being of consumers in the exporting country (see Figure 2). We
previously argued that exports might cause the unemployment rate to jump in the importing countries.
High unemployment and low sales of competing firms in the countries of import breed feelings of
resentment among workers and the business community. Faced with feelings of resentment and pressure
from displaced workers and the business community (plus loss of tax revenues), governments of the
import countries are pressured to adopt protectionist trade policies (cf. Baldwin and Magee 2000; Kletzer
2001). It should be noted that trade retaliations typically occur when the volume of exports reaches a
high level.
The anger with imports is not just felt in developing countries. The Christian Science Monitor (2003)
reports a survey of Americans, where 45% believe that “free trade” is: “good”; 34% said it was “bad.” In
the same survey, only 16% stated that free trade creates more jobs than it loses, while 53% said that it
loses more jobs than it creates.
Increased trade and non-tariff barriers in the importing countries also serve to reduce consumer well being
of the exporting country, because consumers in the exporting country eventually suffer from declining
foreign income. The lack of that income hurts consumers because they cannot use that income to
purchase needed goods and services. The lack of income takes away from market incentives to provide
goods and services in high demand. Furthermore, reduced personal income results in fewer tax revenues,
which in turn affect consumer well being through reduced quality and access to public sector services (cf.
Scheve and Slaughter 2001). In addition, in an effort to focus on the export market, exporters may reduce
the quantity of the exported goods to local consumers. Product scarcity in the local market may cause
domestic prices to increase, which in turn may reduce consumer well being.
The Negative Impact of Exports on Social and Health Well Being: As we argued previously, increases
in exports may lead to high unemployment rates and decreased sales in foreign markets, which may
generate feelings of resentment against the country of export. Feelings of resentment among workers, the
business community, and government of the country of import may lead to trade retaliations, buy
domestic campaigns, and import boycotts, which in turn are likely to adversely affect exports. Decreased
sales and loss of jobs resulting from shrinking of export markets should reduce tax revenues of the
exporting country, which in turn should lead to cuts in spending on social and health programs (cf.
Henderson 2002). That is, an increase in exports may have a negative influence on social and health well
being of residents of the exporting country through a complex process involving feelings of resentment of
workers, the business community, and governments of the foreign markets (see Figure 2). In other words,
158
both loss of jobs (and the reduced income resulting from job loss) and reduced tax revenues (resulting
from both job loss, reduced personal income, and reduced export sales) have a significant and adverse
effect on the social and health well being of the country’s residents. For example, Southside Virginia has
been hard hit by the loss of import sensitive textile jobs and this has decimated the local tax base creating
a host of social and health problems among Southside residents (Chittum and Allen 2002).
Furthermore, to compete effectively in foreign markets, exporters often are forced to restructure by
laying-off current high-wage workers or implementing pay cuts, and/or recruiting a new low-wage labor
force. Doing so reduces tax revenues and in turn public spending, which reduces social well being.
The QOL Impact of a Country’s Imports
In regard to the QOL effects related to the increased inflows of goods and services (imports), we surmise
that the QOL effects are also both positive in both the short and long run and negative in the short run
(see Figures 3 and 4).
The Positive Impact of Imports on Economic Well Being: The positive influence of a country’s
imports on the QOL of the country’s residents is shown in Figure 3. Increased imports serve to enhance
the country residents' economic well being (cf. Campbell 1981; Thorbecke and Eigen-Zucchi 2002). This
comes about as a direct function of increases in personal income and job opportunities.
Increases in imports are likely to have a positive immediate influence on the economic well being of the
country’s residents, because imports increase jobs in the distribution sector, especially in import-related
industries. Distribution firms benefit from increased business opportunities and create jobs. For example,
Galuszka and Kranz (2002) reported that distribution facilities have fueled port-related employment
during the past two decades. Specifically, the number of port-related jobs has nearly tripled in this time
period. Most of that growth came from the proliferation of distribution facilities (cf. Chase and Pascall
1999; Galuszka and Kranz 2002; Witherspoon 1996). Furthermore, it is well known that countries (e.g.,
Belgium, Singapore) and cities (e.g. Hong Kong, Miami) with extensive logistics-based jobs benefit from
import distribution.
In addition, increases in imports expose domestic firms to the discipline of open competition and motivate
them to increase their worker productivity and market competitiveness (Zoellick 2001). Increase in
imports also facilitates the transfer of technology, which in turn increases the overall competitiveness of
domestic firms. Increases in the overall competitiveness of domestic firms, in turn, contribute to the
economic well being of the country’s residents through increased jobs, wages, and tax revenues (cf.
Kletzer 2001; Lane 1991; Samli 1985). For example, Mexican Maquiladoras have successfully adopted
new technologies, such as lean manufacturing, in order to compete in world markets (Axtman 2003).
The Positive Impact of Imports on Consumer Well Being: We surmise that increases in imports may
have a positive immediate influence on the consumer well being of the residents of the importing country
(see Figure 3). Imported goods usually find a market in the home country because these goods often have
a competitive advantage compared to domestic goods. Imported goods enhance consumer well being of a
country because they provide consumers with greater access to high quality products, more choices, and
reduced prices (e.g., Granzin and Olsen 1995; Mullen 1993; Scehve and Slaughter 2001).
In addition, imports from other countries motivate domestic producers to work harder to improve their
product quality and price competitiveness (Scheve and Slaughter 2001). In time, domestic producers
work harder to provide domestic consumers with more quality goods at lower prices. For example,
increases in imports have been a boon in both prices and product selection for India's fast-growing middle
class (Constable and Lakshmi 2001).
159
The Positive Impact of Imports on Social and Health Well Being: We believe that increases in imports
play a role in enhancing the social and health well being of residents of the importing country. This may
occur through the mediating effect of increases in tax revenues and enhanced public services (see Figure
3). Increases in sales of imported goods and creation of import-related jobs in the importing country
provide tax revenues. Increases in imports also induce domestic competitors to be more efficient and
competitive. Faced with import competition, domestic firms make every effort to increase sales (e.g.,
develop new technologies, enhance worker productivity, etc.). This healthy competition, the high level of
firm performance, and worker productivity translate into higher tax revenues. Increased tax revenues in
the importing country contribute to an increase in government public spending and enhance the level of
public services in such areas as health care, education, public safety, leisure, etc. Government spending on
these areas is important for social and health well being (cf. Easton 2001).
The Negative Impact of Imports on Economic Well Being: Figure 4 shows the negative influence of a
country’s imports on the importing country’s well being. Increased imports provide consumers in the
importing country with greater access to high quality and low priced products. This, of course, reflects
increased consumer well being of the country's residents; however, this may come at the expense of
decreased economic, social, and health being. Increased imports may result in decreased sales of
comparable domestic products. Domestic firms react by laying off workers to maintain an acceptable
level of profitability (Granzin and Olsen 1995). Job loss is a major driver for a lower economic well
being (Lane 1991).
During the 21-year period from 1979 to 1999, 6.4 million U.S. workers were displaced as a result of
imports in industries such as electric machinery, apparel, toys, motor vehicles, non-electric machinery and
blast furnaces (US Department of Labor 2001b). It is not just developed nations that face job
displacement due to cheap imports. For example, Mexico lost 256,000 jobs in two years due factories
moving to China (Mireles 2002).
The risk of job loss is usually high in industries with high trade deficits. More balanced-trade industries
tend to have a lower level of job losses (Kletzer 2001). Kletzer estimates that although about 65 percent
of displaced workers (displaced as a direct result of imports) can be re-employed; however, they are likely
to suffer an income loss averaging 13 percent. Displaced workers who are older, less educated, and less
skilled are likely to suffer an income loss averaging 30 percent or more. Therefore, increases in imports
should adversely affect the country residents' economic well being because imports contribute to job
layoffs in competing domestic firms.
A case in point regarding the costs and benefits of imports is Martinsville, Virginia. In the early 1900s,
furniture manufacturing from New England replaced chewing-tobacco factories as major employers. An
abundant supply of lumber and good railroad connections helped with this “new” industry. In the 1990s
and early 2000s, much furniture supply in the United States started coming from China. However,
Martinsville furniture firms including Hooker furniture, receive, inventory, and distribute this furniture
from China. So, the impact of Chinese furniture manufacturing has a negative impact on furniture
manufacturing workers and a positive impact on warehouse and distribution workers (based on “Virginia
City Seeks ‘something else’,” The Washington Times, 20 April 2004).
The Mixed Impact of Imports on Consumer Well Being: Increased imports provide consumers in the
importing country with greater access to high quality and low priced products. This, of course, reflects
increased consumer well being of the country's residents. However, increases in imports can have a
negative influence on the well being of consumers in the target country. Imports often result in high
unemployment and decreased sales of competing domestic firms in the importing country. As a result,
the spending power of consumers in the importing country decreases. That is, reduced disposable income
among consumers in the importing country will negatively influence the well being of consumers (Mullen
160
1993). In addition, reduction in tax revenues from the domestic companies in the importing country
should negatively affect consumer well being. Reduction in tax revenues in the importing country forces
the importing country’s government to reduce spending on public programs, some directly related to
consumer well being such as consumer protection, consumer safety, quality assurance, among others (cf.
Scheve and Slaughter 2001).
The Negative Influence of Imports on Social and Health Well Being: Increased imports can have a
negative influence on social and health well being of the importer’s country residents. This may occur
through the mediation effects of reduced tax revenues and deteriorated public services in the importing
country (Granzin and Olsen 1995). Slow sales of domestic firms and high unemployment rates in the
importing country mean decreased tax revenues. Decreased tax revenues result in reduction of public
spending and deterioration of public services (Henderson 2002). The outcome is decreased social and
health well being of the residents of the importing country (see Figure 4).
Conclusion
One can argue that the rules governing economic globalization have been created through trade
agreements, international law, and institutions dominated by industrialized countries. Examples of these
rules include dropping tariffs, eliminating capital controls, enforcing intellectual property rights,
privatizing public services, and weakening regulations that protect labor, health and safety, and the
environment (Unitarian Univeralist Association of Congregations 2003). It is time to study the effect of
trade globalization in a comprehensive manner. The findings of such studies should allow international
bodies such as the World Trade Organization and the United Nations to revisit public policies related to
international trade and re-examine those trade agreements generated by the industrialized countries.
Based on objective, unbiased QOL assessment of international trade, public policy can be effectively
formulated in ways to maximize the positive impact of a country’s QOL while minimizing the negative
impact of the country’s QOL.
We hope that our proposed model may stimulate a program of research in this area. We hope that future
studies will test the many theoretical propositions of the model, develop valid and reliable measures
involving its various constructs, and expand the model to identify conditions (legal, economic, political,
etc.) that can help account for variations in positive and negative QOL impacts of trade globalization.=
161
REFERENCES
Axtman, K. 2003. NAFTA’s Shop-floor Impact. Boston Globe, 4 November 1.
Baldwin, R. E. & Magee, C. S. 2000. Congressional trade votes: from NAFTA approval to fasttrack Defeat. Policy Analysis in International Economics, 59, Washington: Institute for
International Economics.
Bordo, M. D. 2002. Globalization in historic perspective. Business Economics, January: 20-29.
Bradley, F. 1995. International marketing strategy, 2nd edition, London: Prentice-Hall, pp.
36-37.
Campbell, A. C. 1981. The sense of well-being in America: recent patterns and trends.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Chase, R. & Pascall, G. 1999. Foreign imports and the Washington State economy. Technical
Report Prepared for Washington State Community, Trade & Economic Development.
Christian Science Monitor 2003. Free trade takes increasing hits. Christian Science Monitor 6
November, 2-3.
Constable, P. & Lakshmi, R. 2001. Indians brace for a flood of imports; government lifts restrictions on
numerous goods. The Washington Post (4 April) A18.
Council of Economic Advisors 2002. Supporting global economic integration. Economic Report of
the President. Washington DC: Government Printing Office.
Dornbusch, R., Fischer, S., & Startz, R. 1998. Macroeconomics, New York:
Easton, B. 2001. Globalization and a welfare state. In Managing the global economy:
globalization, employment, and quality of life, edited by D. Lamberton, 163-168. New
York: The Toda Institute for Global Peace and Policy Research.
Fligstein, N. 2001. The architecture of markets. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press.
Galuszka, P. & Kranz, G. 2002. Here comes China. Virginia Business, May 2002. 5.
Granzin, K. L. & Olsen, J. A. 1995. Support for buy American campaigns: an empirical
investigation based on a prosocial framework. Journal of International Consumer
Marketing 8 (1): 43-69.
Henderson, H. 2002. Quality of life: issues and questions. In Managing the global economy:
globalization, employment, and quality of life, edited by D. Lamberton, 51-98. New York:
The Toda Institute for Global Peace and Policy Research.
Held, D., McGrew, A, Goldblatt, D., & Perraton, J. 1999. Global transformations: politics,
economics, and culture. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Hill, C. W. L. 1997. International business: competing in the global marketplace. Chicago, IL: Irwin.
Jaffee, D. 1985. Export dependence and economic growth: a reformulation and re-specification.
Social Forces 64 (September): 102-18.
162
Keegan, W. J. 1995. Multinational marketing management, 5th edition, Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall, 378-381.
Kletzer, L. G. 2001. Job loss from imports: measuring the costs. Washington DC: Institute for
International Economics.
Krugman, P. 1996. Pop internationalism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lane, R. E. 1991. The market experience. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Levitt, T. 1983. The globalization of markets. Harvard Business Review, May-June: 92-102.
Mireles, R. C. 2002. Chinese Maquiladoras threaten Mexico. Transportation & Distribution 43 (11):
26-28
Mullen, M. R. 1993. The effects of exporting and importing on two dimensions of economic
development: an empirical analysis. Journal of Macromarketing 13 (1): 3-19.
Ohmae, K. 1995. The end of then nation state. New York: Free Press Paperbacks.
Peterson, M. & Malahotra, N. 1997. Comparative marketing measures of societal quality of life:
substantive dimensions in 186 Countries. Journal of Macromarketing 17 (Spring): 25-38.
Petras, J. 1999. Globalization: a critical analysis. Journal of Contemporary Asia 29 (1): 3-37.
Porter, M. 1998. On competition. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Publishing, p. 185.
Rhodes, K. 2004. Global gain local pain. Regional Focus: The Federal Reserve Bank of Richmond
8 (1): 12-18.
Samli, A. C. 1985. Technology transfer: geographic, economic, cultural and technical dimensions.
Westport, CT: Quorum Books.
Scott, R. E. 2001. NAFTA's impact on the states: the industries and states that suffered the most in
the agreement's first seven years. Economic Policy Institute Briefing Paper, Washington DC.
Scheve, K. F. & Slaughter, M. J. 2001. Globalization and the perceptions of American workers.
Washington DC: Institute for International Economics.
Sirgy, M. J. 2001. Handbook of quality-of-life research: an ethical marketing perspective. The
Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Sirgy, M. J., Lee, D-J., Miller, C, & Littlefield, J. E. 2004. The impact of globalization on a country's
quality of life: toward an integrated model. Social Indicators Research 68(2): 251-298.
Soros, G. 2000. Open society: reforming global capitalism. New York: Public Affairs.
Sternquist, B. & Phillips. L. A. 1991. Hong Kong, U.S. retail buyers increase purchase of imports.
Marketing News 25 (October 14): 7, 9.
163
Thorbecke, W. & Eigen-Zucchi, C. 2002. Did NAFTA cause a ‘giant sucking sound’?, Journal of
Labor Research 23 (4): 647-658.
U.S. Department of Labor 2001. Bureau of Labor Statistics, http://stats.bls.gov
Unitarian Universalist Association 2003. Revised draft statement of conscience, April 2003, UU
Association of Congregations. p.1.
Witherspoon, J. C. 1996. Foreign trade zone sub-zones: innovation assistance to existing business
in a global economy. Economic Development Review 14 (3): 65.
Yip, G. S. 1989. Global strategy--in a world of nations? Sloan Management Review, Fall: 29-41.
Zoellick, R. 2001. Remarks at the National Foreign Trade Council. July 26, http://www.ustr.org.
164
165
Increases
in sales of
exporting
firms
Increases
in tax
revenues
Increases
in public
sector
spending
Increases
in social &
health well
being
Increases
in
consumer
well being
Increases
in
economic
well being
FIGURE 1: The Positive Short-term (Immediate) Impact of Exports on the QOL of the Country’s Residents
Increases
in the
country’s
exports
Increases
in
jobs
166
Increases
in the
country’s
exports
Increases in
resentment of
business
community in
importing
countries
Increases in
resentment of
government in
importing
countries
Increases in
resentment of
workers in
importing
countries
Importing
countries’
buydomestic
campaign
Importing
countries’
trade
retaliations
Consumer
boycotts in
importing
countries
Reduced
demand for
imported
goods
Limited
Choice for
Domestic
Consumers
Reduced
export
sales
Reduced
tax
revenues
Job loss
and
reduced
income
FIGURE 2: The Negative Long-term (Future) Impact of Exports on the QOL of the Country’s Residents
Decreases in
sales of
competing firms
in importing
countries
Decreases
in tax
revenues in
importing
countries
Increases in
unemployment
in the importing
countries
Restructuring of
domestic
exporting
industry
Decreases
in social
and health
well being
Decreases
in
consumer
well being
Decreases
in
economic
well being
167
Greater
access to
lower price
goods
Greater
access of
high
quality
goods
Increases
in sales of
importing
firms
Increases
in spending
on social
and health
programs
Increases
in tax
revenues of
importing
country
Increases in
competitivene
ss of domestic
firms
Increases
in
consumer
well being
Increases
in social
and health
well being
Increases
in
economic
well being
Increases in
jobs of
competitive
firms
FIGURE 3: The Positive Short- and Long-run Impact of Imports on the QOL of the Country’s Residents
Increases
in the
country’s
imports
Increases
in jobs in
the
importing
sector
Increased
exposure to
foreign
competition
Increases in
transfer of
technology to
importing
country
168
Greater
access of
lower price
goods
Decreases in
sales of
competing
domestic firms
Increases in
unemployment
in competing
domestic firms
Decreases
in public
sector
services
Decreases
in tax
revenues
Decreases in
social and
health well
being
Decrease &
increase in
Consumer
Well Being
Decreases in
economic
well being
FIGURE 4: The Negative Short-term (Immediate) Impact of Imports on the QOL of the Country’s Residents
Increases
in the
country’s
imports
Greater
access of
high
quality
goods
Publication Patterns in Journals Dedicated to Issues of
Marketing and Society: Benchmarking and Analysis
Michael J. Polonsky, Victoria University, Australia
John D. Mittelstaedt, Clemson University, USA
Abstract
The purpose of this paper is to examine the nature and distribution of scholars engaged in
knowledge production in journals focusing on marketing and society, broadly defined. Who is
interested in issues of marketing and society? Where are they from? How does this compare to
patterns of scholarship in recognized journals in marketing? The answers to these questions
have implications for how sub-disciplines like Macromarketing can position themselves for future
growth.
Introduction
Globally there has been an increasing interest in understanding academic research productivity
(Easton and Easton 2003; Polonsky et al. 2003). In many instances this work has focused on
which individuals and institutions are the most productive in “top” journals (Bakir et al. 2000;
Helm et al. 2003), within given regions (Cheng et al. 2003) or within specific sub-disciplines of
marketing (Henthorne et al. 1998; Zinkhan and Leigh 1999). To date, however, there has been no
broad-based examination of scholarship devoted to questions of the interface between marketing
and society.
While marketing and society scholars are likely to be disinclined to participate in the “numbers”
game of journal publication, there are practical reasons that, collectively, we understand who is
and is not engaged in research topics of interest similar to our own, especially in regards to
publishing in journals that are established to promote these issues. Research performance
measures are indeed important, for individuals and institutions (Cheng et al. 2003; Williams and
Van Dyke 2004b), because they affect performance evaluations, promotion and tenure decisions,
and in some cases allocation of resources within and between academic institutions (HEFC 2004;
The Education Commission 2004). In an increasingly “measure and manage” environment,
research interests and outlets (especially among young, untenured scholars) cannot help but be
affected by the reality of journal analyses. In this environment, a sub-discipline that cannot show
how it “stacks up” will be hard pressed to attract the best efforts of scholars worldwide and thus
inhibit the growth and dissemination of research in the specific area.
Consistent with Wilkie and Moore’s (2003) call for greater coordination among marketing and
society-minded scholars, an understanding of who publishes within journals targeting this subdiscipline is an important first step in developing strategies for enhancing interest and
participation in meetings, conferences and journals devoted to research on the impact of society
on marketing, and the impact of marketing on society.
To this end, the purpose of this paper is to examine the geographic nature and distribution of
scholars engaged in knowledge production in the journals focusing in the area of marketing and
society, broadly defined. Where are they from? How does this compare to patterns of
169
scholarship in “leading” journals in marketing? The answers to these questions have implications
for how sub-disciplines like Macromarketing can position themselves for future growth, as well
as serve to better promote the fact that this is a vibrant international area of interest supported in
established journals as well as in more specialized journals that are the focus of this research
topic.
Method
The authorship of five years of articles (1999-2003) was examined by institutional nationality,
across three sets of journals- “A,” “B” and “Social.” The groupings used relied on those identified
by Polonsky and Whitelaw (2004a & b) who undertook a study of how the “average” US
academic views marketing journals on four dimensions- prestige, contribution to theory,
contribution to practice and contribution to teaching. Within their study Polonsky and Whitelaw
identified 20 journals that their sample was most familiar with, i.e., able to evaluate, and these
then formed the basis of their work in regards to develop rankings of these journals (6-A, 11-C
and 3-C, discussed further in Table One). This set of journals was selected for use because it
covered a cross section of marketing-focused journals and focused on US marketing academics
perceptions. Polonsky and Whitelaw (2004a) also identified that these 20 journals had for the
most part been included in previous ranking studies (Hawes and Keiller 2002; Hult et al 1997;
Mort et al 2004; Polonsky et al 1999; Theoharakis and Hirst 2002).
The added benefit to using Polonsky and Whitelaw’s set of journals is that their multidimensional nature allowed journals to be “clustered” using the four evaluative criteria (Polonsky
and Whitelaw 2004b), which is also presented in Table One. In this way their groupings allows
the research presented in this paper to examine performance across clusters of journals, which
reflect broad groups used in regards to evaluation of performance. For example, the tenure
expectations of new faculty are specified in “A’s and B’s” rather than in terms of specific journals
(DocSig 2004). This allows for a more straightforward comparison among institutions in terms of
research productivity, allowing for differences in specialization and mission.
It might be suggested that using US-based definitions of “top” journals or associated clusters
could be problematic. However, evidence exists that these serve as global norms for research
quality. For example, Mort et al. (1994) found that journal rankings by Australian/New Zealand
scholars did not significantly differ from journals ranked by US academics. Additionally,
Polonsky and Whitelaw (2003) found that perceptions of US and Asia-Pacific academics of the
four underlying dimensions of journal quality did not differ significantly. As such, we feel
comfortable using these clusters are general indicators of journal quality.
In addition, data were collected for the same time period (1999-2003) for journals whose mission
statements identify issues of marketing and society as their primary focus. Additionally, these
journals reflect the breadth of “marketing and society” issues identified by Wilkie and Moore
(2003). Included for analysis were the Journal of Macromarketing, the Journal of Public Policy
and Marketing, the Journal of Consumer Affairs, the Journal of Non-Profit and Public Sector
Marketing, the International Journal of Non-Profit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, and Social
Marketing Quarterly. (Because complete records for SMQ were not available for this time
period, it is included in descriptive, but not analytic, analyses). We do of course recognize that
other journals, including those identified in the “top 20” also publish works related to marketing
society issues. For example the Journal of Marketing has published a number of works (such asLichtenstein et al 2004 and Andrews et al 2004) that could have also been published in more
focused social-oriented journals. The examining of social issues within boarder journal is an issue
that could be considered in future research.
170
Data
The data was collected by reviewing all articles published in 26 journals between 1999-2003. All
author’s institutions of affiliation were identified and then tabulated in two ways. First, the
number of authors from each institution was counted. Thus if there were four co-authors each
authors’ institution was allocated a “1.” If more than one author was affiliated with the same
institution, this institution would have been credited multiple times and when an individual listed
more than one affiliation their “score” was split between institutions. Second, the data was
tabulated to reflect the contribution of each author to the article (i.e. relative articles authored),
with a sum of 1.00 points allocated between all contributors’ institutions.
These two sets of publication data were aggregated for each institution across the cluster of
journals. These data were then aggregated by nationality of institutions. Across the 25 journals
there were 3,414 articles, including 929 A articles, 1,861 B articles and 557 Social articles (i.e. in
the social journals). There were scholars from 27, 57 and 23 countries, respectively who authored
these works (See Table 3).
Findings
Table Two summarizes the relative contributions of relative articles authored, the number of
represented institutions, and the number of authors for each country, for each of the six socially
oriented journals. Consultants and industry or government employees are recorded, but not by
country. Across the six journals (in this case the partial data from Social Marketing Quarterly
were included), authors from 23 countries are represented, and the patterns of participation vary
among the journals. During the period 1999-2003, the Journal of Macromarketing had the largest
number of countries represented (14), followed by the Journal of Consumer Affairs (11), though
it had the fewest authors or articles of all the journals (excluding Social Marketing Quarterly, for
which the records are incomplete). This suggests that among journals with a social orientation,
the Journal of Macromarketing has the most diverse contribution base in terms of nations
contributing.
These findings raise the question, how do publication rates among marketing journals with a
“social” orientation compare to more traditional marketing journals? We expected that given the
global interest in this area and global scope of journals, we would find more national diversity of
publishing (and by extension, interest) in social marketing journals than in A- or B- level
journals. Table Three summarizes numbers of institutions, authorships and % of authorships
across countries for A-Level, B-Level and “social” marketing journals (the latter excluding
SMQ). Empty squares indicate no publications for this country in the category. During the
period 1999-2003, A-level journals published articles by authors from 27 countries, while B-level
journals published articles from authors in 51 countries. These numbers reflect the fact that a
broader number of interests and topics are reflected in a broader spectrum of journals (e.g.,
consumer behavior, research methods, econometric modeling, services, advertising, and/or
strategy, in addition to issues of marketing and society). The relatively small number of countries
represented across sub-disciplines among A-level journals, compared to B-‘s, might indicate that
A publishing is more “exclusive”. Further examination of this issue (not included here) indicates
that not only are the vast majority of A-level authors from research focused US universities.
While social marketing journals appear to have some broadness in coverage, it appears that,
compared to A-‘s and B-‘s, social marketing as reflected in these publications, is the purview of
the Anglo world (US, UK, Australia and Canada). In short, “Marketing and Society” is of
worldwide interest, but “worldwide” should not be interpreted to mean “global.” Combined,
171
these results suggest differences in participation patterns, but alone leave open the question of the
dispersion of authorships across and within countries. What can we say about the distribution of
authors of articles on marketing and society, compared to A-level and B-level journals?
To address this question, we calculated a Herfindahl-Hirschman Index (HHI) for each journal
class, by institutions, authorships and number of relative articles authored, at the country level.
The HHI is used in a variety of contexts to measure industrial concentration. It expresses
concentration by taking into account both the distribution and proportions of factors (higher
numbers indicate higher concentrations). It asks if we drew two authors at random, what is the
likelihood that they would be from the same country? Answers depend both on the number of
countries and the relative authorship across those countries. As we might expect, not only are
relatively few countries represented in the A-level journals, even fewer are repeatedly
represented. The most globally diverse journals appear to be the B-'s, at least among those we
examined. “Social” marketing journals are more concentrated than B-'s, although it does need to
be remembers that there are in fact twice the number of journals represented (11- B as compared
to 5- social oriented) and there are also 3 times as many papers in B journals. Thus the diversity
of B journals publishing would be less surprising.
In examining the HHI for authorships to institutions there are large differences for A-‘s and B-‘s
across nationalities (see Table Four). There are however, similarities in regards to US based
contributions to A- journals based on the number of institutions contributing and for B-journals
based on authorship issues. As such it would appear that there is a reasonable “spread” in
institutions as authors of socially focused work. This would suggest that discourse within the
marketing and society journals reflects a conversation, rather than a lecture.
Table Four:
Herfindahl-Hirschman Indices for Number of Institutions, Number of Authorships
and Relative Articles Authored and Number of Authorships, by Journal Class
Journal Class
A – Level
B – Level
Social
Journals
Number of
Institutions
0.4351
0.2733
0.4211
Number of
Authors
0.6419
0.4095
0.4738
Relative Articles
Authored
0.6753
0.4155
0.4728
Conclusions
The results suggest that there is global appeal for research into marketing and society, as
evidenced by publishing the recognized journals focusing on these topics (e.g., Andrews et al
2004 and Lichtenstein et al 2004), as well as the diversity of authors and countries represented
in the focused marketing and society journals. This has important implications for the
development of the discipline. One could infer that as publishing in specialized socially focused
journals increases, it would be gaining prestige within institutions were the authors are based. As
such it may be moving from a “niche” topic of interest to a more widely accepted on. The
increasing numbers of works in “traditional” journals having a social focus would further support
this. While this would seem to be positive it is possible that the dispersion of interest has
172
unintended consequences, the most important being that there is possibly less need for the
focused or specialized journals. While it might be beneficial to move social oriented works in to
the main stream, thus moving beyond “preaching to the converted,” it could also be that the
emphasize of works are shifting as they appear in more generalist journals. While, Shugan
(2003) calls for a valuing of all research and research outlets, it is unclear how
institutions and colleagues perceive these works.
The results of this preliminary examination are of course positive in terms of the spread of
interest of research into social types of issues. More research might be undertaken to examine the
underlying motivation of such research. Is this because of the growing importance of the issue or
underlying psychological motivations of researchers? Is it because individuals’ perceive this area
of research to be one that is “easier” to examine, because of consumers intrinsic interest and the
fact that the industry has not been “over researched”?
The global spread of interest is important and may present different opportunities. There may be
more globally comparative works. Such research allows for an examination of generalisability of
issues and approaches, but also allows for sharing global experiences that enhance research and
practice. If the global examination of issues were to be a fragmentation rather that focusing,
diverse research may preclude synergies in being developed.
For research communities, such as the Macromarketing Society, this analysis should raise
questions (and concerns) about where its resources (conferences, proceedings, journals, library
presence, membership) should be concentrated. Where should we meet in the future? What
topics should we address at meetings and in special issues of JMM? We differ to the discussion
of the conference to address these and related issues. As with most research, this work possible
raises more questions than it answers.
REFERENCES
Andrews, G.J., R.G. Netemeyer, S.Burton, P.D. Moberg and A. Christiansen.2004.
Understanding adolescent intentions to smoke: an examination of relationships among social
influence, prior trial behavior, and antitobacco campaign advertising, Journal of Marketing
68 (3), 110-124.
Bakir, B., S.J. Vitell and G.M. Rose. 2000. Publications in major marketing journals: An analysis
of scholars and marketing departments. Journal of Marketing Education 22(2), 99-107.
Cheng, L.T.W., K.C. Chan and R.Y.K. Chan. 2003. Publications in major marketing journals: an
analysis of research productivity of Asia-Pacific universities,” Journal of Marketing
Education 25(2), 163-176.
DocSig. 2004. Research tenure requirements for new hires according to respondents to DocSig
annual survey. DocSig.eci.gsu.edu Accessed Sept 15th 2004.
Easton, G. and D.M. Easton. 2003. Marketing journals and the research assessment exercise.
Journal of Marketing Management 19(1-2), 5-24.
Hawes, J.M. and B. Keiller. 2002. Assessing marketing journals: a mission-based approach.
Journal of the Academy of Business Education 3(2), 70-86.
Helm, A., D. Hunt and M. Houston. 2003. Citation frequency of research published in the top
173
three marketing journals: ranking the impact of articles, scholars, and institutions" 2003 AMA
Summer Educators' Conference Proceedings 198-208.
Henthorne, T.L., M.S. LaTour, and T. Loraas. 1998. Publication productivity in three leading
U.S. advertising journals: 1989 through 1996. Journal of Advertising 27(2), 53-63.
Hult, G.T.M., W.T. Neese, and R.E. Bashaw. 1997. Faculty perceptions of marketing journals.
Journal of Marketing Education 19 (Spring), 37-52.
Lichtenstein, D.R., M.E. Drumwright and B.M. Braig. 2004. The effect of corporate social
responsibility on customer donations to corporate-supported nonprofits,” Journal of
Marketing 68(4), 16-33.
Mort, G.S., JR. McColl-Kennedy, G. Kiel and G.N. Soutar. 2004. Australian and New Zealand
senior academics’ perceptions of marketing journals,” Australasian Marketing Journal 12(2),
51-61.
Polonsky, M.J., B. Juric and G. Mankelow. 2003. Attitudes about work practices, time allocation
and publication output: profiles of U.S. marketing academics,” Journal of Marketing
Education 25(4), 218-230.
Polonsky, M., G. Jones and M. Kearsley. 1999. Accessibility, an alternative method of ranking
marketing journals? Journal of Marketing Education 21(4), 181-193.
Polonsky, M.J., and P. Whitelaw. 2004a. Exploring multi-dimensional perceptual ranking of
marketing journals by North American academics,” 2004 AMA Summer Educators’
Conference Proceedings, 13-20.
Polonsky, M.J., and P. Whitelaw. 2004b. Grouping journals using multi-dimensional perceptual
criteria of North American marketing academics,” ANZMAC 2004, Session 9.4: 1-7.
Shugan, S.M. 2003. Journal rankings: save the outlets for your research,” Marketing Science
22(4), 437-441.
Theoharakis, V., and A. Hirst. 2002. Perceptual differences of marketing journals: a worldwide
perspective. Marketing Letters 13(4), 398-402.
Zinkhan, G.M., and T.W. Leigh. 1999. Assessing the quality ranking of the journal of advertising,
1986-1997. Journal of Advertising, 28(2), 51-70
174
Table One
Journal Criteria Scores and Rankings
(Adapted from Polonsky and Whitelaw 2004a & 2004b)
Prestige Theory Practice Teaching Cluster
Score Score
Score
Score
JOURNAL
Journal of Marketing Research
6.52
6.35
5.09
4.08
A
Journal of Marketing
6.52
6.17
5.47
4.59
A
Journal of Consumer Research
6.58
6.45
4.34
4.13
A
Journal of the Academy of
Marketing Science
5.52
5.41
4.67
3.91
A
Journal of Retailing
5.59
5.32
5.14
4.07
A
Advance in Consumer Research
4.26
4.69
2.85
3.04
C
Journal of Advertising Research
4.86
4.66
5.06
4.14
B
Journal of Advertising
5.24
5.06
4.76
3.92
B
Psychology & Marketing
4.57
4.59
3.82
3.30
B
Journal of Consumer Psychology
5.26
5.26
4.02
3.55
B
Marketing Science
6.24
6.05
4.98
4.00
A
Marketing Letters
4.68
4.49
4.10
3.44
B
Journal of Public Policy &
Marketing
5.20
4.80
4.85
4.03
B
European Journal of Marketing
4.13
4.10
4.23
3.33
B
Industrial Marketing Management
4.42
3.95
4.71
4.08
B
Journal of Marketing Education
4.11
3.74
3.61
5.39
B
Academy of Marketing Science
Review
3.16
3.55
2.84
2.08
C
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales
Management
4.43
4.43
4.86
4.09
B
Journal of Consumer Marketing
3.69
3.71
3.66
2.94
C
International Journal of Research in
Marketing
4.79
4.82
4.18
3.68
B
175
176
Germany
France
Finland
Denmark
Canada
Australia
CONSULTANTS & INDUSTRY
TOTAL
COUNTRY
JOURNAL
.5
1
2
4
5
11
30
Journal of
Public Policy
and
Marketing
1
1
1
7
10.9
9
29
1.5
3
3
Journal of
Non Profit
and Public
Sector
Marketing
1
1
1
1
1
2
63
10.8
10
26
7.3
7
16
2
3
3
13
Journal of
International
Consumer
Journal of
Affairs
Nonprofit and
Voluntary
Sector
Marketing
Relative
Relative
Relative
RelativeAuthorship
Authorship
Authorship
Authorship
# of Institutions # Institutions # Institutions # Institutions
# of Authors
# of Authors
# of Authors
# of Authors
127
102
147
86
93
95
76
85
271
191
234
190
21.5
2.9
51.5
5.7
26
1
1
2
.5
1
1
Relative
Authorship
# Institutions
# of Authors
36
18
52
17.6
Social
Marketing
Quarterly
Table Two:
Publications in Social Marketing Journals, by Country
1
1
1
1
1
1
2
4.5
6
7
3.8
5
7
1
1
1
Relative
Authorship
# Institutions
# of Authors
59
59
82
1.5
141
27.2
26
64
19.1
24
41
1
1
1
1
1
1
2.5
3
5
2.67
3
4
557
426
1020
100.7
TOTALS
Journal of
Macromarketing
177
Poland
New Zealand
Netherlands
Malta
Japan
Israel
Ireland
HK
COUNTRY
JOURNAL
1.5
2
3
Journal of
Public Policy
and
Marketing
2.33
1
4
1
1
1
Journal of
Non Profit
and Public
Sector
Marketing
1
1
1
2
1
4
1.33
2
2
.14
1
1
Journal of
International
Consumer
Journal of
Affairs
Nonprofit and
Voluntary
Sector
Marketing
Relative
Relative
Relative
Relative
Authorship
Authorship
Authorship
Authorship
# of Institutions # Institutions # Institutions # Institutions
# of Authors
# of Authors
# of Authors
# of Authors
2.25
3
6
2
2
3
Table Two, continued
Relative
Authorship
# Institutions
# of Authors
.5
1
1
1.5
2
2
.33
1
1
1
1
1
Relative
Authorship
# Institutions
# of Authors
2
1
2
Journal of
Macromarketing
Social
Marketing
Quarterly
2.75
4
7
3.5
4
5
.33
1
1
1
1
1
2.33
1
4
4
3
4
4.83
5
9
.14
1
1
TOTALS
178
USA
United Kingdom
United Arab Emirates
Taiwan
Thailand
Spain
Korea
Singapore
South Africa
COUNTRY
JOURNAL
Table Two, continued
1.17
2
3
97.5
82
218
1
1
1
5.2
5
9
72.5
69
125
Journal of
Non Profit
and Public
Sector
Marketing
51.3
34
89
18.7
18
28
.33
1
1
70.9
69
156
.5
2
1.5
Journal of
International
Consumer
Journal of
Affairs
Nonprofit and
Voluntary
Sector
Marketing
Relative
Relative
Relative
Relative
Authorship
Authorship
Authorship
Authorship
# of Institutions # Institutions # Institutions # Institutions
# of Authors
# of Authors
# of Authors
# of Authors
.5
1
1
1
2
1
1.17
1
1
1
4
2
.5
1
1
3
1
4
1
9
3
Journal of
Public Policy
and
Marketing
3.25
1
4
13.2
14
19
Relative
Authorship
# Institutions
# of Authors
Social
Marketing
Quarterly
6.5
9
8.5
32.5
28
43.5
1
1
3
.25
1
1
Relative
Authorship
# Institutions
# of Authors
1.5
2
3
2.17
1
6
.75
2
2
4
5
12
1
1
3
.5
2
1.5
1
1
1
67.75
52
114.5
305.3
280
589.5
TOTALS
Journal of
Macromarketing
179
Consultants
Africa
Argentina
Australia
Austria
Bahamas
Belgium
Bosnia
Brazil
Canada
Chile
China
Columbia
Croatia
Cyprus
Czech Rep.
Denmark
Egypt
Europe
Fiji
Finland
France
Germany
Greece
Hong Kong
Hungary
India
Ireland
Israel
Italy
Country
2
3
5
27.5
19
30
1
2
2
4
9
4
20
2
49.50
2
16
3
14
5
1.5
32
1
11
1
2
65
1
# Authors
Marketing
1
# Institutions
A-Level
7.82
.58
15.83
13.67
8.75
1.12
.83
.58
.5
22.84
5.12
16.83
.25
Relative
Articles
Authored
25.07
.25
Journals
1
23
1
24
48.5
30
15
67
2
4
20
11
3
1
9
20
18
6
6
2
3
8
5
3
223
2
1
162.50
8
2
17
1
2
100
1
7
1
1
# Authors
Marketing
1
7
2
1
29
5
1
8
1
2
28
1
5
1
1
# Institutions
B-Level
13.4
23.52
12.78
8.82
30.02
1.17
2.08
11.66
5.15
1.5
.5
.5
11.63
Relative
Articles
Authored
121.3
.83
1
85.98
4.32
1
7.16
.83
.4
48.59
.33
2.66
.25
.5
Journals
4
1
4
1
3
2
1
20
14
#
Institutions
Social
Table Three:
Publications in A-Level, B-Level and Social Marketing Journals, by Country
5
1
7
1
5
4
1
40
62
115
# Authors
Marketing
3.5
.33
2.75
1
2.5
2.66
1
18.63
26.23
Relative
Articles
Authored
83.09
Journals
180
Japan
Kuwait
Malaysia
Malta
Mexico
Netherlands
Norway
New Zealand
Pakistan
Poland
Portugal
South Africa
Russia
Saudi Arabia
Singapore
South Korea
Slovenia
Spain
Sweden
Switzerland
Taiwan
U.A.E.
U.K.
Ukraine
U.S.A.
Country
2
1
72
7
7.5
10
14
2
1
5
1
33.5
1442.5
1
9
5
2
3
10
2
1
2
1
10
209
# Authors
Marketing
2
# Institutions
A-Level
740.89
15.82
.66
.5
2.25
.25
4.64
5.23
.25
30.38
3.66
3.67
Relative
Articles
Authored
.66
Journals
5
1
1
1
1
14
3
7
1
1
3
3
1
2
4
13
2
20
16
4
7
1
73
1
367
# Institutions
B-Level
2
1
2
1
123
18
67
1
1
4
6
1
2
32
17
3
41
33
7
10
2
321.5
1
2120
5
# Authors
Marketing
Table Three, continued
Relative
Articles
Authored
1.5
.58
.33
1.33
.33
53.84
11.87
32.62
.5
.17
2
3.5
.08
1.33
15.84
7.27
1.5
22.5
16.38
4.58
5.49
1
179.71
1
1101.58
Journals
1
3
1
2
570.5
1.5
1
110.49
2
1
40
192
12
5
6
2
9
4
1
2
4
4
1
# Authors
Marketing
3
1
1
#
Institutions
Social
292.11
1
1
64.47
3.96
2.17
.75
1.5
.14
4.83
4
2.33
Relative
Articles
Authored
1
Journals
Thought and Comment
on Future Scenarios for Macromarketing
Panel
Mark Peterson, University of Texas at Arlington
Sandy Grossbart, University of Nebraska-Lincoln
William Kilbroune, Clemson University
Abstract
From his recent experience as Program Director for the 30th Annual Macromarketing Conference,
Mark Peterson will share his thoughts on an opportunity for repositioning macromarketing after 30
years. The opportunity is to develop a focus on synthesis and strategic issues in the current post-9/11,
globalizing world. Such a focus would help enable scholars to engage enlightened leaders of nations,
transnational organizations, NGOs and companies on broad topics, such as sustainable development.
Sandy Grossbart will give comment. Bill Kilbourne will share his ideas.
The following is from “How I Learned to Think Horizontally” by Thomas P. M. Barnett (Barnett 2004).
“During my eight years in Washington, Hank Gaffney and I did a lot of research together and
coauthored a number of significant reports, and most of the big ideas I have hatched over my career
either began in or were intimately shaped by my time with him. But what Hank really taught me was
how to think horizontally. By that I mean thinking broadly across subject matters versus drilling
down deep into a particular subject, which I call vertical thinking. In both the Pentagon and
Washington in general, the system of debates awards points almost exclusively to those who think
vertically, because intense subject-matter expertise allows you to poke holes in everyone else’s
thinking.”
“Inside the Beltway, vertical thinkers are expert at telling why something will never succeed,
and little else. Horizontal thinkers tend to be the exact opposite. They often argue by analogy and are
quick to borrow concepts from other fields. They are usually synergists, meaning they combine
disparate concepts in new and unusual combinations. For example, in my PhD dissertation I
borrowed from the field of interpersonal psychology to explain how the relative “weakling” Romania
stood up to Soviet bullying tactics within the Warsaw Treaty Organization, ultimately achieving a
certain degree of foreign policy independence.”
“The most important advantage of horizontal thinking is the ability to see a future unfolding in
realistic stages, never becoming too invested in any one particular pathway. Most of what passes for
strategic planning in the Pentagon involves the acquisition (or buying) of future technologies in the
form of weapons systems and platforms (e.g. ships, aircraft, and tanks). Within those narrow
confines most vertical thinkers do well. But these same vertical thinkers are incapable of making
similar forecasts about the messy world lying outside the Pentagon. Here their tendency to define the
future as straight-line projections of current trends consistently betrays them, because you can never
mechanistically extrapolate tomorrow from today.”
Barnett, Thomas P. M. 2004. The Pentagon’s New Map: War and Peace in the Twenty-First Century,
New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons, 112-113.
181
__________________________________________________
Macro-micro Linking Using System Dynamics Modeling:
A Preliminary Examination of Eco-Labeling Effects for
Farmed Shrimp
Steven P. Arquitt, University of Queensland, Australia
T. Bettina Cornwell, University of Queensland, Australia
Abstract
Eco-labeling has been heralded as a market based approach useful in addressing environmental issues.
While a growing number of eco-labeling projects are found in nations around the world, most of the
analysis of the effectiveness of these programs is historical and thus limited in usefulness for policy.
Moreover, models examining eco-labeling effects tend to be focused either on the environmental impacts
or the consumer adoption of eco-labeled goods and services and do not attempt to link consumer
behavior to environmental impact. The current paper offers a somewhat different approach by using
system dynamics modeling to capture both consumer adoption and environmental conservation for a case
example of farmed shrimp. Policy analysis conducted via simulations suggests that public service
announcements can at least lessen the degradation of the natural environment typically impacted in
shrimp farming.
Introduction
The potential value of eco-labeling schemes in supporting sustainable or at least environmentally friendly
behavior has received considerable researcher attention across a number of disciplines including
economics (Bjørner, Hansen and Russell 2004; Moon et al. 2002; Teisl, Roe and Hicks 2002), consumer
policy (Reisch 2001; Thøgersen 2000), and tourism (Kozak and Nield 2004). Eco-labeling has been
defined as “a practice of providing information to consumers about a product which is characterized by
improved environmental performance and efficiency compared with similar products” (Basu, Chau and
Gote 2003). Eco-labeling programs have been called a “market based approach” to address
environmental issues (Wessells, Johnston and Donath 1999). Eco-labeling takes three forms (Reisch
2001) (1) self-declaration labels by individual companies, (2) labels established by industry associations
for their member’s products, and (3) labels established independent of market agents—private or state run
certification bodies. The case example utilized in the current paper is a potential private not-for-profit
certification program for farmed shrimp available to all international growers.
The Argument for a System Dynamics Approach
Recently Cornwell and Drennan (2004) argued that consumer behavior research, especially research regarding the
environment, could benefit from micro-macro linking. This linkage is described by Gerstein (1987) as “how to
create theoretical concepts that translate or map variables at the individual level into variables characterizing social
systems, and visa versa” (p. 86). Moreover, Cornwell and Drennan nominate a system dynamics approach as a
potentially valuable method in this regard. Citing the work of Meadows (1982), they argue that system dynamics
helps address problems that share core elements such as: (1) policy resistance, (2) drift to low performance, and (3)
addiction or dependency. Many environmentally oriented consumer behaviors such as recycling include these core
elements. For example, consumer inducements and encouragements to recycle have been studied in many countries,
but arguably this behavior is policy resistant and drifts to low levels of recycling after campaigns conclude.
Another reason for the suggested use of system dynamics in this area is that it could provide more timely
guidance to managers and policy decision makers regarding the usefulness of green marketing programs
182
and public policy. Perhaps the most convincing empirical paper to date exploring the potential of a
national eco-labeling system, the Nordic Swan in Denmark (Bjørner et al. 2004) utilizes data from 1997
to 2001 and was published a short three years following data collection. While an important work, and a
potential source of information to the type of modeling suggested here, it is at the time of availability,
historical. Sterman (2000) is a strong advocate of the systems approach to guiding decision making and
policy formulation. With regard to managers, Paich and Sterman (1993) argue that mental models of
dynamic environments are generally poor; “Specifically, people do not account well for feedback loops,
time delays, accumulation processes and nonlinearities (p. 1454). Evidence of the consumer’s inability to
understand complex issues of sustainability has also been noted; in particular their inability to recognize
the role of consumer demand (Connolly and Prothero 2003) is disturbing given its importance to
environmental conservation.
Perhaps the most important argument of any for the use of system dynamics modeling in this context is its
ability to consider dynamic complexity inherent in eco-labeling. Current examinations of the effects of
eco-labeling programs necessarily take one piece of the picture at a time. For example, Swallow and
Sedjo (2000) consider a wood demand model that focuses on feedback regarding price in a market
economy. In their model, consumer communication effects are not detailed. In contrast, Thøgersen
provides a very detailed conceptual model of how consumers will pay attention to eco-labels and form the
decision to purchase eco-labeled products. Naturally, their model does not consider the more macro
oriented economic effects of aggregated individual consumer decisions. These types of studies can,
however, be melded at some level of aggregation in an overall model. Moreover, this model could also
include the environmental impact of the eco-labeling scheme. This is essentially what is proposed in the
current paper, a multi-sector model that would allow overall assessment of eco-labeling effects for world
shrimp farming. We have selected shrimp farming as our case study because presently there is
considerable international interest in eco-labeling for farmed shrimp1. The purpose of this model is to
understand how eco-labeling schemes might be designed to yield positive economic, environmental and
consumer outcomes. The following section provides some background detail on world shrimp farming.
Shrimp aquaculture and the environment
Growing international demand for shrimp and stagnating catches of wild shrimp in the early 1980s
created an opportunity for the development of export orientated shrimp aquaculture industries (Csavas
1995). Countries with climate and natural resources suitable for shrimp farming, particularly in Asia and
Latin America, seized on the opportunity, transforming vast stretches of coastline into shrimp farms.
Growth in the sector has been spectacular over the past two decades. In 1982 shrimp aquaculture (also
known as shrimp farming) accounted for only about 5% of world shrimp supply, by 1994 this figure had
risen to 30% (Flaherty et al. 1999). Some experts estimate that aquaculture now accounts for 50 percent
of world shrimp supply (Rosenberry 2004). Globally, farmer earnings from shrimp farming were
estimated at over US$6 billion in 1996 with retail value three times that amount (Flaherty et al. 1999). In
Thailand, one of the world’s largest exporters of farmed shrimp, the industry generated over US$1.7
billion in export earnings in 1996 (Flaherty at al. 1999). In addition to providing social benefits of
employment creation and foreign exchange earnings, many believed that shrimp aquaculture would take
pressure off over-fished wild shrimp stocks and offer an environmentally benign alternative to destructive
practices of the shrimp capture industry.
Despite the apparently bright picture of growth and export earnings at the global scale, the shrimp
farming industry has exhibited an extremely instable pattern of development strongly associated with
ecological damage and social disruption. In the early 1980s the industry grew rapidly in Taiwan and
China only to suffer dramatic production crashes within a few years. Similar patterns can be observed for
The Philippines, Indonesia, and India. These boom and busts have been observed both at the national
scale and within countries. In Thailand, for example, the national production figures have remained high
but mask a series of boom and busts in which the industry has developed rapidly in one region only to
183
crash and migrate to another (Huitric et al. 2002). The production crashes have left trails of abandoned
shrimp ponds and depleted natural resources, in particular coastal mangroves, and have caused social
damage through loss of employment in shrimp farming and related side industries.
The form of shrimp farming examined here is known as “brackish water” shrimp farming. Several species
are farmed but all are marine species requiring salt water. For this reason shrimp farms are typically
found along coastal margins, often on the shores of estuaries and embayments lined or formerly lined
with mangrove forests. “Mangrove” refers to a tropical coastal ecosystem type that is alternately
inundated and exposed by tides and dominated by species of salt tolerant trees. Mangroves are the
dominant ecosystem type found in most shrimp farming areas and serve as an important resource base for
shrimp production by maintaining water quality through assimilation of nutrients and pollutants
(Rönnbäck 1999). Mangroves are an essential resource base for numerous other industries, including
capture shrimp fisheries, and provide a host of ecosystem services important to biodiversity and rural
incomes (Ashton et al. 2003).
System Dynamics Model
We have developed a simulation model using the methods of system dynamics to investigate how an ecolabelling policy might lessen the adverse environmental impacts of the shrimp aquaculture industry.
System dynamics posits that the behavior of a system is a function of its internal feedback structure. The
feedback structure consists of interconnected feedback loops that are either reinforcing (also known as
positive) or balancing (also known as negative). Reinforcing loops cause growth or collapse. Balancing
loops are goal seeking and act to stabilize the system. Thus the behavior of a system is determined by the
interaction of reinforcing and balancing feedback loops and information delays and non-linear
relationships embedded within the loops. For an extensive description of system dynamics see Sterman
(2000)2.
The model is divided into eight interacting sectors. Figure 1 shows the sectors and linkages between
sectors. The points below sketch the key characteristics of each sector.
• The Consumer Sector describes the transition of potential adopters (consumers) of eco-certified
shrimp to adopters. The transition is influenced by the price for eco-certified shrimp relative to
the price for non-certified shrimp. The sector considers the aggregate world population
consumers of farmed shrimp. This sector is described in more detail following this overview.
• The Non-certified Demand and Eco-certified Demand Sectors describe how world-wide demand
for eco-certified and non-certified shrimp responds to shifts in consumption preferences and
changes in price for the two categories of shrimp. The structure of the sectors is based on the
generic commodity demand structure described by Sterman (2000). Demand adjusts after an
adjustment lag to an indicated level that is a function of the ratio of price to a constant reference
price and reference demand. Reference demand is the population of consumers multiplied by the
per capita consumption of shrimp at the reference price.
• The Non-certified Price-setting and Eco-certified Price-setting Sectors describe how inventory
levels and production costs determine prices. Prices are adjusted by inventory holders to maintain
inventories are desired levels (Meadows 1970). Prices are also adjusted toward expected
production costs (Sterman 2000).
• The Non-certified Shrimp Production and Eco-certified Shrimp Production Sectors describes
farmer’s decision rules pertaining to expansion or contraction of production. Production is a
function of the total area under shrimp farms and farming intensity. Farm area expands or
contracts in response to expected profitability which is the ratio of long run expected price minus
expected fixed and variable costs divided by long run expected price (Sterman 2000). Intensity
adjusts to an indicated level which is a function of the expected mark-up ratio, the ratio of short
run expected price to expected variable costs (Sterman 2000). A supply chain is used to capture
184
delays involved with planning and constructing new farms. Non-certified shrimp farms are
assumed to be non-sustainable and are continuously abandoned and replaced with new farms. A
fraction (fifty percent) of non-certified shrimp farms are assumed to be initiated within mangrove
areas. Thus the abandonment and replacement of shrimp farms mimics the sequential exploitation
of mangroves described by Huitric et al (2002). Eco-certified farms are assumed to be sustainable
and not detrimental to mangroves. This would of course be a policy parameter of the ecocertification program.
• The Environmental Impact Sector models the effect of non-certified shrimp aquaculture on
environmental resources. The stock of mangrove erodes through clearance as construction of noncertified farms is initiated. In the present model mangroves are modelled as a non-renewable
resource, i.e., with no regeneration. In reality mangrove regeneration on lands formerly occupied
by shrimp farms is very low due to soil contamination from farm waste and hydrologic
alterations. The initial value in the model for the mangrove stock is a gross estimate of worldwide
mangrove under potential threat of exploitation by shrimp farmers. Our intention is to
demonstrate the process of shrimp farm induced mangrove deforestation rather than predicting
actual loss rates.
Given space limitations, only the consumer sector will be explained in further detail.
Consumer Behavior Sector
A simplified diagram of the causal structure of the Consumer Behavior Sector is shown in Figure 2. The
normal diagramming conventions of system dynamics are followed. The rectangles represent state
variables. State variables change only through inflows or outflows which are indicated by thick arrows
with “valves.” Information linkages are indicated by unidirectional arrows. In the model, Potential
Adopters represents the population of consumers that makes their shrimp purchasing decision only on the
basis of price and availability. In contrast, Adopters buy only eco-certified shrimp and exhibit
willingness to pay (WTP) a premium for eco-certified shrimp. WIP premiums are measured in a number
of eco-labelling and environmentally friendly behaviors (see Bjørner et al. 2004 and Schwepker and
Cornwell 1991 respectively). Both categories are found in shrimp consumers world-wide. The structure
of the sector is based on the well known Bass Diffusion Model (Bass 1969). The system dynamics
interpretation of the Bass model is that the population of adopters grows through a reinforcing feedback
loop representing the increasing influence of word-of-mouth as the population of adopters increases.
Eventually a balancing feedback slows adoption as the population of potential adopters is depleted. The
interaction of the two loops causes the population of adopters to exhibit an s-shaped pattern through time
(Paich and Sterman 1993).
In the current model, word-of-mouth communication is termed, Social Pressure. Eco-labelled shrimp,
rather than creating a “buzz” of excitement as a new durable product might, create a discussion of
environmental responsibility. As the population of Adopters increases, the contact rate with Adopters
will increase, exerting increasing social pressure to buy eco-friendly shrimp (Social Pressure Loop, R1).
As the pool of Potential Adopters is depleted the adoption rate slows (Saturation loop, B). In the original
Bass Model adoption through advertising was modelled as the population of potential adopters multiplied
by an exogenous fractional rate representing advertising effectiveness. Given that we assume that the
certification body is an international non-profit organization public service announcements (PSAs) rather
than advertising are utilized. Clearly this is only a difference in terms from the original model. The Bass
model has been widely used to model the adoption of consumer durables. For application to the
differentiated eco-labelled shrimp within the shrimp commodity system we have made several extensions:
• A flow representing erosion of the adopter population back to potential adopters has been added.
• The variables representing effectiveness of public service announcements (PSA) and social
pressure to adopt are now influenced by the eco-certified shrimp price relative to the non-certified
price. We assume that the effectiveness of PSA and social pressure to adopt will diminish if the
price of eco-labelled shrimp exceeds the adopters’ WTP premium.
185
•
A reinforcing feedback has been put in place that reduces the erosion of adopters as the
population of adopters increases (Social-norm building loop, R2).
Model simulation
The model was developed with STELLA Research version 7.2. The simulation time horizon is 50 years,
a period long enough for the full dynamic patterns to unfold. The base simulation is shown in Figure 3.
The variables of interest are: (1) Eco-certified shrimp production, (2) Non-certified shrimp production, (3)
the population of adopters, (4) the population of potential adopters, and (5) the mangrove stock. With the
exception of the Environmental Impact Sector the model is initialized in equilibrium, i.e., all the state
variables remain constant with inflows and outflows adding to zero. Setting the stocks in equilibrium is
usual practice in system dynamics and allows observation of the behavior inherent in the system structure
when the initial equilibrium is disturbed (Sterman 2000). The mangrove stock decays from year zero
because it is being continuously consumed by the replacement of abandoned shrimp farms in the Noncertified Sector.
At year five the system is “disturbed” by the implementation of public service announcements regarding
the non-profit’s eco-certification and the potential conservation of mangroves. This causes Potential
Adopters to flow to Adopters based on a fractional rate representing PSA effectiveness. As adopters
accumulate the reinforcing feedback of social pressure accelerates the adoption rate. The population of
adopters grows, increasing the demand for eco-certified shrimp. The increase in demand drives up the
price for eco-certified shrimp (price not shown in diagram). However, the price influence is overcome by
the development of a social-norm to remain Adopters as the population of Adopters and the contact rate
among Adopters increases. Production of eco-certified shrimp increases and displaces non-certified
production the population of Potential Adopters falls. The production time paths exhibit patterns of
dampening oscillation typical of commodities (Meadows 1970). By the end of the simulation almost all
production is eco-certified and the mangrove stock is no longer being consumed (compare to mangrove
stock in Figure 4 which shows continuous decay of mangrove when there is no increase in demand for
eco-certification). The objectives of the eco-certification policy are achieved in this simulation, although
admittedly only lessening the negative environmental impact in the near term.
Sensitivity Analyses
A parameter with important policy implications is the effectiveness of PSAs. Figures 5 and 6 compare
simulations of the population of Adopters and of Mangroves respectively under a range of values for PSA
effectiveness. For both figures PSA effectiveness is set at zero (time path 1), 0.01 (time path 2), 0.05
(time path 3), 0.1 (time path 4) and is held constant. Figure 7 shows simulation of the adopter population
and mangrove stock under a one time pulse PSA effectiveness set to 0.10. This fractional rate suggests
that 10% of Potential Adopters will become Adopters in a single year. This sort of large scale
communication impact (presumably supported public discussion accounted for in the model) has
president in the market for tuna and the use of dolphin safe labelling (see Teisl et al. 2002).
The simulations suggest that PSA programs are influential in initiating and speeding growth of adopters
and in protecting mangrove resources. If an adequate pool of adopters can be facilitated through PSA,
then reinforcing feedback through social pressure can exert itself more quickly. Figure 6 suggest that an
intensive PSA program even for short duration can set into motion the feedback process of adopter
population growth through social pressure. The simulations further suggest that a strong one time pulse
may be more effective than a continual weaker PSA program.
Discussion
Clearly, there are a number of limitations to any model of this magnitude. Of particular importance here
is the fact that key parameter estimates are based on secondary sources (e.g., willingness to pay). This
186
however, need not remain the case. Experimental findings considering willingness to pay or potential
effectiveness of a public service announcement could be easily incorporated in the model. The model’s
central value is as a learning tool. Policy entry points and areas needing additional empirical research are
identified in process of model building and simulation. A potential pitfall of the approach is that
simulation results might be construed or promoted as predictions. We emphasize that prediction is not our
objective nor should it be the objective of any system dynamics study. Rather, simulation should be
viewed as a test of hypothetical assumptions embedded in the model.
The contributions of this paper are found in three areas. First, the paper demonstrates the
potential value of the systems approach in linking macro and micro effects in a single model and the
related advantages of explicating specific parameters for a complex system. Also provided is a generic
approach to modeling eco-labeling initiatives. As long as reasonable parameter estimates can be found,
the eight sector model could be used to examine any number of eco-labeling schemes. Additionally, this
model could easily be made country or region specific. Finally, the model offers findings specific to the
case study. The well known value of a large pulse of advertising (or in this case, public service
announcements) can be demonstrated for the case of eco-labeled shrimp. The conversion of Potential
Adopters to Adopters might even be conservative since media coverage and possible special interest
group activities are not included in the model. Public awareness at a level that influences the very nature
of the industry (as was the case with dolphin safe tuna, Teisl et al 2002) is the kind of tipping point
pattern hoped possible.
Notes
1.
Presently an international program entitled “Shrimp farming and the environment” supported by the World
Bank, World Wildlife Fund (WWF), Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (FAO), and
the Network of Aquaculture Centers of the Asia Pacific (NACA) is promoting eco-certification and ecolabeling for the global shrimp farming industry. For details refer to www.enaca.org.
2. A reviewer of this manuscript asked us to explain how system dynamics differs from systems theory as
generally defined in sociology. System dynamics was developed independently from systems theory as
proposed by von Bertalanffy (1968). However, the two are closely related in the sense that both schools of
thought focus on the arrangement of and relations between the elements of a system which together
constitute a whole and which generate the behaviour of the system.
REFERENCES
Ashton E.C., D.J. Macintosh and P.J. Hogarth 2003. A Baseline Study of the Diversity and Community
Ecology of Crab and Molluscan Macrofauna in the Semantan Mangrove Forest, Sarawak,
Malaysia. Journal of Tropical Ecology. 19: 127-142.
Bass, Frank M. 1969. A New Product Growth Model for Consumer Durables. Management Science. 15:
215-227.
Basu, Arnab K., Nancy H Chau and Ulrike Grote. 2003. Eco-Labeling and Stages of Development.
Review of Developmental Economics. 7(2): 228-247.
Bertalanffy, Ludwig von. 1968. General system theory : foundations, development, applications. New
York : Braziller
Bjørner, Thomas Bue, Lars G. Hansen and Clifford S. Russell. 2004. Environmental labelling and
consumers’ choice—an empirical analysis of the effect of the Nordic Swan. Journal of
Environmental Economics and Management. 47: 411-434.
Connolly, John and Andrea Prothero 2003. Sustainable Consumption: Consumption, Consumers, and the
Commodity Discourse. Consumption, Markets & Culture. 6(4): 275-291.
187
Cornwell, T. Bettina and Judy Drennan. 2004. Cross-Cultural Consumer/Consumption Research: Dealing
with Issues Emerging from Globalization and Fragmentation. Journal of Macromarketing, 24(2),
108-121.
Csavas I. 1995. Development of shrimp farming with special reference to Southeast Asia. Presented at
INDAQUA 1995 Exposition of Indian Aquaculture, Madras.
Flaherty M, Miller P. 1999. Rice paddy or shrimp pond: tough decisions in rural Thailand. World
Development 27 (12): 2045-2060.
Gerstein, Dean R. 1987. To unpack micro and macro: Link small with large and part with whole. In The
micro-macro link, edited by Jeffrey C. Alexander, Bernhard Giesen, Richard Münch, and Neil J.
Smelser, 86-111. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Huitric M, Folke C, Kautski N. 2002. Development and government policies of the shrimp farming
industry in Thailand in relation to mangrove ecosystems. Ecological Economics 40: 441-435.
Kozak, Metin and Kevin Nield. 2004. The Role of Quality and Eco-Labelling Systems in Destination
Benchmarking. Journal of Sustainable Tourism, 12 (2): 138-148.
Meadows Donnella H. 1970. Dynamics of Commodity Production Cycles. Wright-Allen Press:
Cambridge MA.
Meadows, Donella H. 1982. Whole systems: Whole Earth models and systems. The CoEvolutionary
Quarterly 34 (Summer): 98-108.
Moon, Wanki, Wojciech J. Florkowski, Bernhard Brückner, and Ilona Schonhof. 2002. Willingness to
Pay for Environmental Practices: Implications for Eco-Labeling. Land Economics. 78(1): 88-102.
Paich, Mark and John D. Sterman 1993. Boom, Bust, and Failures to Learn in Experimental Markets.
Management Science. 39 (12): 1439-1458.
Reisch, Lucia A. 2001. Eco-labeling and Sustainable Consumption in Europe—Lessons to be Learned
from the Introduction of a National Label for Organic Food. Consumer Interests Annual 47: 1-6.
Rönnbäck P. 1999. The ecological basis for economic value of seafood production supported by
mangrove ecosystems. Ecological Economics. 29: 235-252.
Rosenberry, B. 2004. World Shrimp Farming 2004, accessed online at
http://www.shrimpnews.com/WSF2004.html.
Russell, Clifford, Thomas Bue Bjørner and Christopher D. Clark. 2003. Searching for evidence of
alternative preferences, public as opposed to private. Journal of Economic Behavior &
Organization. 51 (1): 1-27.
Schwepker, Charles and T. Bettina Cornwell. 1991. An Examination of Ecologically Concerned Consumers and
Their Intention to Purchase Ecologically Package Products. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing 10
(2): 77–101.
Sterman, John D. 2000. Business dynamics: Systems thinking and modeling for a complex world. Boston,
MA: Irwin Professional Publishing.
188
Swallow, K. Stephen and Roger A. Sedjo. 2000. Eco-Labeling Consequences in General Equilibrium: A
Graphical Assessment. Land Economics. 76(1): 28-36.
Teisl, Mario, Brian Roe, and Robert L. Hicks. 2002. Can Eco-Labels Tune a Market? Evidence from
Dolphin-Safe Labeling. Journal of Environmental Economics & Management. 43(3): 339-359.
Thøgersen, John. 2000. Psychological Determinants of Paying Attention to Eco-Labels in Purchase
Decisions: Model Development and Multinational Validation. Journal of Consumer Policy.
23(3): 285-313.
Wessells, Cathy R., Robert J. Johnston and Holger Donath. 1999. Assessing Consumer Preferences for
Ecolabeled Seafood: The Influence of Species, Certifier, and Household Attributes. American
Journal of Agricultural Economics. 81 (5): 1084-1089.
Consumer Sector
Consumers with preference
for eco-certified shrimp
Consumers with no
preference
Non-certified Shrimp
Demand Sector
Price
Consumption
Non-certified Price-setting
Sector
Production
Production
costs
Price
Non-certified Production Sector
Eco-certified Shrimp Demand
Sector
Relative Price
Consumption
Price
Eco-certified Price-setting
Sector
Price
Production
costs
Production
Eco-certified Production
Sector
Mangrove Clearance
Environmental Impact Sector
Figure 1. Sector structure of model showing linkages between sectors.
189
R
Erosion fractional
rate
<Effect of Relative
Price>
Social norm building loop
Social pressure to
Remain Adopters
Erosion of
Adopters
Potential
Adopters
Adopters
Adoption Rate
B
R
Saturation loop
Social pressure loop
Social Pressure to
Adopt
PSA effectiveness
Effect of Relative
Price
Figure 2. Simplified Causal Structure of Consumer Behavior Sector.
1: prod eco cert shrimp
2: prod non cert shrimp
3: ADOPTERS
4: POTEN ADOPTERS
5: MANGROVE
2000000
1:
2:
3:
4:
5:
500000000
1:
2:
3:
4:
5:
250000000
3
3
4
10000000
3
1000000
2
3
5
5000000
4
5
1
1
1
5
5
5
2
1:
2:
3:
4:
5:
Page 1
4
0
0
0
2
1
0.00
3
2
1
10.00
20.00
30.00
4
2
40.00
4
50.00
Y ears
Base simulation
Figure 3. Base simulation. 1= Production of eco-certified shrimp (tons), 2 = Production of noncertified shrimp, 3 = Population of Adopters, 4 = population of Potential Adopters, 5 = Mangrove
stock (hectares).
190
1: prod eco cert shrimp
1:
2:
3:
4:
5:
500000000
1:
2:
3:
4:
5:
250000000
2: prod non cert shrimp
2000000
3: ADOPTERS
4
4: POTEN ADOPTERS
4
4
5: MANGROVE
4
4
10000000
1000000
2
2
5
5000000
2
2
2
5
5
5
5
1:
2:
3:
4:
5:
0
0
0
3
1
3
1
0.00
3
1
10.00
3
1
20.00
Page 1
3
1
30.00
40.00
50.00
Y ears
Simulation with no PSA
Figure 4. Continuous decay of mangrove stock when there is no increase in adopters of eco-certified
shrimp.
ADOPTERS: 1 - 2 - 3 - 4 1:
500000000
4
3
4
3
3
4
4
2
2
1:
3
250000000
2
4
2
1:
Page 5
0
1
0.00
2
3
1
1
1
1
10.00
20.00
30.00
40.00
50.00
Y ears
Sensitiv ity of PSA ef f ectiv eness on population of adopters
Figure 5. Sensitivity of the population of adopters of a range of values of PSA effectiveness.
191
MANGROVE: 1 - 2 - 3 - 4 1:
10000000
1:
5000000
1:
0
1
2
3
4
0.00
1
2
3
4
1
10.00
2
3
4
20.00
Page 4
2
1
3
4
3
1
30.00
4
2
40.00
50.00
Y ears
Sensitiv ity of mangrov e stock to PSA ef f ectiv eness
Figure 6. Sensitivity of mangrove to a range of values of PSA effectiveness.
1: ADOPTERS
1:
2:
2: MANGROVE
500000000
10000000
1
1
1
1:
2:
250000000
5000000
2
2
2
2
2
1
1:
2:
0
0
1
0.00
Page 1
10.00
20.00
30.00
Y ears
40.00
50.00
1:11 PM Fri, Jan 14, 2005
SImulation with PSA in one time pulse
Figure 7. Simulation of population of adopters and mangrove with one time pulse of PSA.
192
Globalization: For Better or Worse?
The Effects of International Trade on the Economy,
Physical Quality of Life and the Environment
Selima Ben Mrad, Florida Atlantic University
Hongwen She, Florida Atlantic University
Michael Mullen, Florida Atlantic University
Abstract
A great debate has emerged on the world stage about globalization and its consequences for society. We
examine the hotly debated question of globalization’s effect on the economy, the environment and on the
quality of the lives of ordinary people. Building on Mullen (1993, 1996) and Kilborne’s (2004) and
analytical frameworks, this study examines the effects of international trade (exporting and importing) on
the economy, physical quality of life and environmental pollutants. The multidimensional analysis uses
time-lagged data from more than 100 countries and two-staged least square regression models. We
consider direct, indirect, and total effects. The findings have practical implications for public policy
officials and provide an alternative empirical explanation of effects of globalization on the economy,
physical quality of life and the environment.
Introduction
In recent years we have seen heightened concern about the effects of globalization on ordinary people and
their environment. Anti-globalization demonstrators have campaigned against free trade in Seattle, Davos,
Cancun and Miami. They have consistently argued that an increase in international trade harms the
environment and that the economic benefits of increased trade accrue to the benefit of multinational
corporations and the international elite rather than ordinary citizens. Discussions over the benefits and
costs of globalization have been of interest to public policy makers and managers for many years.
Recently there has been a heightened interest among marketing academics in the role of marketing in
globalization and in the effects of globalization, for better or worse. McDonagh and Schultz (2002) make
the point that “Irrespective of conflicting sentiments about globalization, marketing – and its effects – are
globally visible”, (pg. 518). They point out that marketing academics ought to analyze strategic factors
contributing to sustainable, positive outcomes for the general population (McDonagh and Schultz 2002).
Further, the Journal of Macromarketing recently published a Special Issue (Vol. 24, No. 2, 2004) on the
topic of globalization and its consequences. In that Special Issue Kilbourne (2004) argued that substantive
freedoms, quality of life and an improved environment were precursors of economic development that led
to globalization with its free markets and trade liberalization. Kilbourne sees this process as continuous
with free markets and trade liberalization in turn leading to an improved quality of life (QOL),
environment and substantive freedoms. Hill and Dhanda (2004) and Cornwell and Drennan (2004) also
looked at the effects of globalization on the environment.
Shultz, Rahtz and Speece (2004) argue that globalization is inextricably linked to economic development
and quality of life. As globalization becomes more important, trade flows are growing and production is
shifting from developed countries to developing countries making people worry not only about their
economic future but also about the environment. Interest groups and their more vocal protestors have
heightened concern about the adverse effect of trade on the environment. This issue arose when the
NAFTA (North American Free Trade Agreement) and Uruguay round of GATT negotiations took place.
193
The main concern of environmentalists was global warming brought on by air pollution. The World Trade
Organization (WTO) seemed to strengthen the position of environmentalists against increased trade since
the WTO rules do not aim at the enforcement of environmental laws. Situations like the Dolphin-Tuna
case involving United States and Mexico is an example of the lack of enforcement of national
environmental laws by the WTO. The United States banned imports of Mexican tuna because Mexico had
not taken steps to reduce the number of Eastern Pacific Tropical dolphins killed each year due to tuna
fishing. Mexico appealed the case to the WTO where the panel ruled in favor of Mexico’s fisherman. As a
result some environmentalists argue that the WTO is an environmentally hostile organization and
increased trade under their auspices will likely harm the environment.
While some environmentalists argue that globalization worsens air pollution and harms the environment,
economists and trade policy analysts believe that economic growth generates an improvement in the
environment. Bhagwati (1993) and others argue that an increase in GDP results in a reduction of
pollutants and an improved environment. Wealthier societies are more likely to implement environmental
protections because they can afford the cost associated with implementation of environmental policies.
Economic growth provides countries with the resources to improve human health and adopt new
environmental technologies. If Maslows’ hierarchy of needs is considered, we see that people in poverty
are concerned about their survival rather than protecting the environment. However, when income rises,
people become more concerned about higher order issues such as the quality of the natural environmental.
Purpose
Stiglitz (2002) sees globalization as the diffusion of any process from its point of origin to points around
the world. Others elaborate on globalization as a complex, multidimensional construct (Shultz, Rahtz and
Speece 2004; Kilbourne 2004). Kilbourne, relying on Dicken 1998 and Jones 2000, defines globalization
as “the process through which nation-states become more economically, financially and culturally
integrated by the economic actions of transnational actors,” (2004: p.123). Here we use a similar
definition proffered by Shultz, Rahtz and Speece: Globalization is a process that “fundamentally is
entrenched in the dynamic relationship(s) among societies, governments, markets, marketers and
consumers throughout time,” (2004: p. 169).
The purpose of this study is to empirically measure the effects of globalization vis-a-vi international trade
(i.e., exporting and importing) on the economy, physical quality of life for individuals and the natural
environment (i.e., environmental pollution). Most previous literature has only linked environmental
pollution to economic growth without taking into consideration globalization variables such as
international trade. This study is a first step toward assessing the relationship between globalization
variables (exports, imports) on economic growth along with the environmental (i.e., pollution) and
physical and quality of life.
Literature Review
Debates over the costs and benefits of globalization have been of interest to societies, governments,
markets, marketers and consumers and their respective interested parties (i.e., labor unions, economists,
environmentalists, etc.). Opponents of globalization assert that it is characteristic of capitalism
undermining the nation state and exploiting rather than benefiting people (Beck 2000). Globalization is
also considered by some as a source of global injustice (Beck 2000; Cronin, 2003). According to
globalization dissenters, it is a force that delays the demise of capitalism at home and harms innocent
people living board (Bhagwati, 2002). Nader (1993) also noted that operating under the deceptive banner
of “free” trade, multinational corporations are working hard to expand their control over the international
economy and undo vital health, safety and environment protections won by the citizen movements across
the globe in recent decades.
194
In contrast with anti-globalization “protestors”, there are many pro-globalization advocates. Traditional
economic theories posit that globalization in the form of international trade benefits both parties by taking
advantage of relatively scarce or abundant factors of production resulting in increased production for the
benefits of all involved. Many scholars have found evidence against anti-globalization theories through
systematic study of data. Ellis (2001) argued that the rise of global corporations has increased business
efficiency, increased prosperity for many people, disseminated technology for many countries, and
expanded consumption possibilities, knowledge and awareness of different cultures among countries.
Research by Harvard University has shown that between 1970 and 1990, the economies of poor countries
with an open economy, grew by 4.5 per cent a year, while that of the autarkic nations stagnated at 0.7 per
cent (www.zenit.org). Similarly, for countries that took the road to the globalization, their average annual
growth rates accelerated from 1 per cent in the 1960s to 3 per cent in the 1970s, 4 per cent in the 1980s
and 5 per cent in the 1990s. While rich countries’ rates showed growth of about 2 per cent in the 1990s,
opponents of globalization saw their growth rate decline from 3 per cent in the 1970s to 1 percent in the
1980s and 1990s. Gardner (2000) stated that globalization is a powerful, dynamic, and complex
phenomenon that we cannot stop or control but may be able to diminish its harmful effects and reinforce
its benefits. Deliver (2003) showed that 90% of South Koreans and Kenyans, 88% of French and
Ghanaians, 87% from the U.K., 84% from Bangladesh, 79% of Mexicans and 78% of Americans think
that globalization is good for their own country.
Economic Development
International economic development has been of special interest for many decades in the macromarketing
literature (for a review see Wood and Vitell 1986) following the early work of Charles Slater (1968),
Johan Arndt (1972), Bartels (1977) and Cundiff (1982) among others. Much of that very broad economic
development literature points to the multidimensional aspects of economic development. Economic
growth is central to the broader construct of development and is featured in most text books and
macromarketing articles. Quality of life has also been widely discussed with dual focuses on the physical
quality of human life and on higher order aspect of quality of life, especially the natural environment (c.f.,
Lazer 1987). More recently, Kilbourne (2004) added substantive freedoms as an important additional
dimension for macromarketers to consider. We briefly discuss the first three dimensions of economic
development leaving the construct of substantive freedom as the subject of future research.
Economic Growth
Most economists, marketers and text books, consider GDP or GDP per capita as indicators of economic
output. Taken cross-sectionally across nations, these data can be used to study the causes and effects of
economic growth. Many scholars including sociologists (i.e., Bollen 1979, Sofranko et al. 1975) and
macromarketers (Mullen et al. 2001) argue that this measure may be distorted by foreign exchange
markets or the lack thereof (i.e, short term currency fluctuations or government interference). Consistent
with established practice (Mullen 1993, 1996), we use GDP per capita and with energy consumption per
capita as multiple measures of a country’s economic level.
Quality of Life (QOL)
There are many different views about the nature of quality of life. Quality of Life has been defined as “the
general state of wellbeing experienced by society's members” (Morris et al., 1995). Societal or country
QOL comprises two components - objective QOL (material conditions of life) and subjective QOL
(perceptions of wellbeing) (Sirgy, 1991). Miles and White (1998) referred to QOL as consumer wellbeing in the context of QOL marketing. Furthermore, Kilbourne, McDonagh and Prothero (1997) suggest
the necessity of analyzing QOL from the perspective of macromarketing, which entails three dimensionstechnological, economical, and political environments.
195
Sirgy (2001) stated that QOL is multidisciplinary including such perspectives as personal utility,
happiness, opulence, the just society, ecology, health, and culture. The study of QOL may focus on
individuals, families, communities, regions and or countries. Sirgy defined QOL marketing from the
perspective of consumer good firms as marketing designed to enhance customer well-being while
preserving the well being of other stakeholders. Lee and Sirgy (2004) define marketing “nonmaleficence”
as marketing decisions designed to preserve the well-being of other stakeholders – other than consumers
including the local community and the environment. In this study, we extend Sirgy’s (2001) definition to
include a public policy perspective. Here we are concerned with Sirgy’s second dimension, marketing
nonmaleficence, and examine the effects of marketing on the well-being of other stakeholders including
the local community and the environment from a macro perspective. However, we separate the effects on
individuals in the local community and the environment as different constructs.
Though many scholars mention QOL in their articles, few try to measure it. Morris (1979) maintains that
economic performance is not a suitable way of measuring human well-being. He argues that “GDP used
by the World Bank and USAID as a basic indicator of QOL is seriously flawed” because GNP overlooks
the differences in the prices and distribution of income in the world. Morris developed a Physical Quality
Of Life Index (PQOL) to address this problem by indicating how effectively people’s lives have
improved.
Morris’s (1979) index for PQOL (physical quality of life) has been generally accepted by the UN and
others studying international development. It is a composite of 3 indicators, namely, basic literacy (the
percentage of a population 15 years and older that is literate), infant mortality rate (per 1000 individuals),
and life expectancy at age one (where 0 represents the shortest life –expectancy recorded for any country
and 100 represents the longest forecast life expectancy). GDP per capita is positively correlated with the
physical quality of life index but is not part of the index. Interestingly, Morris’s (1979) index shows that
the quality of life has improved faster for many of the world’s poorest people than it has for people in
some of the world’s richest nations.
Recently Mullen (1993, 1996) used a similar composite to measure a population’s level of basic needs, a
construct related to physical quality of life. He used infant mortality rates, calorie consumption per capita
per day and secondary school enrollment ratios.
More recently, Peterson and Malhotra (2000) came up with a series of indexes to measure QOL, which
includes 6 factors: cost of living, health, freedom, economic prosperity, ‘curtainment’, (recreation,
culture, and entertainment), and infrastructure.
Given that we are measuring how the international trade effects physical quality of life, Morris’ PQOL
index is a better fit in the context of this study than Mullen’s or Peterson and Malhotra’s indexes and is
more widely recognized. While Mullen’s studies used indicators of basic needs rather than Morris’ (1979)
physical quality of life index, the theoretical content and intent of ‘basic needs’ and PQOL are the same.
Therefore, we will use Morris’ PQOL index in this study.
The Environment (Pollution)
Many studies have looked at the effect of economic growth on environmental pollution and have
demonstrated that pollution and economic growth are related to each other. Some studies (World Bank,
1992) have shown that air and water pollution usually initially deteriorate as GDP rises but improve as the
country attains a certain level of economic development.
The description of the relationship between trade and environment has been “borrowed” from “Kuznet’s
curve”. In 1955, Simon Kuznet postulated that economic growth and income inequalities follow an
196
inverted U-Shaped curve. This relationship has been applied to the environment showing the relationship
between pollution and income.
Gross and Krueger (1993, 1995) examined the relationship between per capita income and environmental
indicators by using cross-country data on air pollutants between 1977 and 1986 from the Global
Environmental Monitoring System (GEMS). They found that the turning point for different pollutants
varies and that in most cases countries reach a per capita income of $8,000 before pollution emissions
decrease. They came to the conclusion that economic growth generates a short-run period of deterioration
followed by a phase of decreased pollution and therefore environmental improvement.
Selden and Song (1994) used cross-national data on the emission of four pollutants (particulate matter,
sulfur dioxide, oxides of nitrogen, and carbon monoxide) and found that per capita emission of each
pollutant also had an inverted U-shaped relationship with per capita GDP. Besides these two studies
(Shafik and Bandyopadyay 1992), Holtz Eakin and Selden (1995) and Kaufman et al, (1998) presented
evidence that some pollutants have an inverted U-Curve linking pollution to income. However, the studies
showed differences in the income levels at which the U-curve takes an inverted turn.
While Beckerman (1992) stated that only economic growth could generate an improvement in the
environment, Arrow et al (1995) believed that economic growth is not a perfect replacement for
environmental policy. In addition, Torres and Boyce (1998) claimed that an equal distribution of power is
important to improving the environment and stated that a positive development in the level of education
and in political rights may also be correlated with income. De Bruin (1997) has linked income level to
environmental policies and found that environmental policies generate a reduction of sulfur dioxides
emissions. Furthermore Belmont (2002) believes that pollutants have different impact depending on
geographic location and original atmospheric and environmental conditions. He also found that an
improvement in education, income distribution, and information access are necessary conditions for
environment improvement.
The theory that as GDP increases, the quality of the environment improves (Bhagwati 1993) is deficient
according to Lofdahl (2002) because it does not include the effects of international trade. Lofdahl’s
(2002) analysis separates the effects of domestic GNP and trade related GNP. He finds that increases in
GNP leads to an improved national environment however, increase in trade related GNP are negatively
related to environmental quality.
Conceptual Model
The traditional macromarketing model holds that globalization with its free markets and trade
liberalization leads to increased economic development resulting in an improved quality of life and
environment (see Figure 1). For instance, Mullen’s (1993, 1996) empirically validated model holds that
increases in international trade lead to improvements economic development and in the physical quality
of life (i.e., basic needs). Kilbourne (2004) recently proposed an Expanded Macromarketing Model.
Kilbourne argues that the traditional model should be rearranged. He sees substantive freedom, QOL and
an improved environment as necessary building blocks that lead to economic development followed by
globalization (see Figure 1 for an adapted version of Kilbourne’s Model). The feedback from
globalization with its free markets and trade liberalization leads to improvements in QOL, the
environment and substantive freedoms feeding increased development. The essential difference in the two
models is the question of whether economic development is a cause or result of QOL, environmental
changes and substantive freedoms.
197
Figure 1
Traditional Macromarketing Model
Globalization
Free Markets
Trade Liberalization
Development
(Economic)
QOL
Environment
Kilborne’s (2004) Expanded Macromarketing Model
Constrained
Globalization
Free Markets
Trade Liberalization
QOL
Environment
Substantive Freedom
Development
(Economic)
While there is merit in Kilbourne’s (2004) model touting substantive freedoms and QOL as necessary for
economic development and globalization, we do not address the feedback loop and the precursors of
globalization (i.e., substantive freedoms) in this study. Beck (2000), Gardner (2000) and others make the
point that globalization is irreversible although its consequences are not. Therefore, we focus on the
outcomes of globalization (with it free markets and trade liberalization) on development of the economy
and on two of the three measure of societies well being in the traditional and expanded models, QOL and
the environment.
Following Lofdalh (2002), this study looks at the effects of trade related GNP (exports and imports as
percentages of GNP) and GNP per se, on the natural environment. We also follow Mullen (1993, 1996)
and Kilbourne’s (2004) Model’s examining the effects of international trade related GNP on development
(economic) and on quality of life. This multidimensional structural model allows us to also examine the
direct, indirect and total effects of international trade on three dimensions of development: economic,
QOL and the environment.
The diagram of the conceptual framework used in this study is in Figure 2.
198
Figure 2
Exporting
GDP/cap
Economic
Level
Environment Pollution
Importing
GDP/cap
Physical
Quality of
Life
Research Hypotheses
According to neoclassical trade theory, a country that has greater production efficiency stimulates more
productivity by exporting. Therefore, exporting activities are beneficial to the economy. The contribution
of export expansion to overall economic growth is that export expansion positively influences economic
growth because it will increase the resource allocation efficiency, increase capacity utilization, allow a
country to take advantage of scale economies and promote technical change and overall productivity
(Balassa, 1985 and 1988; Tyler 1981). In recent years, Edwards (1992 and 1993) has considered this last
effect implying that trade will positively affect growth because it exposes the developing country to new
ideas and techniques that can be used to improve new production methods and productivity. Therefore we
hypothesize that:
H1: Exports have a positive direct effect on economic growth.
Even though domestic consumers are better off with imports because they consume more products at
lower prices, local producers and the local economy are worse off (Carlton and Perlof, 2000, Mullen
1993, 1996). In fact local producers have to face foreign competitors. As imports increase, foreign
competition takes more business from local firms. As a result, domestic production will be adversely
affected. Thus, imports have an adverse effect on economic growth. Therefore we hypothesize that:
H2: Imports have a negative direct effect on economic growth.
199
Barkett (1985) noted that developments specialists acknowledge that high level of per capita GNP and
industrialization are not sufficient for meeting basic needs. He added that development experts (Morris,
1979) have introduced the physical quality of life index to measure how ordinary citizens of a nation are
doing. Following Mullen (1993, 1996), we believe that as the economic level increases, the better the
physical quality of life will be for the people of the country in question. As such, we expect that when the
economic level is high, the basic literacy rate will be higher, infant mortality will be lower and life
expectancy will be longer. Therefore we hypothesize that:
H3: Economic growth has a positive direct effect on the PQOL.
Following Mullen (1993, 1996) we expect exports to have a negligible negative direct impact on the
PQOL. However, combining the logic of Hypothesis 1 and 3, we expect a positive total effect of
exporting on PQOL. Since exports are expected to have a positive impact on economic growth, we think
that exports will indirectly impact quality of life through economic growth. By taking into account the
positive indirect effect of exporting via economic growth, we expect the total effects of exporting on
PQOL to be positive. Therefore we hypothesize that:
H4: Exports have positive (a) indirect and (b) total effects on the PQOL.
We do not expect any significant effect of importing on PQOL. However, since we expect imports to
have a negative impact on economic growth and economic growth to have a positive effect on PQOL, we
believe that imports will have a negative indirect effect on PQOL. By taking into account the direct,
indirect effect of imports on the PQOL via its effect on the economic level, imports will have an overall
negative effect on PQOL. Therefore we hypothesize that:
H5: Imports have negative (a) indirect and (b) total effects on PQOL.
Some researchers (Gross and Krueger, 1993, 1995) believe that economic growth is necessary for
pollution improvement. As countries experience economic development, the demand for environmental
quality is positively and strongly linked with income level. It seems that in the short-run period as
income rises, pollution worsens. Pollution deteriorates mainly because as income rises, the infrastructure
improves leading to more pollution due to transportation, and production increases leading to more use of
non-renewable resources and more greenhouse pollution. Additionally, if increased economic activity
leads to higher income, it might also lead to better environmental policies. In developing countries,
environmental laws are not undertaken until the governments determine that these nations can afford to
implement environmental policies. Expenditures on environmental cleanups only take place whenever
other basic needs are satisfied. Therefore we hypothesize that:
H6: Economic growth has a positive direct effect on the environment (i.e., a decrease in the level
of pollutants).
Trade increases the level of economic activity, which in turn increases the level of pollution. In fact,
developing countries switch from agricultural-based economies to industrialized economies.
Industrialization and production of goods for exports may in turn generate harmful chemical products and
increase the emission of greenhouse gases (Swami, 2001). Furthermore, countries trying to export will
enlarge their agricultural cultivation; build new facilities for production leading to deforestation,
generating air and water pollution. However, exports have a positive effect on economic development
because according to Mullen et al (1993), as countries start exporting goods, they expand their production
in the more productive sectors of the economy. As exports expand, the economy becomes wealthier and
income starts rising. As income rises, people become more environmentally concerned and work toward
cleaning their environment. Therefore we hypothesize that:
200
H7: Exporting has an indirect positive effect on the environment (i.e., a decrease in the level of
pollutants).
Mullen’s 1993 findings showed that imports result in a decline in economic growth. In fact, when a
country imports goods, the goods are by definition produced somewhere else. Since the goods are not
produced in the importing country, there are few pollutants generated domestically by these imported
products. Therefore we hypothesize that:
H8: Importing has a positive indirect effect on the environment (i.e., a decrease in the level of
pollutants).
METHODOLOGY
Data and measures
Complex relationships among marketers, countries, consumers and society may not be evident for years
after the exchange (i.e., exporting or importing) takes place (McDonagh and Schultz 2002). Mullen
(1996) demonstrated that the effects of exporting and importing on economic level and basic needs were
most pronounced with several year time lag. Therefore we analyze the effects of international trade from
1990 to 1994 on the three dimensions of economic development in1995, although the pollution data are
from various years. The data (1990-1995) on exporting, importing and economic growth are extracted
from the United Nations at http://www.un.com. The independent variables are measured from 1990 to
1994 and the dependent variables are from 1995 in order to show lagged effects, if any.
Independent variables are measured as the ratio of exports to GDP and imports to GDP. As Mullen
(1996) mentioned, ratios are used to better compare “across varying size of national economies”. With
regards to the dependent variables: the economic level is measured as a composite of GDP per capita and
energy consumption per capita; the PQOL is measured by using Morris (1979) index. For environmental
pollution, we ue data on Sulfur Dioxide (SO2) and Carbon Dioxide (C O2) emissions and water
pollutants. SO2 data are from 1990 and 1995 and C O2 data from 1990 to 1999. SO2 emission data were
collected from the website: http://humandevelopment.bu.edu and extracted from the World Bank
Development indicators 2002. For CO2, the data are gathered from the United Nations at
http://unstats.un.or/unsd. For water pollutants, the data are from: http://humandevelopment.bu.edu.
Missing values are deleted from analysis as mean substitute was not logical and other techniques such as
building up multiple regression models to predict the missing values would bias the results in favor of the
model. Thus, in the 5 two-staged least squares regression models, 105, 106, 114, 110, and 109 countries
were included in the analysis.
Methods
1) Normality. In order to make sure that all of the values are normally distributed rather than skewed
before performing multiple regression analysis, each variable is plotted showing all of them are positively
skewed (left-skewed). Therefore, the log transformation is used. Regarding economic level, the GDP/per
capita and energy consumption are then standardized and added up to form the score of a country’s
economic level as those two variables have different measurements units. Regarding the PQOL, a
country’s life expectancy at age one has been transformed into percent of its own value relative to the
greatest value. After these 3 indexes have been switched to percent, they are added up and log
transformed to ensure normality.
2) Linearity. To ensure that economic level has a linear relationship with exporting and importing, and
the PQOL or the environments have a linear relationship with exporting, importing, and economic level,
the graphic plot of each dependent variable with respective independent variable has been reviewed and it
201
indicates no obvious curvilinear relationship. Also, collinearity is also checked as there is no large (<10)
VIF value.
3) Two-staged least squares regression. As we need to put all the variables into the regression model and
economic level is effected by exporting and importing, we have to use two-staged least squares
regression. At the first-stage, we specify economic level as an endogenous variable predicted by
instrument variables, namely, exporting and importing. Then, both PQOL and environments are predicted
by economic level, exporting, and importing at the second stage.
First stage of the regression:
Economic level = intercept + β1*exporting + β2 *importing where exporting and
importing data are from 1990 to 1994 while data on economic level are from 1995
to indicate the lag effect.
Second stage of the regression:
The software will predict the economic level by exporting and importing and put
the estimated value in the models. The models are:
PQOL = intercept + β3 *economic level + β4 *exporting+ β5 *importing, and
Environment = intercept + β6 *economic level + β7 *exporting+ β8 *importing
where exporting and importing data are the same as the first stage, the economic
level is predicted at the first stage, and PQOL and environment data are in 1995.
Results
The results of the analysis on the effects of exporting and importing on economic growth, PQOL and the
environment (pollution) are in Table 1.
Table 1
Effects of
On 1995:
Economic
Level
(Dir.&Tot)
PQOL
PQOL
PQOL
Pollution
Pollution
Pollution
(Direct)
(Indirect)
(Total)
(Direct)
(Indirect)
(Total)
Exporting
1.97 a
0
0.13 a
0.13 a
0
1.48 a
1.48 a
(In 1990)
(In 1991)
1.23 a
0
0.11 a
0.11 a
0
1.39 a
1.39 a
a
a
a
a
(In 1992)
1.83
0
0.13
0.13
0
0.88
0.88 a
a
a
a
a
(In 1993)
2.57
0
0.18
0.18
0
2.06
2.06 a
(In 1994)
(NS)
(NS)
0
0
0
0
0
Importing
-2.08 a
(NS)
-0.14 a
-0.14 a
(NS)
-1.56 a
-1.56 a
(In 1990)
(In 1991)
-1.40 a
(NS)
-0.126 a
-0.126 a
(NS)
-1.57 a
-1.57 a
a
a
a
a
(In 1992)
-1.95
(NS)
-0.137
-0.137
(NS)
-0.94
-0.94 a
a
a
a
a
(In 1993)
-2.65
(NS)
-0.19
-0.19
(NS)
-2.12
-2.12 a
c
(In 1994)
-0.59
(NS)
0
0
(NS)
0
0
(Note: a = p-value < 0.01; b = p-value < 0.05.; c = p-value < 0.10 and NS = p-value > than 0.10.)
202
The results show that the exporting is beneficial to economic growth while importing is adverse to it from
2 to 5 years lag. We do not find an effect for exporting on economic growth with only a one-year lag
while the effect is consistent for the other years. These results confirm Hypotheses H1 and H2.
Neither exporting nor importing has significant direct effects on PQOL as expected. However, exporting
has an indirect positive impact via economic growth producing a positive total effect of exporting on
physical quality of life supporting H4. Importing has the reverse effects with negative indirect and total
effects on PQOL providing support for H5.
Similarly, neither exporting nor importing has significant direct effects on the level of these pollutants,
i.e., the environment. However, exporting is found to have an indirect positive impact on pollution levels
via economic growth (and thus negatively effects the environment) contrary to H7. Importing has the
reverse effects in that it is negatively related to pollution (and thus positively effects the environment)
providing support for H8.
Figure 3 graphically shows the effects of exporting on the economy, PQOL and pollution (labeled
environment) from 1990-1993.
Figure 3
Effects of exporting on economic level, QOL, and
environments.
3
2.5
Effect on economic level.
2
Effect on QOL
1.5
1
Effect on environment
0.5
0
90
91
92
93
Year
It is obvious that exporting has positive effects on economic growth, PQOL and pollution. Exporting from
1990-1993 has a small but positive cumulative direct effect on PQOL in 1995. Exporting in recent years
(1992 and 1993) has positive effects on the economy and the level of pollution. Unfortunately, that result
indicates a direct negative effect of exporting on the natural environment. Exporting in 1994 does not
have a significant impact in 1995.
Figure 4 shows the effects of importing on the economy, PQOL, and pollution (labeled
environment) from 1990-1993. Imports from 1994 do not have a significant impact in 1995.
203
Figure 4
Effects of importing on the economic level, QOL, and
environment.
0
-0.5
90
91
92
93
-1
Effect on economic level
-1.5
Effect on QOL
Effect on environment
-2
-2.5
-3
Year
Similarly, it is clear that importing has a negative direct effect on the economy, PQOL and pollution.
However, as importing is negatively related to pollution, it is inversely related to the natural environment,
as importing goes up, pollution goes down.
The regression showed no statistically impact of economic level on pollution for any of the three we
examines: SO2, CO2 and water pollution. To further test H6, we attempted to show the relationship
between GDP differences between 1990 and 1980 and SO2 emission difference between 1995 and 1990.
Table 2 shows the statistical results of regressing GDP differences on SO2 differences. The results
indicate that as GDP increases, SO2 emission decreases providing tentative support for H6. At that point,
the study shows that it takes time after an increase in GDP for the sulfur dioxide emission to decrease.
Table 2
Coefficientsa
Model
1
(Constant)
GDP10
Unstandardized
Coefficients
B
Std. Error
33.559
83.955
-3.77E-02
.017
Standardized
Coefficients
Beta
-.168
t
.400
-2.271
Sig.
.690
.024
a. Dependent Variable: SO2DIFF
In addition, we also explored the relationship between GDP differences between 1990 and 1980 and
PQOL difference between 1995 and 1990. The coefficient is 0.60 while the p-value of the test statistics is
0.06. This result shows that as GDP increases at the same time, PQOL also improves providing additional
support for H3.
204
Discussion
Many points have appeared of an interest in this study. From the data analysis it appears that the results
have shown more proof support for Mullen’s (1993) model. This paper has confirmed the relationship
between trade and economic growth. As exports increase, and imports decrease, GDP per capita and
energy consumption rise and the economy becomes more developed and wealthier. Furthermore the
consideration of the relationship between economic growth and physical quality of life has been of a
considerable insight. Previous research has shown that GDP per Capita is a determinant to Quality of Life
Index. However this study has examined the effect of economic growth as a separate variable in a
nominalogical net including trade variables relating it to Morris’s (1979) index. In fact it seems that as
exports rise, countries become wealthier generating a better life expectancy, a decrease in infant
mortality, and an increase in literacy rate. However, this study has provided mixed results on the
relationship between globalization, economic growth and environmental pollution.
Limitations and Future Research
This paper has examined the effects of trade and economic growth on quality of life and pollutant
emissions. The key insight of this study is that our results show a strong relationship between exporting
and importing on economic growth and therefore on physical quality of life. Public policy analysts and
academics should investigate more the benefits of economic development on the improvement of both
aspects of quality of life. Furthermore this study will provide more evidence to globalization advocates
and detractors alike about the benefit and cost of trade to the economic growth of countries and the wellbeing of people including their natural environment.
A limitation of this study is that environmental data were limited to few years. More data would
have shown the relationship between environmental pollution in developed and developing countries.
The studies of Grossman and Krueger (1993 and 1995) have not mentioned which control
variables they used to measure this relationship. However, Frankel and Rose (2003) recently proposed
that future research should include more complete environmental data and more control variables to
examine the same relationships.
REFERENCES
Anonymous (2002), “Globalization is Not an Ideology,” Institute of Public Affairs, 54(2), Page 32.
Arndt, Johan (1972), "Temporal Lags in Comparative Retailing," Journal of Marketing, 36(Oct.), 40-45.
Avner Offer, Ed (1997), “In Pursuit of the Quality of Life,” Journal of Macromarketing, Fall.
Balassa, Bela (1985), “Exports, policy choices, and economic growth in developing countries after the
1973 oil shocks”, Journal of Development Economics, Vol. 18, 23-35.
Balassa, Bela (1988), “The lessons of the Asian development: an overview”, Economic Development and
Cultural Change, Vol. 36, supplement 3.
Bartels, Robert (1977), "Marketing and Economic Development," in Macro-Marketing: Distributive
Processes from a Societal Perspective, Charles Slater, ed., Boulder, Colorado, 211-217.
Beck, Ulrich (2000), What is Globalization?, Polity Press: Cambridge, UK, in Kilbourne (2004).
Bhagwati, Jagdish (1993), “The Case for Free Trade,” Scientific American, 269(November), 42-49.
Bhagwati, Jagdish (2002), “Coping with Antiglobalization: a Trilogy of Discontents,” Foreign Affairs.
81(1), 2-.
205
Bollen, Kenneth A. (1979), "Political Democracy and the Timing of Development", American
Sociological Review, 44, 572-587.
Burkett L. (1985), “Systematic Influences on the Physical Quality of Life: A Regression Analysis of
Cross Sectional Data” Journal of Comparative Economics, 9, 145-163
Carlton Dennis and Perloff Jefrey (2000), “Modern Industrial Organization” Third Edition, Addison
Wesley.
Cornwell, T. B. and J. Drennan (2004), “Cross-Cultural/Consumption Research: Dealing with Issues
Emerging from Globalization and Fragmentation,” Jnl. of Macromarketing, 24(2), 108- 121.
Cromin, Christopher (2003), “Rethinking Globalization: Teaching for Justice in an Unjust World”.
Cultural Survival Quarterly, 27(2), 82-.
Cundiff, Edward W (1982), "A Macromarketing Approach to Economic Development," Journal of
Macromarketing, Spring, 14-19.
Deliver, Mark (2002), “The Parochial Nature of Anti-Globalization”, Adweek. Jul 2003, 44, 28.
Dicken, P. (1998), Global Shift, London: Paul Chapman.
Dollar, David and Aart Kraay, “Spreading the Wealth,” Foreign Affairs, 81(1), 120-.
Ellis, Vernon (2001), “Enterprise or Exploitation – Can Global Business be a Force for Good?,” The New
Statesman Lecture: 11 July.
Gardner, Steve (2000), “A Perspective: Which Way Globalization,” Baylor Business Review, 18(2), 18.
Hill, Ronald and Kanwalroop Kathy Dhanda (2004), “Globalization and Technological Achievement:
Implications for Macromarketing and the Digital Divide,” Journal of Macromarketing, 24(2),
147-155.
Jones, M. (2000), “The Contradictions of Globalization,” Journal of Australian Political Economy, 48(1),
5-22.
Kilbourne, William E. (2004), “Globalization and Development: An Expanded Macromarketing View,”
Journal of Macromarketing, 24(2), 122-135.
Kilbourne, William E., McDonagh, Pierre and Andrea Prothero (1997), “Sustainable Consumption and
Quality of Life: A Macromarketing Challenge to the Dominant Social Paradigm,” Journal of
Macromarketing, 17(1), 4-24.
Kilbourne, William E., McDonagh, Pierre and Andrea Prothero (2002), “The Possibility of Sustainable
Consumption,” in an Editorial on Global Policy and the Environment, Journal of
Macromarketing, December.
Lazer, William (1987), "Income and the Quality of Life Interface," in Marketing and the Quality-of-Life
Interface, A. Coskun Samli, ed., NY: Quorum Books, Green wood Press, Inc., 175-187.
206
Lee, Dong-Jin and Sirgy, M. Joseph (2004), “Qualtiy of Life (QOL) Marketing: Proposed Antecedents
and Consequences,” Journal of Macromarketing, 24(1), 44-58.
Lofdahl, C.L. (2002), Environmental Impacts of Globalization and Trade, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, as
reported in Kilbourne (2004).
McDonagh, Pierre. and Shultz II, Clifford J. (2002), “Guest Editorial: On Society, Market Logic and
Marketing,” European Journal of Marketing, 36(5/6), 517-522.
Morris M.D (1979) “Measuring the condition of the world’s poor: The physical quality of life index,”
Washington DC: Oversea Development Council.
Miles, Morgan P. and John B. White (1998), “Commentary: Setting Socially Irresponsible Marketing
Objectives: a Comment on a “Quality of Life Approach,” European Journal of Marketing, VOl
32, 413-417.
Mullen, Michael R. (1993), “The Effects of Exporting and Importing on Two Dimensions of Economic
Development: an Empirical Analysis,” Journal of Macromarketing, Spring, 3-19.
Mullen, Michael R., Eric Beller, Joseph Remsa, and Donald Cooper (2001), “The Effects of International
trade on Economic Growth and Meeting Basic Human Needs”. Journal of Global Marketing, 15
(1), 31-55.
Mullen, Michael R., Patricia M. Doney, and Thomas Becker (1996), “Time-Lagged Effects of Exporting
and Importing on Economic Development: Replication and Extension of Mullen’s (1993)
Model”. Journal of Macromarketing, Fall, 24-43.
Peterson, Mark, and Naresh Malhotra (2000), “Country Segmentation Based on Objective Quality-of-Life
Measures,” International Marketing Review, 17 (1), 56-67.
Shultz, Clifford II, Don Rahtz and Mark Speece (2004), “Globalization, Transformation and Quality of
Life: Reflections on ICMD-8 and Participative Marketing and Development,” Journal of
Macromarketing, 24(2), 168-172.
Sirgy, M. Joseph (2002) “Handbook of Quality-of-Life Research: an Ethical Marketing Perspective,”
Journal of Macromarketing, 63-64.
Sofranko, Andrew J., Michael F. Nolan, and Robert C. Bealer (1975), "Energy Use and Alternative
Measures of Societal Modernity," Sociology and Social Research, 59, 301-317.
Steingard, David S., and Dale E. Fitzgibbons (1995), “Challenging the Juggernauts of Globalization: a
Manifesto for Academic Praxis,”.Journal of Organizational Change, 8(4), 30-54.
Stiglitz, Joseph E. (2002), Globalization and its Discontents, New York and London: W.W. Norton.
Tyler, William G. (1981), “Growth and export expansion in developing countries, some
empirical evidence,” Journal of Development Economics, Vol. 9, 121-130.
Wood, Van R. and Scott J. Vitell (1986), "Marketing and Economic Development: Review, Synthesis
and Evaluation," Journal of Macromarketing, 6(1), 28-48.
207
Death Saliency and
Environmentally Friendly Consumer Behavior
Bertrand Urien, University of Western Brittany
Abstract
This study is aimed at attempting to answer the following question: Is it worth using a death
related message in order to encourage people to adopt ecologically friendly behavior and, more
specifically, to spend more money on products with positive environmental attribute? We will
first introduce the Terror Management Theory (TMT) along with its behavioral influences, and
then we will assess the nature of possible connections between a situation of "Death Saliency"
and a specific behavior i.e. the willingness to pay more to get ecologically friendly products and
services. Secondly, we will implement each construct and analyze the content of these links. We
will show that there are sex-linked differences in the adoption of such behavior by individuals in
a context of Death Saliency. Finally, managerial and academic implications will be considered.
Introduction
"Without nature, we have no future because our lives are all related", this was the message broadcast on
French TV channels during the summer of 2004, by the Nicolas Hulot foundation (a famous French TV
presenter ). This slogan was aimed at increasing public awareness of the necessity to protect the
environment and biodiversity. It showed silhouetted plants and animals falling like dominos and
threatening to knock over a man, a woman and a child (N.Hulot foundation, 2004). By using this to
underline “the possible extinction of human life”, Hulot open the door to speculations about the efficiency
of using death-related stimuli to drive people to encourage environmentally friendly behavior.
This study is aimed at providing some possible explanations to the following question. In order to
encourage individuals to behave in accordance with environmentally friendly principles, particularly
through the purchase of various products and services with ecologically friendly attributes, would it be
worth using a death related message as the communication axis ? This question is paramount. Cornwell
and Drennan (2004) recently outlined issues which emerge from the effects of globalization and
fragmentation and suggest that environment should be considered as one of the key areas in their program
of macro-oriented cross-cultural research. Specifically, globalization has raised serious concerns for the
environment in terms of production and consumption and they wonder what inducements and
encouragement for environmentally responsible behaviour are likely to be most effective. In this regard,
regulatory agencies are looking for ways to modify the consumption pattern of consumers. According to
Myers (1997, quoted in Cornwell and Drennan, 2004), media might play a role in promoting
environmentally conscious behavior.
Recently, a new light has been thrown on ecologically friendly behavior in two major ways : new
reflections on environmentally friendly behavior as a pro-social behavior (Lee and Holden, 1999) and the
use in this context of the Terror Management Theory (TMT), which is still relatively unknown in
marketing.
First, we will introduce this theory and its behavioral influences before considering the nature of possible
connections between a situation of "Death Saliency" and a specific behavior expressed by the willingness
208
to pay more for ecologically friendly products and services. Then, in the second part, we will implement
each construct in order to analyze these relationships. Finally, managerial and academic implications will
be considered.
TERROR MANAGEMENT THEORY (TMT) AND BEHAVIORAL ADAPTATIONS
In this part, after a brief description of this theory based on studies about the influence on behavior of
Death Anxiety, we will outline two behavioral reactions of prime importance: faith in cultural worldview
and an increase of self-esteem.
Death Anxiety and TMT
The first studies about the effects of death on consumer behavior were published about 15 years ago
(Hirshman 1990, Gentry et al., 1994; Gentry et al., 1995). Death anxiety was recently defined as a set of
negative emotional reactions of variable intensity caused by conscious and unconscious thoughts in
relation to the loss of self (Urien, 2003); it is considered either as a state or as a personality trait.
Greenberg et al. (1990) prefer to talk about anxiety generated by a state of “Death Saliency or Mortality
Salience (MS)”. A subject is said to be in a situation of Death Saliency when his thoughts in relation to
death lie out of his focal attention, in a state qualified by Freud as “preconscious”.
Though the Terror Management Theory was unknown in marketing untill 1999 (Mandel and Heine, 1999)
it nevertheless constitutes one of the main psychosocial theories published in the last 15 years. It is worth
noting that it has recently been the topic of a special issue of Journal of Consumer Psychology (2004).
First identified by Jeff Greenberg, Tom Pyszczynski and Sheldon Solomon (Greenberg et al., 1990,
1992), this theory has been the subject of more than 175 empirical studies. Specifically it states that Death
Anxiety is the emotional manifestation of the fundamental instinct of preservation of Self. Preservation of
the Self may, find fulfillment not only through direct means, e.g. remaining healthy, avoiding bad
food…., but most importantly, through symbolic means in relation to the culture of the individual. To be
more precise, there are two symbolic and closely linked mechanisms of self-preservation : i) unfailing
support of the worldview and of the values proposed by the culture of the individual and ii) an increase of
self-esteem, which is reached further to the adoption of culturally valued behavior.
Behavioral adaptation based on faith in a cultural worldview
The theory states that we build a culture, and take part in it so as to minimize concern associated with
death or the loss of self. If culture can offer immortality through religious faiths, it is also implicated in
the foundations of a cultural framework or of a "Worldview" composed of values clearly identifiable to
the individual. It is, in fact, a symbolic conception of reality, which strongly influences life in relation to
order, stability and permanence. Faith in this cultural vision of the world is the first way to decrease Death
Anxiety. Indeed, it allows us to feel unify with other people of the same culture, and thus to belong to a
structure larger than oneself. Furthermore, any person confronted by Death Anxiety will tend to defend
the value-system of their own culture as well as its various modes of expression, for example laws…..
Through a series of 6 experiments, Rosenblatt et al. (1989) showed that individuals experiencing
situations that confront them with their own mortality, have particularly positive reactions towards those
who share their values and the moral prescriptions of the culture in which they live, and particularly
negative ones towards those who challenge them. For example, it has been found that individuals forced
by someone to think about their own mortality, have a far more severe opinion of a woman accused of
prostitution than those who have not been forced to have this thoughts.
Maheswaran and Agrawal (2004) have recently presented some marketing implications. According to
them, under Death Saliency conditions, human beings may amplify their preferences for products or
brands that help them to consolidate their existing vision of the world. For example, in such situations,
brand loyalty could be encouraged. Another implication is resistance to persuasion. Under Death Saliency
209
conditions, individuals could reject comparative advertisements denigrating their favorite brands. They
could also prefer household products based on patriotic considerations, and therefore reject foreign brand.
Behavioral adaptation grounded on self-esteem
The more an individual adheres to and respects the values of his culture, the more his self-esteem
increases and the less anxious he feels about his own death. Experimental studies have shown that
behavioral adaptations could lead to: i) risky behavior (Orit Taubman Ben-Ari, 2000), ii) conspicuous
consumption and greed and iii) pro-social behavior. In this study, focus will be on the second two types of
the behavioral adaptations.
Increase of self-esteem, conspicuous consumption and greed.
A recent report (Arndt et al. 2004) pointed out that, further the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001,
Americans have bought more houses, cars, electrical appliances, furniture…, and therefore the annual rate
of consumer spending increased by 6% between October and December. Data analyzed through the
Terror Management Theory, explained that, following the events, in order to relieve Death Saliency,
subtly present in the fringes of consciousness, American citizens needed to increase their standing within
their culture by behaving in agreement with their values, e.g. materialism, which is strongly anchored in
the culture of their country. Such behavior would enhance self-esteem and relieve Death Anxiety by
reaching a type of symbolic immortality (Tomer, 1994). Two empirical studies conducted on a control
group and a group under Mortality Salience conditions are worth citing. The first one (Mandel and Heine,
1999) evidenced a higher interest among individuals under MS conditions for the purchase of luxury
items. The second experiment by Kasser and Sheldon, (2000; a study n°1), where the two groups where
composed of American students who were asked about the level of material wealth they assumed they
would possess in 15 years, resulted in the group under MS conditions expecting more material wealth
than the other one.
However, beyond a trend to materialism, money itself, far from being confined to its utilitarian role as a
means of exchange, would constitute "a new ideology of immortality" (Solomon et al., 2003). Though the
"quest for wealth" has, for a long time, been condemned by the Church because of the corruption
associated with personal enrichment, it is, according to Becker (in Solomon et al., 2003), the human way
to deny our mortality and bestiality. In a study with a suggestive title "Cash is King", Solomon and Arndt
(1993, in Arndt et al., 2004) show that a situation of Mortality Salience enhances an individuals greed for
money. During a walk, the group under Death Saliency conditions was more excited by finding a 20 $
note than the control group was. In other experiments by Kasser and Sheldon (2000) and Dechesne et al.
(2003) the avarice of the group under the MS conditions also increased.
Increase of self-esteem and pro-social behavior.
For Solomon et al. (1991), beyond the purchase of tangible symbols of status valued within particularly
materialist societies, other behavior also allow the increase of self-esteem ; socially promoted and valued,
desirable in most of the cultural visions of the world they are still qualified as pro-social behavior. They
are defined more exactly as behavior of help, sharing, charitable donations, involvement in charities and
other similar positive and deliberate behavior, which does not provide any direct profit for the people
who act in this manner (Baron and Byrne, 2002). Following this hypothesis, Pyszczynski et al. (1996) and
especially Jonas et al (2002) have tried to get a deeper insight into the nature of the connection between
pro-social behavior and Death Saliency. As indicated by Jonas et al. (ibid ) "most people learn very early
that being friendly and worrying or caring about those that are less fortunate than oneself is honorable and
highly desirable". Moreover, this is strongly encouraged by the rules of society and most religions, e.g.
Christianity, Judaism, Hindouism, Taoism, Confucianism (Shroeder, Penner, Dovidio and Piliavin, 1995);
indeed, generosity, empathy and helping are particularly valued within numerous cultures through
religious messages, emblematic people, or even popular myths and legends. On the other hand, numerous
studies have shown that adopting these behavior positively affect self-esteem. (Cialdini et al., 1997). Two
210
studies connecting pro-social behavior and the TMT have confirmed this hypotheses. Greenberg et al.
(1997), an Jonas and al. (2002) demonstrated using two studies that subjects in a Death Saliency situation
where they were asked questions in front of a funeral home, tend to respond more favorably to charities
as opposed to the other group which was located about 150 m from the funeral home. Jonas et al. (2002)
conducted a second experiment to test the influence of a Death Saliency situation on the amount of money
given by a subject and on the likelihood that a donation would be made to a national, rather than foreign,
charity. It confirmed these two assumptions: when individuals are placed in a Death Saliency situation,
they give more money, but only to national charities; this situation has no significant effect for foreign
charities.
Therefore, in reaction to Death Saliency conditions, an individual either exhibits more greed towards
ostentatious tangible items and money, or shows pro-social behavior demonstrated by financial support
for charities. How can one explain these contradictory reactions to Death Saliency? The first studies have
been focused on the effects of the hierarchy of individual values, but produced quite disappointing results
(Kasser and Sheldon, 2000, study n°1). Another route of investigation has been the influence of gender.
However, though most studies have been initially aimed at checking the whole set of behavioral reactions
to the Terror Management Theory, with no consideration for the gender of the subject under study, more
recent investigations (Hirschberger et al., 2002) have clearly shown that, according to gender, situations
of death-related anxiety can induce different behavioral reactions; Arndt et al. (2004) and Cozzolino et al.
(2004) have argued for the need for further investigations into this parameter. Kasser and Sheldon
(2000), then Dechesne et al. (2003) have both demonstrated that greed is increased, but only in men under
Death Saliency conditions, with no consideration for possible harmful consequences to the environment.
INFLUENCE OF DEATH SALIENCY ON ENVIRONMENTAL CONSUMER BEHAVIOR :
AN ILLUSTRATION RELATIVE TO “WILLINGNESS-TO-PAY BEHAVIOR "
One may wonder whether, individuals placed in a situation of Death Saliency, would be ready to pay
more to get ecological products or services or, would they be less prone to spend money to get them ?
Besides the significance of ecologically friendly and sustainable development in Management,
environmentally friendly behavior has recently been presented as pro-social behavior (Lee and Holden,
1999). On the other hand, according to Arndt et al. (2004) ecologically friendly behavior constitutes a
possible alternative to the Death Saliency-generated greed towards material items and money. Let us now
investigate the nature of possible links between Death Saliency and “willingness-to-pay-more for getting
ecologically friendly products and services”. In reply to Gianelloni (1998), who encouraged researchers to
pursue their investigation into the antecedents of environmentally friendly behavior, first, we will
introduce the elements in favor of a positive link, then those favorable to a negative link.
Environmentally friendly consumer behavior:
from environmental concern to ecologically sound actions
Environmentally friendly behavior can be broadly considered as a combination of environmental concern
and true ecologically friendly actions. As early as thirty years ago, as underlined by Giannelloni (1998),
Management Sciences and, more specifically, Marketing, started to get interested in environmentally
friendly behavior through concern about the purchase of products with environment benefits (recycled
papers etc), the recycling of household waste and energy conservation, for example (Samdahl and
Robertson, 1989). Kinnear, Taylor and Ahmed, (1974), started from the assumption that individuals who
show environmentally friendly behavior are particularly aware of these issues, then they identified what
motivated individuals worried about environmental issues. A literature review allowed Giannelloni (ibid )
to identify several factors and among them social or altruistic values in relation to benevolence (Granzin
and Olsen, 1991). All the studies about the relationship between environmental concern and true
ecologically friendly actions have found conclusive links, but, which are not very strong. Webster (1975)
underlined the lack of a strong connection between attitudes and behavior. The theoretical models used to
211
understand environmentally friendly behavior rely on the use of an attitudinal variable (environmental
concern) explaining a behavioral variable (the ecologically friendly action under study); this link of
causality is moderated by a set of psychosocial and socio-demographic variables. However, this seeming
simplicity hides a rather weakly explained variance. Therefore, several authors have criticized the lack of
a true framework which allows analysis and understanding of the factors at the origin of ecologically
friendly behavior (Lee and Holden 1999).
Berger and Corbin (1992) and Lee and Holden (1999) have expanded the original classification of
environmentally friendly behavior (category of products, or individual or collective actions) into three
classes of eco-friendly behavior: i) environment-positive consumer behavior, ii ) regulatory support
behavior iii) willingness to pay - more - behavior for eco-friendly products or services. The third type of
behavior investigated here is of key interest because individuals have to decide whether or not to spend
more for currently using environmentally-friendly products or services.
Positive connections between death anxiety
and willingness to pay more for environmentally-friendly products and services:
the pros.
These arguments should be examined through a new interpretation of eco-friendly behavior. Lee and
Holden (ibid) have recently presented environmentally friendly behavior as pro-social behavior. In social
Marketing, and more specifically in Consumer Behavior, the concept of pro-social behavior has already
been used either in the analysis of the decision process for making donations or in the case of social
causes marketing (Herdsman and Cunningham, 1999). But, according to Lee and Holden (1999), within
an ecological framework the pro-social aspect has been underestimated. Environmentally-friendly
behavior is time- and cost-consuming with, very limited direct financial compensation for the individual
who has adopted them. So, the benefits of personal actions are now, and ever increasingly shared by the
whole community (Lee and Holden, ibid). It is therefore interesting to use the pro-social behavior
framework for a better understanding of eco-friendly behavior. The model of pro-social behavior by
Batson (1987, 1991) is in this respect very helpful and was described, in his very first study, as the
behavioral expression of a dual condition, selfishness and altruism. The former occurs when someone
hopes to get a bonus, avoid a punishment, or even reduce personal embarrassment towards a difficult
situation experienced by a third party, whereas the latter takes place when someone wants to express
empathy or tentatively solve the problems of others. This "empathy-altruism" hypothesis is today Batson's
favorite (1997). A subject imagines and feels the way other people are, and will be, affected by
environmental issues. This condition can be relieved either by performing an environmentally friendly
action or by prompting a third party to make it.
So, by considering i) the developments relative to the symbolic mechanisms of self preservation
stemming from the TMT, ii) the theoretical and empirical positive links (Greenberg et al. 1997, Jonas et
al., 2002) between Death Saliency and pro-social behavior, including financial generosity and iii) the
works by Lee and Holden (1999) interpreting environment-friendly actions as pro-social actions, it seems
reasonable to think that individuals placed in a Death Saliency situation would agree to pay more for
environment-friendly products and services.
Negative connection between death anxiety
and willingness to pay more for environmentally friendly products and services:
the pros.
Based mainly on studies by Kasser and Sheldon (2000, 2003) and by Dechesne et al. (2003), as shown
above, Death Saliency conditions can enhance greed. This led Kasser and Sheldon to wonder whether it
was at other people’s and the environment’s expense. Therefore, they carried out research, which
ultimately demonstrated that individuals under Death Saliency conditions tended to over-consume rare
natural resources (Sheldon, 2000, study N° 2): during a simulated game of forest management, the
212
individuals under study were supposed to be at the head of a company in competition with three others to
cut down the trees of a national forest. Once they had read that intensive cutting generates short-term
profits, but causes long-term deforestation to the point that there would be no more trees, they were asked
which part of the 100-acre forest they would destroy during the first year. The results were enlightening!
The individuals under the Death Saliency conditions tended to cut down more trees than the control group
with the aim of generating more profits than their competitors. This was borne out in all tested individuals
regardless of their previous value system (Kasser and Sheldon, 2000). Dechesne et al. (2003) use the
same experiment model and drew the same conclusions, but noticed that only men behaved in that way.
These results show that in the event of a choice between earning more and behaving in an
environmentally friendly way, greed is, at least partly, amplified by Death Anxiety at the expense of
environmental well-being (Arndt et al., 2004). Even though the context here is different, it therefore
sounds reasonable to think that under Death Saliency conditions, consumers would be prone to keep their
money rather than spending more to get environmentally-friendly products and services.
On the other hand, one should note that, even though Kasser and Sheldon (2000) observed no sex-related
difference in behavioral reaction, this was not demonstrated in the experiment by Dechesne et al (2003).
Further to these results, it is worth noting that, in the event of choice between whether or not to spend
more in order to promote ecologically friendly behavior, a gender influence can be expected.
THE STUDY
To our knowledge, in marketing this study is among the first of its kind and certainly the first one in a
French-speaking context; it therefore constitutes an exploratory work. We have followed the methodology
used in all the classic empirical studies about the behavioral effects of the TMT; however, it is worth
mentioning that our sample was larger than usual with 130 individuals against less than 100 in most other
studies. It consisted of 61 males and 69 females; the average age of this population was 23 . Sixty six (n2)
out of the 130 individuals were randomly selected and placed in a situation of Death Saliency, while the
last 64 constituted the control population (n1)
Creation of Death Saliency conditions
TMT theorists have designed and tested several protocols in order to create Death Saliency conditions.
Two among these protocols are used most frequently. Rosenblatt (1989), and Greenberg et al., (1994)
asked subjects: i) briefly describe the feelings induced by thoughts about your own death, and ii) what
happens to you at the time of your death and once you are physically dead ? In such experiments, two
sub-samples are studied: the first one is asked to answer the two above-mentioned questions, whereas
individuals of the second sub-sample, i.e. “controls”, are requested to answer similar questions where the
words and expressions about death have been replaced by neutral expressions like watching TV or eating
in a restaurant. In the second method (Greenberg et al., 1990; Mandel and Heine, 1999) sub-samples
including a control group are under study; a scale of Death Anxiety is used not for Death Anxiety
assessment, but for conditioning the subjects in a situation of Death Saliency. Finally, other methods
previously tested may be used. For example, watching a film containing a particularly bloody car accident
on TV (Orit Taubman Ben-Ari, on 2000), questioning subjects in front of a funeral home (with a control
group further away), subliminal stimulus created by flashing the word “dead" across a computer screen
faster than conscious perception. One should remember, as previously mentioned, an individual is said to
be in a situation of Death Saliency when his thoughts in relation with death lie out of his focal attention,
in a stage qualified by Freud as “preconscious”. So, when a scale of death anxiety, for example, is used to
create a situation of Death Saliency (independent variable) with the aim of observing adaptive behavior
(dependent variable), it is of key importance to administer another measurement scale of any nature
straight afterwards. This will allow the attention of the individual under study to temporarily focus on
213
another object instead of consciously focusing on his own finitude. By shifting to the pre-conscious level,
any Death Anxiety felt will trigger the adaptive behavior identified by the TMT theorists.
In this study we placed half the sample population in a situation of Death Saliency by applying the two
most frequently used protocols. Appendix 1 introduces the scale of Death Anxiety administered to the
subjects and previously confirmed in a French context (Urien, 2001). Then, to strengthen this situation of
Death Saliency, the subjects were immediately asked: i) to briefly describe their future after their death
and ii) to briefly describe the feelings induced by thoughts about their own death. The Death Anxiety
scale was not administered to the individuals constituting the control group; moreover, in the questions
they were asked, the words and expressions in relation to death were replaced by eating several courses in
a good restaurant.
Assessment of “willingness-to-pay-more” for environmental products and services
As underlined by Gianelloni (1998), the assessment of ecologically friendly behavior is a delicate task.
One needs first to distinguish the status of environmental attitude from the subject’s intention to carry out
environmentally friendly or of effective actions. Concerning the exploratory character of this study, we
restricted it to the assessment of behavioral intention. As underlined by Berger and Corbin, (1992),
despite its limits this type of assessment provides fairly good estimates of effective actions because, in
such experimental case-studies the aim is not to provide descriptive and quantitative assessments of
behavior, but only to highlight the possible changes under certain conditions. Concerning the behavioral
intention, we used the indicator of “willingness-to-pay-more” for environmentally friendly products and
services, defined by Berger and Corbin (1992), and used later by Lee and Holden (1999) (Willingness- topay behavior). The interest in this indicator stems from the fact that the subject is placed in various
situations of consumption and has to decide whether or not he will spend more by consuming
environmentally friendly products / services; in the event he is not ready to pay more, it is to the detriment
of ecologically friendly actions. In our study, the scale was translated by a method of back-translation to
be sure of the intercultural validity of the measuring instrument. This 7-item indicator corresponds to one
of three scales created by Berger and Corbin (1992) to assess the behavior of environmental
consciousness; it was tested on 1521 Canadians considered as representative of the population. Lee and
Holden (1999) used it, and after testing it on a much smaller sample (78 students), confirmed its
unidimensionality and kept 6 out of the 7 items. The discarded item had a factorial weight lower than 0.3.
The scale reliability measured by α of Cronbach was 0.77 and 0.75 in the studies by Berger and Corbin
(1992) and by Lee and Holden (1999), respectively. Here, the 6-item scale was administered by means of
Likert's scale in 7 points presented in appendix 2
DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS
The analysis we used were like those employed in most of the experiments dealing with the behavioral
effect of Death Saliency conditions
A principal component analysis was applied to items measuring willingness to pay more for
ecologically friendly products. We kept only the components with an eigenvalue greater than 1; this
provided us with only one factor (eigenvalue equal to 2. 962) as the other eigenvalues were less than 1.
All the factorial weights of items being greater than 0.6 were kept. Calculation of Cronbach’s α gave
0.78, a value acceptable in terms of reliability. By taking into account this value, we kept the average of 6
items as a composit variable (WILTOPAY); its characteristics were: Average: 4; Standard
deviation: 1.05; Median: 4; Skewness: - 0.23; Kurtosis: 0.410. These results show that this variable has a
distribution close to normality.
An univariate variance analysis ANOVA 2X2 was then carried out on keeping the metric
composite variable as the dependent one (WILTOPAY) and using the state of Death Saliency and gender
214
(two nominal variables each with two modalities, 1 (no) or 2 (yes) and 1 (man) or 2 (woman)
respectively) as independent factors. Table 1 gives the results of ANOVA design, the distribution of
observations according to the various cells, and the average of the dependent variable by cell.
TABLE 1
Averages and Standard Deviations of Dependent Variable by Cell
Standard
SEX
MS
Average
Deviation
1
1
3.8925
1 , 18
31
2
4.4833
1 , 07
30
Total
4.1831
1 , 16
61
1
4.1465
1 , 04
33
2
3.5787
0 , 74
36
Total
3.8502
0 , 93
69
Total
MS1
4.0234
1 , 11
64
Total
MS2
3.9899
1 , 01
66
Total
4.0064
1 , 05
130
2
N
MS: 1 (situation of control); 2 (situation of death saliency)
SEX: 1 (Male); 2 (Female)
The H0 hypothesis expressing that the variance of errors of the dependent variable is equal in the various
groups, was assessed using the Levene Test of equality on the variances of errors tests. It demonstrated
the respect of this constraint (F (3, 126) = 1, 956, ns). In addition to the normality of the distribution of
this variable, that this constraint was satisfied, allows us to see the results of the variance analysis.
According to these results neither Death Saliency (F (1, 126) = 0.004, ns), nor sex (F (1, 126) = 3.316, ns)
demonstrated a significant effect on the willingness-to-pay more for environmentally friendly products
and services. On the other hand, Figure 1 clearly shows a marked “Mortality Salience” x
“sex” interaction (F (1 , 126) = 10 , 515 ; p = 0,002)
215
E stim a ted a v era g es o f W IL T O P A Y
4 ,6
4 ,4
4 ,2
4 ,0
3 ,8
SEX
3 ,6
1
2
3 ,4
1
2
MS
MS: 1 (control); 2 (death saliency)
SEX: 1 (Male); 2 (Female)
FIGURE 1
INTERACTIVE EFFECT OF SEX AND DEATH SALIENCY ON WILLINGNESS
TO PAY MORE FOR ECOLOGICAL PRODUCTS AND SERVICES
Analysis of Figure 1 and the averages of the variable with post hoc tests (contrasting effect) shows that
Death Saliency condition (MS2) affects the tested ecological behavior. In contrast to a female in a
situation of Death Saliency, under the same conditions a male has a 25% greater score of "willingness-topay-more"(3 , 57 against 4 , 48 ; test t = 3 , 91, This difference is significant for α= 1 % and 64 degrees of
freedom)
DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS
In conclusion this experiment data highlights sex-related differentiation of the adaptive reactions due to
condition of Death Saliency. These results confirm those issued from the few TMT studies about this
issue (Hirschberger 2002, Dechesne et al., 2003). However, there is no agreement between these results
and those of Dechesne et al (ibid ): indeed, in situations of Death Saliency we showed that females had an
opposing reaction to that of males, who tended to be more prone to purchase environmentally friendly
products and services. Mortality Salience seems to lead to a reversal of the direction of natural order. This
reaction has already been noted by Jonas et al (2002). The results regarding women however, are
surprising as they are widely considered to have strong links with the environment. In this regard, Dobsha
et Ozanne (2001) even suggest the existence of eco-feminism. Further experiments are needed to confirm
or contradict our observations. In addition, to our understanding, the data in the available literature does
not allow us to interpret this result satisfactorily. A possible interpretation, relative to the fact that women
"give life" might also suggest a special connection with death. Exploratory research on female sample
216
could lead us to better understand the deep nature of these questions. However, if/once these results are
confirmed, the managerial implications will be immediate. If an advertising campaign, aimed at
encouraging ecologically friendly behavior, is thinking about basing its message on a death-related
stimulus, it is advisable for the decision-makers to be very careful: paying special attention to the target
audience and the theme of the message. As we have seen that men and women react very differently when
put in the same situation and forced to make choices. Specifically when deciding, whether or not to spend
money on ecologically friendly products/ services. The question of using death in promotional tool
requires some further comments.
We have seen that in Death Saliency situations, individuals can adopt either prosocial behavior (Jonas and
al, 2002), or materialistic behavior (Kasser and sheldon, 2000). Of course, at a societal level, the aim is to
support the former, for the well-being of society and the defense of our ecosystem. Moreover, Kasser and
Sheldon (ibid) have shown that materialistic people display anti-ecological behavior.
If we succeed in pushing people towards prosocial behaviour, using Death Saliency to manipulate people,
poses an ethical problem. Ethics can be considered on three levels: intention, method and effect. If putting
people in Mortality Salient conditions to increase sales is against enlightened marketing (Kotler, 1996), it
could be justified putting people in Death Saliency situation for altruistic purposes. As Mandel and Heine
write, it is important for the marketer – and the macromarketer – to determine whether these methods will
be used in a positive manner: increasing membership to volunteer organizations, making donation etc…
“an awareness of mortality can be a springboard leading to meaning and health rather than a trapdoor
leading to dissolution and destruction” (Rindfleisch and Burroughs, 2004)
CONCLUSION
It can be seen from the various evidence, both primary and secondary, used in this study, that the answer
to the question: Is it worth using a death related message in order to encourage people to adopt
ecologically friendly behavior and, more specifically, to spend more money on products with positive
environmental attributes? does not have a simple answer. However, the implications of this study are
numerous. Some are cited below.
From a theoretical point of view, it underlines recent developments in the Terror Management
Theory, a theory only known to very few people interested in Consumer Behavior though it opens the
ways to innovative interpretations of marketing concepts like materialism (Rindfleisch and Burroughs,
2004, Ferrandi et al., 2003), attitude towards money (Urbain, 2000), "prosocial" behavior (Lee and
Holden, 1999), or the effects of “made in”.
As regards methodology, this is, to our knowledge, the first study carried out in a Frenchspeaking context which uses the experimental protocols of the TMT to investigate consumer behavior.
The previous studies concerned mainly American, Asiatic, Israeli individuals, and even Australian
Aborigine subjects.
The results demonstrated by our research show a sex-related difference in attitude towards the
adoption of environmentally friendly conduct induced by Death Saliency conditions, in our sample.
Some questions, needing further investigations, have arisen from this study:
The protocol was implemented on a specific sample. External validity of results needs to be
pursued through the questioning of other sample groups.
It would be worth deviating from the simulated context in order to create “true” conditions by
measuring the impact on the purchase of environmentally-friendly products/ services by individuals in a
real situation of Death Saliency.
217
Another question deals with willingness-to-pay surveys. According to O’Neill (1993),
commitments to others, - including items of value such as the preservation of landscape, species and so on
-, are displayed by the refusal to treat them as commodities that can be bought or sold. Such
considerations can explain that some respondents either refuse to put a price on an environmental product,
or put an infinite price on it. In this study, however, we have not noticed such behavior. But such
considerations should be taken into account for further surveys, carried out on other samples.
In other respects, the use of covariates such as personal value-system would be further investigated for the
understanding of the mechanisms involved in the activation of adaptive behavior when faced with death.
In every case, the increasing number of death-related stimuli in today’s media: news reports, novels,
crime fictions, documentaries…often puts us in a Death Saliency situation. So, the behavioral
consequences of the TMT, including our consumer actions “should occupy an army of investigators for,
at least, a decade" (Arndt et al., 2004)
REFERENCES
Arndt J., Solomon S., Kasser T.,Sheldon K.M. 2004 a. The Urge to splurge : a Terror Management
Account of Materialism and Consumer Behavior, Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(3), 198-212
Arndt J., Solomon S., Kasser T.,Sheldon K.M. 2004 b. The Urge to splurge revisited : Further reflections
on Applying Terror Management Theory to Materialism and Consumer Behavior, Journal of
Consumer Psychology, 14(3), 225-229
Baron R.A. et Byrne D. 2002. Social psychology, 10th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Batson, C.D. 1987. Prosocial motivation: Is it ever altruistic? (pp. 65-122). In L. Berkowitz (ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press.
Batson C. 1991., The altruism question : toward a social-psychological answer, Hillsdale, NJ : Erlbaum
Batson, C., Sager, K., Garst, E., Kang, M., Rubchinsky, K. & Dawson, K. 1997. Is empathy-induced
helping due to self-other merging ? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 495-509.
Berger, I. 1999. Cunningham, P., "Consumer Persuasion Through Cause-Related advertising” in Eric
Arnould and Linda Scott (eds.), Advances in Consumer Research , Vol. XXVI, 491-497.
Berger I.E. et Corbin R. M. 1992. Perceived consumer effectiveness and faith in others as moderators of
environmentally responsible behaviors, Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, vol 11, 2, 79-84
Cialdini R., Brown S., Lewis B., Luce C. et Neuberg S. 1997. Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism
relationship : When one into one equal oneness, The Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, vol 73
Cornwell T B., Drennan J. 2004. Cross–cultural consumer/consumption research : dealing with issues
emerging from globalization and fragmentation, Journal of Macromarketing, 24 (2), 108-121
Csikszentmihalyi M. 2003. Materialism and the evolution of consciousness, in T.Kasser & A.D Kanner
(Eds) Psychology and the culture of Consumption, AMA, Washington D.C
218
Dechesne M., Pyszczynski T. Arndt J. Ransom S. Sheldon S. Van Knippenberg A. et Janssen J .2003.
Literal and symbolic immortality : the effect of evidence of literal immortality on self-esteem
striving in response to mortality salience, Journal of personality and social psychology, 84, 4,
722-737
Dobscha S, Ozanne JJ.L 2001. An ecofeminist analysis of environmentally sensitive women using
qualitative methodology : the emancipatory potential of an ecological life, Journal of Public
Policy & marketing, .20 (2) : 201-214
Ferrandi J.M. Louis D. Valette-Florence P. 2003. L’influence des valeurs personnelles et du matérialisme
sur les motivations en matière de don de cadeau, Actes du Congrès international de l’AFM, Eds
D. Merunka, Tunis.
Gabel T.G., Mansfield P. et Westbrook K 1996. The Disposal of consumers : an exploratory analysis of
death-related consumption, Advances in Consumer Research, 23, Eds. Kim Corfman and John
Lynch, Association for consumer research, 361-367
Gentry J. W., Kennedy P.F., Paul K. et Hill R.P. 1994. The vulnerability of those grieving the death of a
loved one : implications for public policy, Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 13, 1, 128142.
Gentry J.W, Baker S.M. et Kraft F.B 1995. The role of possessions in creating, maintaining and
preserving one’s identity : variation over the life course, Advances in Consumer Research, 22,
Eds. Frank Kardes and Mita Sujan, Association for Consumer Research, 413-418
Giannelloni 1998. Les comportements liés à la protection de l’environnement et leurs déterminants : un
état des recherches en marketing, Recherche et Applications en marketing, vol.13, 2, 49-72
Granzin, K.L. and Olsen, E.J., 1991. “Characterizing Participants in Activities Protecting the
Environment,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 10(Fall), 1-27.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S., et Lyon, D. 1990.
Evidence for terror management theory II: the effects of mortality salience on reactions to those
who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
58, 2, 308- 318.
Greenberg, J., Simon, L, Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., et Chatel D. 1992. Terror management and
tolerance: does mortality salience always intensify negative reactions to others who threaten one's
worldview ? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 2, 212-220.
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Rosenblatt, A., Burling, J., Lyon, D., Simon, L., & Pinel, E.
1992. Why do people need self-esteem? Converging evidence that self esteem serves an anxietybuffering function, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 6, 913-922.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S.., Simon, L., and Breus, M. 1994. Role of consciousness and
accessibility of death-related thoughts in mortality salience effects, Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 67, 4, 627-637
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. 1997. Terror management theory of self-esteem and
cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements, In M.P. Zanna (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology, New York: Academic Press, Vol. 29, 61-139
219
Hirschberger G, Florian V., Mikulinger M., Goldenberg, J et Pyszczynski T. 2002. Gender differences in
the willingness to engage in risky behavior : a terror management perspective, Death studies, 26,
2, 117-141
Jonas, E., Schimel, J., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. 2002. The Scrooge effect: Evidence that mortality
salience increases prosocial attitudes and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
28, 1342-1353.
Kasser, T. et Sheldon, K.M. 2000. Of wealth and death : materialism, mortality salience and consumption
behavior, Psychological Science, 11,4, 348-351
Kasser, T., Ryan, R.M., Couchman, C., & Sheldon, K.M. 2003. Maerialistic values : their causes and
consequences, in T.Kasser & A.D Kanner (Eds) Psychology and the culture of Consumption
Kopp S. W. et Pullen B.K..2002. Death Goods : Death anxiety prevents purchase and preparation,
American Marketing Association Winter Educators’ Conference, Austin, Texas, 273
Lee, J.A and Holden, S.J.S 1999. Understanding the determinants of environmentally conscious behavior,
Psychology and Marketing, 16(5), 373-392
Maheswaran D et Agrawal N.2004. Motivational Cultural Variations in Mortality salience Effects :
Contemplations on Terror Management Theory and Consumer Behavior, Journal of Consumer
psychology, 213-218
Malhotra, N., Decaudin, J.M., Bouguerra, 2004. A., Etudes marketing avec SPSS, Pearson Education,
Mandel N. et Heine S. J. 1999. Terror management and marketing : he who dies with the most
toys wins, Advances in consumer research, 26, Eds. Eric J. Arnould and Linda M. Scott,
Association for consumer research, 527-532
O’Neill J. 1993. Ecology, Policy and Politics, New York, Routledge
Orit Taubman Ben-Ari 2000. The Effect of reminders of death on reckless driving : A Terror
Management Perspective, Current directions in psychological Science, 9,6, 196-199
Price L.L, Arnould E.J et Curasi C. F. 2000. Older Consumers’ disposition of special Possessions,
Journal of Consumer Research, 27, 2, 179-202
Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. 2003. In the wake of September 11: The Psychology of
Terror. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association
Rosenblatt A., Greenberg J., Solomon S., Pyszczynski T.et Lyon, D.1989. Evidence for terror
management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or
uphold cultural values, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 4, 681-690.
Samdahl D. M. et Robertson R. 1989. Social determinants of environmental concern : specification and
test of the model, Environment and Behavior, 21, 1, 57-81
Shroeder D.A., Penner, L., Dovidio, J.F., Piliavin, J.A 1995. The psychology of helping et altruism :
problems and puzzles, New York, McGraw-Hill.
220
Solomon S., Greenberg J. et Pyszcyzynski T. 1991. A terror management theory of social behavior : the
psychological functions of self-esteem and cultural worldviews, Advances in experimental social
psychology, Ed M. Zanna, 24, 93-159
Solomon S. Greenberg J. et Pyszcyzynski T. 2003. Lethal Consumption : death-Denying Materialism, in
Psychology and Consumer Culture: The Struggle for a Good Life in a Materialistic World,
edsTim Kasser, Allen D. Kanner, Sheridan books, Ann Arbor, MI
Tomer A. 1994. Death anxiety in adult life : theoretical perspectives, Death anxiety handbook: Research,
instrumentation, and application, ed R. A. Neimeyer, Washington DC, Taylor et Francis, 3-28
Tomer A. 1994. Death anxiety in adult life : theoretical perspectives, Death anxiety handbook: Research,
instrumentation, and application, ed R. A. Neimeyer, Washington DC, T & F, 3-28
Urbain 2000. L'attitude à l'égard de l'argent : une première tentative de validation de deux échelles de
mesure américaines dans un contexte culturel français, Recherche et applications en Marketing,
15, 3, 3-28
Urien B. 2001. L’influence de l’anxiété face à la mort sur le comportement exploratoire du
consommateur, Actes du 17ème congrès international de l’Association française du marketing,
Ed. J.Brée, Deauville,
Urien B. 2003. L’anxiété face à la mort et son influence sur le comportement du consommateur,
Recherche et Applications en Marketing, vol.18, 4, 23-41
APPENDIX 1
SCALE EXPERIMENTALLY USED TO PLACE FRENCH INDIVIDUALS
UNDER DEATH SALIENCY CONDITIONS
( Urien 2001, adapted from Dickstein, 1972)
M1. La perspective de ma propre mort fait naître en moi un sentiment d’anxiété
M2. La perspective de ma propre mort me déprime.
M3. J’envisage ma propre mort comme une expérience douloureuse et cauchemardesque
M4. J’ai peur de mourir
M5. Le fait d’être mort me fait peur
221
APPENDIX 2
SCALE OF « WILLINGNESS TO PAY MORE FOR ECOLOGICAL PRODUCTS AND SERVICES”
(Berger et Corbin 1992) Translated and adapted in French language
Dans l’ intérêt de protéger
l'environnement, seriez-vous prêts
à…
Payer cinq centimes d’euro de plus,
un litre d’essence, pour diminuer la
pollution de l’air ?
Payer 10 % plus cher pour des
produits d'épicerie emballés et
fabriqués dans des conditions
environnementales saines ?
Payer 1000 euros de plus pour une
voiture qui émet beaucoup moins de
pollution dans l’air ?
Payer 50 % de plus pour le ramassage
des déchets de façon à ce qu’ils soient
entreposer dans des conditions sûres,
à long terme ?
Payer 250 euros d’impôts en plus par
an, pour le nettoyage des égouts dans
votre quartier ?
Payer 10 % d’impôt sur toute
l'énergie que vous employez pour
encourager les gens à ne pas la
gaspiller?
222
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
pas du
tout
d’accord
pas
d’accord
plutôt
pas
d’accord
neutre
assez
d’accord
d’accord
tout à fait
d’accord
__________________________________________________
“Plant a tree, save the world” –
Hobson’s choice or an environmental citizen?
John Connolly, Dublin Institute of Technology
Andrea Prothero, University College of Dublin
Abstract
This paper presents a study of green consumption in Ireland. It explores the concepts of individual
responsibility taking and collective action, which tend to be presented as opposites in the literature. Our
qualitative study shows that green consumers engaged in both sets of activities in their everyday lives.
Examples of such activities are presented and what this means for our theoretical understanding of green
consumption.
Introduction
With consumption now seen as a key driver of environmental degradation (UNDP, 1998) consumers have
been allocated joint responsibility for ecological reform (Halkier, 1999). Many environmentalists have
advocated this strategy over the last 35 years (e.g. Holliman, 1970) and believe it is vital to achieving
ecological sustainability. According to Murphy (2001) consumer responsibility is seen as a very good
starting point if the aim is to achieve the integration of environmental considerations into consumption
practices. Thus, the individual consumer, family or household is viewed as the agent of change (Halkier,
2001).
Calls for individuals to accept personal responsibility for ecological reform were particularly salient in the
late 1980s and 1990s when a plethora of green consumer guides became available to aid consumers in
greening their consumer choices (Connolly et al, 2004). This was the heyday of green consumerism and
green marketing (see Prothero, 1990; 1998). At the same time, this form of green political consumerism
has also been questioned as to its ability to constitute change. Critics of individual responsibility, such as
Maniates (2002), believe that it legitimates the idea of consumer sovereignty and a self-balancing and
autonomous market, thereby diverting attention from political arenas that matter.
However, the idea of “shopping for a better world” (Carrigan, et al, 2004) as a strategy has not declined:
in fact its ideology appears to have spread. This paper begins by outlining the perspectives of both the
advocates and critics, of what Maniates (2002) refers to as the “individualisation of responsibility” thesis.
We refer to individualism as that descriptor that emphasises the importance of the individual, and the
value attached to both individual freedom and individual choice, essentially the political philosophy of
liberal individualism.
The second part of the paper provides some insights on this subject from a recent empirical study
conducted in Ireland on the wider issue of green consumption.
223
THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
The case of consumer responsibility
The discourse of environmental action manifested through individual consumer concern is now a central
component in state policies (Hobson, 2002). Here, the solution to ecological sustainability is manifest
through individual action, where green and ethical consumerism will be sufficient to transform markets to
produce continual and "clean" economic growth (Seyfang, 2004). This is part of what Hobson (2001,
2002) believes is an attempt to make the environment part of an individual’s consumption preferences,
achieved through the "4 Rs" of environmentally responsible behavior—redesign, reduce, reuse, and
recycle (Amine, 2003). Three of these “Rs” are currently part of a public policy advertising campaign in
Ireland.
The consumption of green products/services is also part of this agenda. Here, green consumerism works
as a ballot. It is even equated with a form of direct democracy (Beck, 1996). According to Nava (1991)
“…it offers ordinary people access to a new and very immediate democratic process: “voting” about the
environment can take place on a daily basis…they are able through their shopping to register political
support or opposition. Furthermore, they are able to exercise some control over production itself, over
what gets produced and the political conditions in which production takes place” (p.168). This invariably
allocates consumers with the individual power and agency to transform the current situation. Indeed, in
Moisander and Pesonen’s (2002), assessment, this discourse of green consumerism positions individual
consumers as powerful actors. Moreover, the discourse appears to argue that:
Consumers have a significant role and power in the market;
• Individual consumers can make a significant contribution to sustainable development by doing
small good deeds;
• The informed, rational and environmentally friendly choices of everyday private life constitute
those small good deeds;
• Social change, and thus sustainable development, can be achieved through those rational and
responsible choices;
• People who make those choices are morally responsible green consumers (Moisander & Pesonen,
2002).
The faith that many consumers/citizens3 place in individual responsibility also appears to be founded
upon a belief in the efficacy of the system (Eden, 1993; Halkier, 1998, Shaw & Clarke, 1999). Indeed,
Shaw’s (2004) study on ethical consumers found that although acting individually, ethical consumers
believed they were part of a wider collective of concerned consumers. This appears to support Halkier’s
(1998) contention that green consumers feel they belong to a diffuse, imagined community linked to other
unknown consumers, whose consumption behaviour is as environmentally responsible as their own. It
appears that this image of such a community of active co-consumers makes single activities make sense to
individual consumers, where they are dependent on one another’s actions when it comes to pressurising
producers and retailers in a more sustainable direction.
It is hardly surprising then, that not only has the number of green/ethical labels increased but so too has
the literature in this area (e.g. McDonagh, 2002; Renard, 2003; Micheletti, 2003; Jordan et. al, 2004;
Crane, 2005). Critics perceive this as creating a reality for change within the market as opposed to
outside, with labels providing the information required by consumers to exercise power against the social
conditions they agree/disagree with.
3
It is important to note that consumers and citizens are not dichotomous positions and have a long history of being
intertwined identities (see Trentman, 2001).
224
For other advocates (Kennedy, 2004; Follesdal, 2004; Micheletti, 2003) the individuality of responsibility
is less about the adoption of the specific form of power relations, rather that, no alternative option actually
exists. They locate the issue within the current socio-political conditions of contemporary societies
(mainly Western), where individuals, living under conditions of increasing globalisation that appear
beyond the control of accountable governments, have no alternative to express a sense of justice as
citizens’ of the world. Advocates of this type of consumer activism (Micheletti, 2003; Micheletti,
Follesdal, and Stolle, 2004; Kennedy, 2004; Bennett, 2004; Halkier, 2004) maintain that several key
social changes underpin this new form of activism. These changes include: continuous technological
development and the rise of the informational economy; the geo-politics of the post Cold War era
including the advent, even before 1989, of neo-Liberal economics and the disappearance of a realistic
alternative to capitalism; the rise of postmodern (or late modern) sensibilities; the growing impact on
socio-political life of knowledgeable, self-monitoring individuals; and the intensifying processes of
globalisation (Kennedy, 2004).
As result Micheletti (2003) now believes that consumer action is a form of individualised collective
action, reflecting the political landscape changes of late-modernity. According to Micheletti (2003) ideas
about responsibility and responsibility taking are central for the theoretical discussion used to develop the
concept of individualised collective action. Moreover, “the market, the home, and other seemingly private
or nonpolitical arenas are also appropriate venues for general-responsibility taking. A second implication
is that citizen activism crosses the public and private divide that has determined our conception of
political participation and politics. Finally, this new form of citizen activism implies that responsibility
for problem-solving cannot be delegated to other actors and spheres and the actors and institutions of
representative democracy. It must be taken by each individual who leaves footprints after their actions
and choices” (p.34). In this regard Micheletti (2003) and others (Beck, 1996, 1999; Micheletti, Follesdal,
and Stolle, 2004; Kennedy, 2004; Bennett, 2004; Halkier, 2004) see individual responsibility as a force
for positive social change.
This, however, creates the question whether increasing individualisation may increase ecological
deterioration or somehow becomes part of the solution. This supposed dilemma is summed by Stevenson
(2002): “Individualisation processes open up questions of individual responsibility necessary for
ecological reflection while simultaneously contributing to environmental dangers given that the meanings
involved in consumption are important sources of modern identity” (p.313). Indeed, the notion that
supposed increasing individualisation of society (Giddens, 1991, 1994; Beck, 1992, 1996; Willmott &
Nelson, 2003) could lead to further environmental deterioration has also been suggested (Stevenson,
2002).
The problem of individual responsibility and ecological sustainability
Despite or rather because of the increasing potential attributed to individual consumer action a number of
substantial critiques have been directed at the phenomenon. In terms of policy approaches that call for
consumers to exercise individual environmental responsibility, Hobson (2002) and a number of other
researchers whom she cites, claim these approaches will be relatively ineffective considering the
complexities and entrenched nature of individual consumption practices that are positioned within
contexts and infrastructures not conducive to living (eco) sustainably.
For critics such as Maniates (2002), Smith (1998), and Luke (1997), green consumerism is a means of
individualising the ecological crisis. As Luke (1997) puts it, “each individual consumer or family
household is now the key decisive ecological subject, whose everyday economic activities are either a
blow for environmental destruction or a greener earth” (p.118).
Following a similar line, Maniates (2002) contends that “individualisation, by implying that any action
beyond the private and the consumptive is irrelevant, insulates people from the empowering experiences
225
and political lessons of collective struggle for social change and reinforces corrosive myths about the
difficulties of public life” (p.59). Furthermore, there is the contention that green political consumerism or
ethical consumerism does not translate into thrift in the use of natural resources and sustainable
consumption (see Johnston, 2002). Instead, buycotts such as fair trade and ecolabelled products are as
Goodman and Goodman (2001) describe, constructed within the relational and ideological spaces of the
current capitalist system and seek little fundamental change in the social and economic pathways of food
production and trade, which could they believe be part of a wider, more progressive agenda of sustainable
consumption. Indeed, scholars in the field of macromarketing maintain that green consumerism is not
enough: what is needed is a fundamental rethinking of both consumption goals and capitalist business
practices (see Kilbourne et al, 1997). Here, the fear is that people believe that they need not think about
sustainable development and their consumption patterns as long as they buy environmentally friendly
products. They can buy as much as they want as long as the products are eco-labelled.
Like those who advocate individual responsibility, critics also locate individual responsibility within the
metaphor of the ballot, and as such their critique follows from this. The first part of this critique is that
“the consumer role as political strategy is often seen as less democratic than traditional citizen roles,
because the economic resources to vote with one’s purse and one’s feet are unequally distributed in
society” (Halkier, 1999, p.36). Dolan (2002) puts it more succinctly. He contends, firstly, that the irony
of consumer activism as a solution is that the richer the consumer the more powerful they become.
Secondly, eco-politics through consumer boycotts strengthens the significance of consumption practices –
“not only does it become the symbolic mediator of social and cultural relationships, but also political
ones. Thus, the commodity, whether consumed or not, would become the totem of the power ratio
between consumer and producer, and the commodity would become the site of resolution of moral
disputes and dilemmas” (p.22).
A further exegesis is in terms of the unequal distribution via gender. In contemporary society, women still
perform the vast majority of household shopping (Thompson, Pollio & Locander, 1994; Miller 1995,
1997) and are arguably the decision makers with great potential for impact in the marketplace (Miller,
1995, 1997; Dobscha & Ozanne, 2001). Thus, the shopper as voter thesis tends to lay considerable and
unequal responsibility in the hands of women. This suggests that not only is the environmental crisis
being individualised, but also it is being individualised with a disproportionate level of responsibility
falling on women. In fact, it would suggest that structural impediments are rejected at two levels, firstly,
structural obstacles to environmentally benign behaviour and, secondly, the impediments that leave
women in the position of sole responsibility for household duties.
Having outlined both positions on the potential effectiveness of individualised ecological responsibility, it
appears that these two discourses appear as complex and contrasting positions. However, in terms of
establishing either as a prescriptive socio-political strategy to address ecological problems it seems a case
of Hobson’s choice. Apparently, if consumers adopt the environment within their personal consumption
preference’s critics argue that this is not only not enough, but the structural changes required will not be
introduced. On the other hand, others claim the socio-political conditions of contemporary society have in
effect led business and government to abrogate or even avoid their responsibilities for the provision of
pro-environmental structures that are apparently necessary to create an ecologically sustainable society,
and that consumers must attempt to individually change. Against the background of these two divergent
positions the situation of a group of green consumers in Ireland is now explored, which describes an
alternative position to those already recounted, and thus questions this dichotomy.
226
Research Methodology
The empirical study that informs this paper is part of a larger study conducted in the area of green
consumption. It is a qualitative in-depth study of how consumers experience green consumption, using
‘green/alternative’ (see McDonagh & Prothero, 1997; Gabriel & Lang, 1995) consumers. Data was
collected through phenomenological interviews and diaries with seven participants. The method of
interviewing adopted has been used extensively in consumer research (Thompson, 1997; Thompson,
Pollio & Locander, 1994).
The sample selection was purposive. According to Mason (1996) purposive sampling (or theoretical
sampling) means selecting groups or categories to study on the basis of their relevance to one’s research
questions, one’s theoretical position and analytical framework, one’s analytical practice, and most
importantly the explanation or account which one is developing. Data analysis proceeded with the
interpretation of the respondents reflections on their green consumer experiences through a circular
interplay between a developing understanding of the interviews and an ongoing immersion in a number of
key literatures. It also involved constantly comparing emerging themes with the data (the consumers lived
experiences) not only within the case of a single participant but also across the texts from the other
participants. This was used as a means to eliminate idiosyncratic themes and the problem of anecdotalism
which can occur in qualitative research. The method involves a repeated back and forth movement
between the different parts of the data. From this several themes emerged, one of which is discussed in
the next section.
FINDINGS AND INTERPRETATIONS
Personal Responsibility
Throughout the study, personal responsibility was not only frequently spoken of by the participants, but
was also implicit in many of the specific consumption practices they described.
Deirdre: I’ve stopped eating fish. Until our governments get to grips with the fact that
fish inhabit the arena of global commons and start dealing with this, and stop grabbing
all they can, and we find a way to fish without killing non-target species like dolphins
and turtles, I’m not eating it.
Helen: I do have long-life bulbs…[and] I’ve been meaning to use non-toxic, organic
paints – there’s some small company hidden away off the South Circular that I have
never managed to find.
Anne: …and stuff like not flushing the toilet ten times a day and water – more or less
everything I would have touched on it. I’d usually get them to turn off electrical
appliances. We’ve insulated the house. What else? Waste management, I have this
nasty habit of keeping every container that comes into the house!
Ciara: You have got to take responsibility for the amount of waste you are taking out.
You have got to take responsibility for the amount of waste you are taking in, in your
food, in your lifestyle. If you buy a standard cleaning product, you are going to be
flushing a load of chemicals down the drain. OK, it takes a lot of chemicals to cause
damage to the environment, but say a freshwater river, if every person took
responsibility for what they were getting rid of, the amount would be significantly
reduced. I think it comes down to personal responsibility for the way you interact with
your environment. Even if you are in a high-rise block or working in an office, you can
still take responsibility for your impact on the environment.
227
Like all of the participants, Ciara maintains that everyone must take personal responsibility for his or her
effect on the natural environment. In the following excerpt, for example, Deirdre talks about her like of
liberal democracy as opposed to the alternative systems often espoused by environmentalists, such as ecoMarxist or eco-anarchist political systems, and LETS systems (see Eckersley, 1992). She likes her
independence, her decision to choose as opposed to a choice being made for her by the system.
Deirdre: I am a relatively independent person – I like the idea of making my own
choices. I am not sure how well I would fit in with the commune. And what the money
system does is it gives you that choice – it allows you to decide where you want your
efforts to go. Like, if you look at the LETS system, if you want a particular thing done
then you have to do something in return; I am not sure I want to be locked into that. I
like the independence of doing a piece of work, and the credit I get it is something I
then use to make my own decisions about what I want, and I think a lot of people like
that independence; they don’t want to be locked into having to do favours for Joe
Bloggs down the road in order to get something done for themselves. I’d rather just
have the money and pay for it. So, it gives a degree of freedom.
Here, Deirdre uses the words, choice, freedom, and independence. Implicit within this idea of individual
responsibility is a form of environmental liberalism. It is the language of personal choice and selfresponsibility. Deirdre also refers again to the individual, that one must act at a personal capacity to
change things, to take that personal responsibility rather than leave it to others. One should make one’s
voice heard not only through the established political system of the ballot box, but also through the
subpolitical system of the boycott/buycott.
Deirdre: We are also to blame to a large extent for not letting large corporations know
what our buying concerns are, and, I mean, that can be difficult to do. You know we
are all to blame in one way or another, I don’t think you can blame any one group
particularly. I think we all make choices, and we all make choices frequently to do what
is easiest rather than what is best
Active personal involvement is clearly very important to the participants in the study, and in a sense it
helps reinforce the notion that something they themselves can do will make a difference. Though acting
as individuals, they feel they are not alone, and, that once more people adopt this lifestyle, ecological
change will take place. This is precisely what Deirdre is referring to above. There is a belief in the
efficacy of individual responsibility among the participants. This tends to be expressed through a moral
responsibility to take individual action.
An interesting feature from this study revolves around the concept of individual responsibility and its
meaning. It is not just about the impact of one’s ecological footprint; rather, it requires one to participate
and become involved in ecological issues either at a local community or national level. The next feature
of the participants’ lived experiences illustrates a critical element that distinguishes green consumers from
many mainstream “green” consumers who tend to recycle and occasionally made green purchases.
Collective Action
Despite the participants’ perceived individualism, all are involved in, or attempting change via some form
of collective action. The following extracts describe the participants’ involvement at various levels in
attempting to make broad changes within the society in which they live, as opposed to reducing their own
personal individual ecological footprints on the environment.
228
To begin with Deirdre’s experience: Deirdre left her job in the Irish civil service to work permanently for
an environmental organisation, after working voluntarily for a number of years for the group. Afterwards,
she became involved with Voice (Voice of Irish Concern for the Environment), working initially on a
voluntary basis and later as part of a job-share scheme. Presently, the main campaigns on which Deirdre
is working include the problems of water fluoridation in Ireland; safe, clean water; sustainable waste
management; sustainable forestry; and the issue of genetic resources. This Voice campaign on genetics is
against patents on life and genetic engineering – an issue to which Deirdre brings substantial experience
and knowledge with her Ph.D. and masters in these areas.
Colin also works with an environmental organisation, Earthwatch. He too began as a volunteer and is now
the manager of the Earthwatch office in Dublin. A socialist at heart, he believes the green movement must
mean something to working class people if environmental change is to occur. Presently, he feels it is a
middle class movement and is not tackling many of the underlying issues. Recently, he travelled to
Scotland to look at an initiative set up there by Friends of the Earth Scotland and is hoping a similar
strategy may be put in place in Dublin. Below, he describes the initiative and the underlying problems
that the strategy, he believes, needs to address.
Colin: Places like Friends of the Earth in Scotland are coming up with initiatives
that work with people on the ground to try and get them to do more recycling,
waste management, and create green consumerism and so forth, and they’re trying
to work with the poorer communities, and they’ve got funding from the National
Lottery in Scotland, and that’s the type of thing I think we need to look at. We need
to look at making ourselves relevant to the people who are on the ground and
struggling, because it’s very easy for people with a few bob to say “I buy organic
chicken” or whatever – they can afford to pay the extra £1.50, and it doesn’t make
much difference to them if there’s an extra £10 or £15 added to their food bill.
They’ll regard that as OK, but if you’re on the bottom end of the rung, a pound can
matter, let alone £15. If you’re on the dole and you’re living on £75 a week, you
can’t afford to buy all organic foods and all the extra that it costs. You certainly
can’t afford to buy all the green products; you’ll go for the cheaper toilet roll, the
cheapest tissues, anything that’ll save you money. It needs a big shift at
government level as well.
Hence, Colin is thoroughly convinced by the need for collective political action and structural change.
Although he makes changes in his own personal consumption habits for environmental reasons, he is also
actively engaged in attempting greater structural changes to the present system.
Meanwhile, Alice takes part in protest marches as part of the Irish No-War movement4. More recently,
she is involved in protests outside Shannon Airport in Co. Clare against American military use of the
airport facilities in relation to the invasion of Iraq. Alice is disillusioned by the numbers of people who go
shopping as an activity on Saturdays as opposed to the paltry turnout for protest marches against the war
in Afghanistan (this war was taking place when the first interviews began).
Alice: I would have felt that it was very important what was happening in Afghanistan and,
OK, the second protest was a little bit bigger, but I can remember sitting on the top deck of a
bus looking down Grafton Street on a Wednesday at lunchtime, and there were probably
more people there and they were all shopping.
4
No-War movement is an organisation in Ireland opposed to the recent wars in both Afghanistan and Iraq.
229
Alice’s anti-war stance fits closely with environmental concerns, and green movements traditionally have
been anti-war, with Campaign for Nuclear Disarmament (CND) for example. Indeed, one of Greenpeace
International’s current campaigns is opposition to the current war in Iraq5.
At the more extreme end of the environmental spectrum is Andrew who has spent a number of years in
various parts of Europe working with Direct Action groups (see McKay, 1998; Doherty, 1999). This
radical wing of the environmental movement is prepared to use disruptive protest to highlight what are
seen as fundamental failings of modern society (Doherty, 1999). In this first excerpt, Andrew describes
how he became involved in militant green action and some of his first experiences.
Andrew: I went to university in Cardiff, and I got involved in the green movement in
Cardiff, and I started a campaign there against opencast mining and I would have been
about 19. And then I moved to another university in Toulouse, in the south of France,
and was the time of the bombings in the South Pacific, so, again, I was involved in
demonstrations against that. Then I would have become involved in more militant
activities like Manchester runway, the A30 campaign, Sellafield nuclear power station,
the whale campaign.
At a similar Direct Action initiative in London as part of an animal rights campaign he was arrested for
chaining himself to the Norwegian Embassy. This was in protest at Norway’s whaling policy.
Andrew:…I got involved with the people who did the Dover live animal exports.
They were involved in hunts, vegans, and basically we had been asked to bring this
petition into Westminster and we were in Westminster. We were delivering this
animal rights campaign and, basically, after that we went up to Victoria dressed up in
white suits, jumpsuits and chained ourselves to the Norwegian Embassy with
Greenpeace inflammable whales and the usual stuff. The media were tipped off and it
was a standard Direct Action. We were arrested and taken by Metropolitan Police to
Belgravia and questioned and then detained.
Meanwhile, Anne’s involvement at a collective-structural level is quite diverse. She was a founder of the
Earthwatch society when a university student and is still a member of the Earthwatch organisation today.
Although a busy mother with three children, she has started a kids’ club in her local estate to introduce
them to environmental concepts and ideas. Here, she describes the composting experience.
Anne: Well, when we first set up our compost heap, some of them had never – the
concept was absolutely revolting to some of the children who saw it. So, you’re
working at the level, where they’re going “What?! And you’re going to use this
afterwards, ugh!?”
She is also involved in GAP, which she describes below. Also, as noted in a previous theme, she is a
central collection point for a “bag system” for seasonal vegetables.
Anne:…I started up Earthwatch in UCD and kept that going against the odds for a
couple of years. Then I got involved with GAP as well.
Researcher: Who are GAP?
5
See www.greenpeace.org
230
Anne: Global Action Plan. They go round and deal with everyday existence and come
up with a schedule which deals with all the different issues from waste management to
energy management to everything, and they have a checklist of things that you can do
to improve your environmental standing and performance as well. And now I’m
working with everybody else’s children and trying to indoctrinate them!
Anne: When I started this GAP thing here, I had six or seven people coming along…
Researcher: What would you do locally with it?
Anne: Well, you just work in your house and you take on board – one week you’d deal
with water. There’d be a questionnaire to assess your current standing, and then you
would take on board for a week or two whatever changes you thought you were going
to be able to make realistically, and then you come back and meet together. So it’s like
an environmental support group.
Ciara’s way of addressing the situation is also distinctly different. Although not involved in any organised
environmental grouping, she is one of the participants most committed to reducing her own ecological
footprint and limiting her consumption. Her way of collective action is in trying to change the people
around her, family, work colleagues, etc.
Ciara: There are ripples everywhere, because, if you buy that you are supporting an
industry that makes it, it goes on and on, but I think that at the end of the day you can
only do what you can do. You can’t be a superman, you can’t do everything. One
person, some people would argue, can’t save the world. I think if you are just true and
do the best you can do and hopefully to encourage other people to do something
similar. Fair enough, but a lot of people believe in certain things. One person that my
boyfriend works with thinks that global warming is pie in the sky and doesn’t exist;
that there is no such thing, he firmly believes this. This is his realistic view of the
world. No such thing, pie in the sky. It is all a lot of hippie mumbo jumbo. But there
would be people who would see what we are doing and like it. At work now, people
like say “Ah you eat a load of weird food”, and then it’s like; “What’s that? Can I try?”
I suppose there is a bit of an influence there too.
This is the dynamic where ordinary consumer influence is a voice in (Ciara’s) social networks where
decisions about purchases and consumption are discussed. According to Halkier (2004), this dynamic is a
more long-term one, which does not necessarily influence short-term policy making, but influences the
social and cultural norms and the political legitimacy of different solutions.
Helen, too, despite her orientation towards individual responsibility, is an active member of an Irish
environmental organisation, An Tasice, The National Trust for Ireland, and she is quite passionate about
the work they do.
Helen: I’ve always had an interested in An Taisce. It’s an organisation I’d been
meaning to join for years…Then I joined, and I took redundancy, so I had time on my
hands and I volunteered to be on the committee…I was Secretary of the local
association for about nine years and now I’ve escaped, because no-one wants the job
and I’m on the Planning Committee. I was helping out with planning anyway, and now
I’m on the subcommittee for planning…
231
In this excerpt, Helen talks of the various environmental campaigns in which they are engaged and the
work they do. Indeed, sustainable development is one of the central objectives of An Taisce6.
Helen: …It’s trying to do, because it’s trying to work miracles with what it has in its
resources, and it’s spearheading the Litter Campaign, the Green Schools campaign.
It’s doing a lot of very good work, so I would be biased.
Helen’s situation is symbiotic of all the participants. On the one hand, she feels that personal individual
responsibility is crucial, while at the same time she is actively involved at a collective level in attempting
structural change. Follesdal (2004) maintains that the individualism inherent within political consumerism
in late modernity is often combined with respecting the autonomy of others, prizing individual liberties
and social tolerance, and fostering civic participation, but in new social movements rather than in
established organisations. This resonates closely with the lived experiences of the participants in this
study.
It is evident from this study that the participants engage with different methods of individual and
collective action. They see a connection between the kitchen table and global environmental concerns,
and political consumerism is a key feature of their consumptive action. However, there is also another
feature to their activism, one driven by a personal sense of responsibility but manifested in collective
action to achieve structural changes that may enhance or lead towards environmental sustainability. It is
the combination of these two activities, personalised individual action visible in their personal consumer
choices and an activism to enhance structural change, that defines the green consumers in this study.
Indeed, many examples of this activism exist in many different forms, expressions, and intensity of
activism. Nevertheless, responsibility taking in whatever form is objectified in both practical
consumerism and collective activism, as exemplified in the lived experiences recounted.
While the lived experiences of the participants in this study show that individual responsibility taking in
the form of political consumerism is important, it is only part of the jigsaw, and collective action is also
seen as crucial, so they engage in both. As such, it is possible that the green consumers in this study
recognize that environmental problems are complex, persistent, and threatening (Macnaghten, 2003;
Amine, 2003) and cannot be solved simply by scattered individual efforts to follow the "4 Rs" of
environmentally responsible behavior—redesign, reduce, reuse, and recycle (Amine, 2003). Hobson
(2002) has referred to this as a kind of environmental citizenship, whereby an environmental citizen is
someone who has internalised information about environmental problems, creating a sense of personal
responsibility and duty that is then expressed through consumption and community actions.
CONCLUSION
In conclusion, given the experiences of the participants in this study, individual responsibility in the form
of personal consumption choices is not the dominant practice in dealing with environmental issues.
Despite the perceived environmental liberalism, the participants were all engaged at a more collective
level. They choose disparate (subpolitical) movements, different and diverse agendas, a variety of
methods and levels of activism, yet all fit closely with their own sense of self and indeed their lifepolitical
agendas.
In this regard, this particular study stands in contrast to the polar positions outlined at the beginning of
this paper. It calls into question the general assumptions resulting from debates on individual
responsibility whereby it is positioned as a key strategic instrument or a limited tool that essentially
‘makes people feel they are doing something’. Many calls, both for and against individual responsibility
6
See www.antaisce.org
232
taking, tend to take the form of a polemic with little empirical research to support either position. What is
required is research to explain and describe the role of individual responsibility taking within different
social and cultural worlds.
This study is just one example. However, from it, the notion of a singular or indeed universal concept of
individual responsibility taking as confined to the realm of personal consumption choices is questioned.
Now, whether, this situation is confined to urban green consumption in Ireland or is replicated in other
social and cultural contexts can only be resolved through further research. This, the authors believe is a
more useful and valuable direction in attempting to address ecological concerns rather than facile
normative polemics. In addition, we contend that macromarketing researchers can and should play a part
in this research objective as macroconsumption (Belk, et al, 1996) issues (of which this is one) are clearly
within the domain of the macromarketing academy.
What the study shows quite clearly is that green consumption involves both personal individual action
and also collective action, in varying degrees. Consequently if it is these activities that green consumers
are actually engaged in, it seems imperative for future research to investigate if such activities may lead to
global environmental change.
REFERENCES
Amine, Lyn S. (2003), “An integrated micro- and macrolevel discussion of global green issues. “It isn’t
Beck, U. (1996), "World Risk Society as Cosmopolitan Society? Ecological Questions in a Framework of
Manufactured Uncertainties," Theory Culture & Society, 13 (4), 1-32.
Beck, U. (1999), World Risk Society. Cambridge: Polity.
Bennett, Lance W. (2004), "Branded Political Communication: Lifestyle Politics, Logo Campaigns, and
the Rise of Global Citizenship," in Politics, Products, and Markets. Exploring Political
Consumerism Past and Present, Michele Micheletti, Andreas Follesdal and Dietlind Stolle, eds.
New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers, 101-126.
Connolly, John, Pierre McDonagh, Michael Polonsky, and Andrea Prothero (2004), "Green Marketing
and Green Consumers: Exploring the Myths," in International Handbook on Environmental
Technology, David Annadale, Dora Marinova and John Phillimore, eds: Edward Elgar.
Crane, A. (2005) Meeting the ethical gaze. In: R. Harrison, T. Newholm and D. Shaw The ethical
consumer: Sage (forthcoming)
Dobscha, Susan and Julie L. Ozanne (2001), "An Ecofeminist Analysis of Environmentally Sensitive
Women Using Qualitative Methodology: The Emancipatory Potential of an Ecological Life,"
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 20(2) (Fall), 201-14.
Doherty, Brian (1999), "Paving the Way: The Rise of Direct Action Against Road-Building and the
Changing Character of British Environmentalism," Political Studies, XLVII, 275-91.
Dolan, Paddy (2002), "The Sustainability of 'Sustainable Consumption'," Journal of Macromarketing, 22
(2), 170-81.
Eckersley, Robyn (1992), Environmentalism and Political Theory: Toward an Ecocentric Approach.
London: UCL Press Ltd.
233
Eden, S. E. (1993), "Individual Environmental Responsibility and its Role in Public Environmentalism,"
Environment and Planning A, 25, 1743-58.
Follesdal, Andreas (2004), "Political Consumerism as Chance and Challenge," in Politics, Products, and
Markets: Exploring Political Consumerism Past and Present, Michele Micheletti, Andreas
Follesdal and Dietlind Stolle, eds. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers, 3-20.
Halkier, Bente (1998), "Environmentally Friendly Consumption Practices - Life Politics?," in
Consumption, Everyday Life and Sustainability Summer School. Lancaster University.
Halkier, Bente (1999), "Consequences of the Politicisation of Consumption: The Example of
Environmentally Friendly Consumption Practices," Journal of Environmental Policy & Planning,
1 (1), 25-41.
Halkier, Bente (2001b), "Consuming Ambivalences. Consumer Handling of Environmentally Related
Risks in Food," Journal of Consumer Culture, 1 (2), 205-24.
Halkier, Bente (2004), "Consumption, Risk, and Civic Engagement," in Politics, Products, and Markets:
Exploring Political Consumerism Past and Present, Michele Micheletti, Andreas Follesdal and
Dietlind Stolle, eds. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers, 223-244.
Hobson, Kersty (2001a), "Sustainable Lifestyles: Rethinking Barriers and Behaviour to Change," in
Exploring Sustainable Consumption. Environmental Policy and the Social Sciences, Maurie J.
Cohen and Joseph Murphy, eds. London: Pergamon, 191-212.
Hobson, Kersty (2002), "Competing Discourses of Sustainable Consumption: Does the 'Rationalisation of
Lifestyles' Make Sense?," Environmental Politics, 11 (2), 95-120.
Gabriel, Yannis and Tim Lang (1995), The Unmanageable Consumer. London: Sage.
Goodman, David and Michael Goodman (2001), "Sustaining Foods: Organic Consumption and the SocioEcological Imaginary," in Exploring Sustainable Consumption. Environmental Policy and the
Social Sciences, Maurie J. Cohen and Joseph Murphy, Eds. London: Pergamon, 97-120.
Holliman, Jonathan (1974), Consumers' Guide to the Protection of the Environment. London: Pan Books
(for) Friends of the Earth.
Johnston, Joseé (2002), "Consuming Social Justice: Fair Trade Shopping and Alternative Development,"
in Protest and Globalisation: Prospects for Transnational Solidarity, James Goodman, eds.
Annadale, NSW: Pluto Press Australia, 38-56.
Jordan, Andrew, Rudiger K. W. Wurzel, Anthony R. Zito, and Lars Bruckner (2004), “Consumer
Responsibility-Taking and Eco-Labelling Schemes in Europe” in Politics, Products, and Markets:
Exploring Political Consumerism Past and Present, Michele Micheletti, Andreas Follesdal and
Dietlind Stolle, eds. London: Transaction Publishers, 21-44.
Kennedy, Paul (2004), "Selling Virtue: Political and Economic Contradictions of Green/Ethical
Marketing in the United Kingdom," in Politics, Products, and Markets: Exploring Political
Consumerism Past and Present, Michele Micheletti, Andreas Follesdal and Dietlind Stolle, eds.
London: Transaction Publishers, 21-44.
234
Kilbourne, William E., Pierre McDonagh, and Andrea Prothero (1997), "Sustainable Consumption and
the Quality of Life: A Macromarketing Challenge to the Dominant Social Paradigm," Journal of
Macromarketing (Spring), 4-23.
Luke, Timothy W. (1997), Ecocritique. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Maniates, Michael (2002), "Individualisation: Plant a Tree, Buy a Bike, Save the World?," in Confronting
Consumption, Thomas Princen, Michael Maniates and Ken Conca, eds. London: The MIT Press,
43-66.
Mason, Jennifer (1996), Qualitative Researching. London: Sage.
Macnaghten, P. (2003) Embodying the environment in everyday life practices. The Sociological Review,
63--84.
McDonagh, Pierre (2002), "Communicative Campaigns to Effect Anti-slavery and Fair Trade," European
Journal of Marketing, 36 (5/6), 642-66.
McDonagh, Pierre and Andrea Prothero (1997), "Introduction and Overview," in Green Management: A
Reader, Pierre McDonagh and Andrea Prothero, eds. London: The Dryden Press, ix-xv.
McKay, George (1998), DiY Culture. Party & Protest in Nineties Britain. London: Verso.
Micheletti, Michele (2003), Political Virtue and Shopping, Individuals, Consumerism and Collective
Action. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Micheletti, Michele, Andreas Follesdal, and Dietlind Stolle (2004), Politics, Products, and Markets:
Exploring Political Consumerism Past and Present. London: Transaction Publishers.
Miller, Daniel (1995), "Consumption as the Vanguard of History," in Acknowledging Consumption,
Daniel Miller, eds. London: Routledge, 1-57.
Miller, Daniel (1997), "Could Shopping Ever Really Matter?," in The Shopping Experience, Pasi Falk
and Colin Campbell, eds. London: Sage, 15-30.
Moisander, Johanna and Sinikka Pesonen (2002), "Narratives of Sustainable Ways of Living:
Constructing the Self and the Other as a Green Consumer," Management Decision, 40 (4), 32942.
Murphy, Joseph (2001a), "From Production to Consumption: Environmental Policy in the European
Union," in Exploring Sustainable Consumption. Environmental Policy and the Social Sciences,
Maurie J. Cohen and Joseph Murphy, eds. London: Pergamon, 39-60.
Nava, Mica (1991), "Consumerism Reconsidered: Buying and Power," Cultural Studies, 5 (2), 157-73.
Prothero, Andrea (1990), "Green Consumerism and the Societal Marketing Concept: Marketing Strategies
for the 1990's," Journal of Marketing Management, 6 (2), 87-103.
Prothero, Andrea (1998), "Green Marketing: The 'Fad' That Won't Slip Slide Away," Journal of
Marketing Management, 14 (6), 507-12.
235
Renard, M. (2003) Fair trade: quality, market and conventions. Journal of Rural Studies 19, 87--96.
Shaw, D. (2004) Consumption as voting in ethical consumer choice. In: Australian and New Zealand
Marketing Academy, Wellington.
Shaw, Deirdre and Ian Clarke (1999), "Belief Formation in Ethical Consumer Groups: An Exploratory
Study," Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 17 (2), 109-19.
Smith, Toby M. (1998), The Myth of Green Marketing: Tending Our Goats at the Edge of Apocalypse.
Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Stevenson, Nick (2002), "Consumer Culture, Ecology and the Possibility of Cosmopolitan Citizenship,"
Consumption, Markets and Culture, 5 (4), 305-19.
Thompson, Craig (1997), "Interpreting Consumers: A Hermeneutical Framework for deriving Marketing
Insights from the Texts of Consumers Consumption Stories," Journal of Marketing Research,
XXXIV, 438-55.
Thompson, Craig. J., Howard R. Pollio, and William B. Locander (1994), "The Spoken and Unspoken: A
Hermeneutic Approach to Understanding the Cultural Viewpoints That Underlie Consumers'
Expressed Meanings," Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (December), 432-52.
UNDP (1998), "Human Development Report 1998." Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Willmott, Michael., and William Nelson (2003), Complicated Lives, London, Wiley.
236
Consumer Well-Being, Income, Religious Attitudes
And their Relationship to Selected Measures of Life
Satisfaction among Professionals
Gerald M. Hampton, New Mexico State University
Marco Wolf, New Mexico State University
__________________________________________________
Abstract
We were born to be consumers. This tenet is a fundamental of the marketing discipline. Marketing
activities are developed and implemented to enhance the act of consumption. As a result, effective
marketing practice begins with the acceptance of the marketing concept. This marketing philosophy
attempts to focus most organizations’ activities on the consumer. It has been suggested that to create and
set in motion marketing activities without the adoption of the marketing concept is not marketing (Joseph
1996; Trustrum 1989). When the marketing concept is implemented, an organization is said to have a
market orientation (Kohli and Jaworski 1990; Narver and Slater 1990; Ruckert 1992).
Because of this organization effort and focus, marketing scholars contend that marketing practices
positively influence life satisfaction (Handy 1978; Samli, Sirgy and Medow 1987; Sirgy 1999). After all,
marketing functions are formulated to assist with consumption, and to create and maintain satisfied
customers. Economic theory also views consumption as essential for economic well-being (Slesnick
2000). Many agree that life satisfaction is derived in large part from the goods and services consumed.
Leelakulthanit, Day and Walters (1992) demonstrated that material possessions have a positive impact on
overall life satisfaction. Others have demonstrated that subjective well-being is also related to objective
measures like income (MacDonald and Douthitt 1992). Some have also shown that life satisfaction is
positively related to religious attitudes and behavior such as church attendance or belief in God (Greene
and Yoon 2004).
The purpose of this research is to examine the relationships between subjective assessments of life
satisfaction and consumer well-being, income levels and selected religious attitudes among a group of
professionals.
Background
Consumer Well Being: Numerous scholars argue that marketing activities contribute to the enhancement
of overall satisfaction with the quality of life (Day 1987; Handy 1978; Lee and Sirgy 2004; Lee, Sirgy,
Larsen and Wright 2002; Samli, Sirgy and Meadow 1987). Leelakulthanit, Day and Walters proposed
and tested a conceptual framework or model that views life satisfaction as related to various domains of
life, both marketing and non-marketing activities, such as work satisfaction, social life, acquisition
satisfaction and possession satisfaction of goods and services (1991). This study of professionals uses
the multidimensional measures of consumer well-being developed by Lee, Sirgy, Larsen and Wright to
examine the relationship between marketing and the quality of life (2002). The expectations are the
different aspects of consumer well-being, satisfaction with the acquisition, possession, consumption,
maintenance, and disposition of consumer goods and services, are positively related to overall life
satisfaction.
237
Income: While a positive relationship between current income and life satisfaction has been documented,
a review, integration and critique of the literature on the relationship between wealth and life satisfaction
shows that income has a modest relationship with life satisfaction (Ahuvia and Friedman 1998). To
explore why income and life satisfaction correlations are so small, the review analyzed the literature from
three different conceptual perspectives. The comparative perspective suggests that life satisfaction results
for a comparison of one’s situation to some material norm. The reason the correlations are so small under
this perspective is because material norms are for ever changing and are so flexible. The goal attainment
perspective holds that money assists people to attain set goals. Under this goal attainment perspective the
correlations are also low because money has little value in helping achieve goals people care most about.
The hedonic perspective views income as the device that enables people to use their time in more
satisfying ways. Under this perspective correlations are weak because many desirable leisure experiences
have variable price points (some may cost nothing) and satisfying work is not always highly paid.
Therefore, in this study we expect weak correlations between measure of income and life satisfaction.
Religion: In general, researchers indicate that people with a high degree of religiosity are more positive
toward their quality of life (Hadaway and Roof 1978). The correlations between religiosity and life
satisfaction, while positive, tend to be small (Ellison, Gay and Glass 1989). It has also been suggested
that religiosity tends to be negatively related to education – the higher the education the less religious
individuals tend to be (McDaniel and Burnett 1990). Therefore, in this study of highly educated
professionals, we expect to find low correlations between religion and life satisfaction.
Methodology
Sample: There appears to be limited research on the relationship of life satisfaction and marketing among
professionals. For this reason, college professors from a large university in the American Southwest were
randomly selected and asked to participate in an academic panel. Three hundred professors were selected
and 180 agreed to participate during an academic year. They agreed to complete a number of surveys on
a variety of topics. No incentives or rewards were used to encourage participation. A total of nine surveys
were administered over a nine month period. Included in these surveys were assessments of life
satisfaction, consumer well-being, income and religious attitudes. Information on the respondents is
show in Table 3. This sample is representative of the faculty at this one university. The usable number of
surveys for this study is 122.
Measures: The scales used in this study to measure the constructs were derived from instruments designed
for previous studies where the reliability and validity were assessed. Each item in the scales used was
assessed to make sure they were appropriate for use among college professors. The scales contained a
large number of items designed to measure a limited number of constructs. To determine if the scales
functioned as designed, the following procedure was used. First, a factor analysis was performed on each
construct using varimax rotation and estimated with maximum likelihood to validate the previous scales.
Then reliability coefficients for items retained in each factor for each construct were calculated. Next,
items in each factor of every construct with sufficient reliability were summed and divided by the number
of items to create an overall factor mean.
Life satisfaction scales: Two scales were used to measure global life satisfaction. The first was the
Temporal Satisfaction With Life Scale (Pavot, W., Diener, E. and Suh, E. 1998), which is designed to
measure past, present and future life satisfaction. This scale has been shown to be reliable and valid
(McIntosh 2001). The scale contains 15 statements (seven point likert scales from 7=strongly agree to
1=strongly disagree) with five items representing each of the life satisfaction stages or factors. All items
were subjected to a factor analysis with varimax rotation to determine the number of factors in the scale.
The results indicate there are three factors – past, present and future life satisfaction. Only one of the 15
items, in future life satisfaction, was discarded. A reliability analysis demonstrated Coefficient Alpha’s
of .87 to .88 (see Table 1). The second measure of global life satisfaction was the Campbell scale
238
(Campbell 1976), which has been shown to be reliable and valid (La Barbera, P.A. and Gurhan, Z. 1997).
The Campbell Scale assesses life satisfaction on 10 semantic differential scales including such items as
boring/interesting, enjoyable/miserable, and useless/worthwhile. Factor analysis indicated one factor with
a reliability coefficient of .91 (see Table 1). The Campbell and Temporal Satisfaction With Life Scale
(TSWLS) are highly correlated. The Pearson Correlation coefficients are as follows: Campbell with past
life satisfaction is .42; Campbell with present life satisfaction is .56; and Campbell with future life
satisfaction is .51.
Consumer Well-Being: Consumer well-being was assessed with a scale developed by Lee, Sirgy, Larsen
and Wright (2002). This survey measures consumer satisfaction on the acquisition, possession,
consumption, maintenance (satisfaction with repair services and satisfaction with services for do-ityourself repairs), and disposition of goods and services. In development, the scale was shown to have
predictive validity on three of the six dimensions – acquisition, possession, and consumption. The scale
consist of 48 items assessed on a seven point Likert type scale that ranges from 1=awful to 7=wonderful.
A factor analysis indicates that there are eight factors or dimensions. The factors and number of items in
each factor is shown in Table 1. The scale performed rather well with most items loading on the
appropriate dimensions. The exception is the consumption dimension. In this study there are three
consumption factors, rather than one. The three factors were identified as goods satisfaction, services
satisfaction, and food satisfaction. Reliability analysis showed alphas ranging from .75 to .91 (see Table
1).
Income and Religion Measures: Incomes were measured by asking respondents to 1) estimate their total
monthly household or gross income before taxes and paid for benefits, 2) to estimate their total monthly
household take-home income, and 3) to estimate their total monthly expenditures. Relative income was
calculated as the difference between the respondent’s total monthly estimated expenditures and the
sample mean monthly expenditures (MacDonald and Douthitt 1992). To assess religious attitudes four
questions were ask that are similar to questions found in the World Values Survey Questionnaire
conducted by Inglehart and Baker (2000). The questions were: 1) Apart from weddings, funerals and
christening, about how often do you attend religious services these days (1= more than once a week to
7=never)?, 2) Independently of whether you go to church, how religious do you view yourself (1=anti
religious to 5=very religious)?, 3) How important is religion to you (1=extremely unimportant to
5=extremely important)?, and 4) How important is God to your life (1=not at all to 7=very important)?
Analysis and Conclusions
Factor means, standard deviations, and Pearson correlations are shown in Table 2. First, it is interesting
to note that none of the measures of income have a significant relationship with any of the measure of life
satisfaction. This is in keeping with the past literature. The same applies to religious attitudes. In fact,
the correlations between religious attitudes and income and life satisfaction are quite low as expected.
Note that while the correlations between the importance of God, past, and present life are negative, the
correlation is positive and higher for perception of future life satisfaction. In terms of how religious one
is and the importance of religion, it appears the relationship turns from negative to positive and is stronger
as one moves from the past to future life perspective.
The correlations show present, future, and general well-being measures of life satisfaction are
significantly related to the consumption factors of consumer well-being. In addition, acquisition and
maintenance (repair services) satisfaction is significantly related to present, future and general well-being
measure of life satisfaction. One should note that relationships seem to be strongest between the measure
of consumer well-being and future life satisfaction. Perhaps this is an indication of optimism toward the
future of the respondents.
239
While this study is limited to a small sample, and the analysis is rather simple, it does lend support to the
notion that marketing activities as measured in terms of consumer well-being are associated with life
satisfaction of professionals.
REFERENCES
Ahuvia, A. and Friedman D. (1998). “Income, Consumption, and Subjective Well-Being: Toward a
Composite Macromarketing Model.” Journal of Macromarketing, 18 (Fall): 53-168.
Campbell, A. (1976). “Subjective Measures of Well-Being.” American Psychologis, 31
(February): 117-124.
Day, Ralph L. (1987). “Relationship Between Life Satisfaction and Consumer Satisfaction.” In
Marketing and the Quality-of-Life Interface, edited by A.Coskun Samli. New York: Quorum
Books, pages 289 - 311.
Ellison, C.G., Gray, D. A. and Glass, T.A. (1989). “Does Religious Commitment Contribute
to Individual Life Satisfaction?” 68: 100-123.
Greene, K.V. and Yoon, B.J. (2004). “Religiosity, Economics and Life Satisfaction.” Review of Social
Economy, LXII (June): 245-261.
Hadaway, C.K. and Roof, W.C. (1978). “Religious Commitment and the Quality of Life in
American Society.” Review of Religious Research, 19(Spring): 295 – 307.
Handy, C. “Marketing and the Quality of Life – Any Concensus” In Marketing and the Quality of Life.
American Marketing Association Proceedings Series, 1978, pages 77 – 80.
Inglehart, R. and Baker, W.E. (2000). “Modernization, Cultural Change, and the Persistence of
Traditional Values.” American Sociological Review, 65 (February): 19 – 51.
Joseph, W.B. (1996). “Internal Marketing Builds Service Quality.” Journal of Health Care
Marketing, 16 (Spring): 1 – 12.
Kohli, A.K. and Jaworski, B.J. (1990). “Market Orientation: The Construct, Research
Propositions, and Managerial Implications.” Journal of Marketing, 54 (April): 1 – 18.
La Barbera, P. A. and Gurhan, Z (1997). “The Role of Materialism, Religiosity, and
Demographics in Subjective-Well Being. Psychology & Marketing, 14 (January): 71 – 97.
Lee, D., and Sirgy, J. (2004). “Quality-of-Life (QOL) Marketing: Proposed Antecedents and
Consequences.” Journal of Macromarketing, 24 (June): 44 – 58.
Lee, D., Sirgy, J., Larsen, V., and Wright, N.D. (2002). “Developing a Subjective Measure of Consumer
Well-Being.” Journal of Macromarketing, 22 (December): 158 – 169.
Leelakulthanit, O., Ralph Day and Rockney Walters (1992). “Investigating the Relationship between
Marketing and Overall Satisfaction with Life in a Developing Country.”
Journal of Macromarketing, 11 (Spring): 3-23.
MacDonald M. and R.A. Douthitt (1992). “Consumption Theories and Consumers’
Assessments of Subjective Well-Being.” 26(Winter): 243-261.
240
McIntosh, C. N. (2001). “Report on the Construct Validity of the Temporal Satisfaction with
Life Scale.” Social Indicators Research, 54 (April): 37 – 56.
Narver, J.C. and Slater, S.F. (1990). “The Effect of a Market Orientation on Business
Profitability.” Journal of Marketing, 54 (October): 20 – 35.
Pavot, W. E., Diener, E. and Shu, E. (1998). “The Temporal Satisfaction with Life Scale.”
Journal of Personality Assessment.70: 340 – 172.
Ruckert, R.W. (1992). “Developing a Market Orientation: An Organizational Strategy Perspective.”
International Journal of Research in Marketing, 9 (October): 225 – 245.
Samli, A. Coskun, M.J. Sirgy and H.L. Meadow. (1987). “Measuring Marketing Contribution to Quality
–of-Life.” In Marketing and the Quality-of-Life Interface, edited by A.Coskun Samli. New York:
Quorum Books, pages 3 – 14.
Slesnick, D.T. (2000). Living Standards in the United States: A Consumption-Based Approach.
Washington, D.C.: The AEI Press.
Sirgy, M. Joseph (1999). “Consumption and Quality of Life: A Review and Critique of the
Work of the United Nations Develop Programme. Social Indicatos Network News
,
Spring, pages 6-7.
Trustrum, L.B. (1989). “Marketing Concept and Function.” European Journal of
Marketing,23 (Summer): 48 – 56.
241
Factor
Table 1
Measures
Number of Items Selected
(Total Number of Items)
Coefficient Alpha
Life Satisfaction
Past Life Satisfaction
5
(5)
.88
Present Life Satisfaction
5
(5)
.87
Future Life Satisfaction
5
(4)
.87
Subjective Well-Being
9
(10)
.91
Consumer Well-Being
Acquisition Satisfaction
7
(7)
.87
Maintenance Satisfaction
w/repair services
6
(9)
.91
Maintenance Satisfaction
w/materials and services
for Do It Yourself
6
(8)
.87
Possession Satisfaction
4
(6)
.84
Disposition Satisfaction
5
(7)
.79
Consumption Satisfaction
a. Goods satisfaction
b. Services Satisfaction
c. Foods Satisfaction
5
2
2
.82
.75
.76
Income
Estimated Gross Household income
1
na.
Estimated Take Home
Income
1
na.
Relative Income
1
na.
Religion
Attend Services
1
na.
How Religious?
1
na.
Religion Importance
1
na.
Importance of God
1
na.
(11)
______________________________________________________________________________
242
Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations and Correlations Between Life Satisfaction, Consumer Well-Being, Income and
Religion
X
Past Life Satisfaction
Sat
4.48
SD
life
Sat
1.39
Present Life Satisfaction
4.63
1.27
Future Life Satisfaction
4.85
1.19
Subjective Well-Being
5.68
.95
4.67
.89
4.30
for Do It Yourself
Past
Life
Present
Future
Life
General
Well-
Sat
Being
-.07
.17***
.17***
.13
1.07
-.01
.16*****
.18**
.19**
4.32
.88
-.13
.07
.16*****
.08
Possession Satisfaction
5.69
.76
-.03
.13
.23*
.19**
Disposition Satisfaction
3.77
.94
.08
.03
-.12
-.03
a. Goods satisfaction
4.92
.90
.08
.23*
.30**
.29*
b. Services Satisfaction
4.82
1.15
.13
.23*
.32**
.36*
c. Foods Satisfaction
5.04
1.06
.13
.18**
.13
.16****
$8011
$3898
.02
.08
.06
.02
$4939
$1931
.09
.10
.07
.03
Relative Income
$.71
$1435
.17
.12
.07
.04
Attend Services
4.32
2.17
.15
.09
-.08
How Religious?
3.27
1.20
-.01
.09
.15
.14
Religion Importance
3.42
1.30
-.10
.008
.11
.002
Importance of God
4.60
2.44
-.16
-.009
.12
.05
Consumer Well-Being
Acquisition Satisfaction
Maintenance Satisfaction
w/repair services
Maintenance Satisfaction
w/materials and services
Consumption Satisfaction
Estimated Gross Household income
Estimated Take Home
Income
-.002
* Correlation significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)
** Correlations significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)
*** Correlations significant at the .06 level (2-tailed)
**** Correlations significant at the .07 level (2-tailed)
***** Correlations significant at the .08 level (2-tailed)
243
Table 3
Respondent Profiles
Age
Mean of 51;
range 33 to 68 years old
Years as Professor
Mean of 14;
range of 1 year to 41 years
Years at current university
Mean of 11;
range from 1 to 35 years
Ethnic Identity
88 percent Caucasian
7 percent Hispanic
5 percent other
Gender
40 percent female
60 percent male
Position
32 percent assistant professors
23 percent associate professors
30 percent full professors
5 percent non-tenure track professors
5 percent other
Martial Status
75 percent married
11 percent single
9 percent divorced
5 percent other
Primary Responsibility
20 percent teaching
63 percent research and teaching
8 percent administration
9 percent other
Highest Degree Earned
80 percent Ph.D. in Field
14 percent Masters
6 percent other
Department
244
Respondents are from 45 different university departments
________________________________________________
Introducing the Rasch Model as an Alternative
Measurement Paradigm when Investigating Quality-of-Life
Alexandra Ganglmair-Wooliscroft, University of Otago, NZ
Rob Lawson, University of Otago, NZ
Abstract
Quality-of-Life (QOL) or Life-Satisfaction is one of the few areas of positive emotions that has received
considerable interest in the last years. However, while a large number of studies explore components of
QOL, less research focuses on the actual measurement of the concept. The current paper introduces
Rasch Modelling (Rasch 1960/80) as alternative measurement paradigm to the dominant Classical-TestTheory (Nunnally and Bernstein, 1994). Rasch Modelling helps to overcome shortcomings in current
Quality-of-Life measurement, especially regarding the negative skewness of obtained results. The life of
Georg Rasch and the development of the Rasch Model are discussed as background, before theoretical
merits are explored and practical aspects in the development of a measure using Rasch Modelling are
discussed. Finally, the application of this alternative measurement paradigm to future research is
explored.
The Role of Quality-of-Life and Positive Emotions
Since the ancient Greeks, mankind has been fascinated by negative emotions (McLemee, 2003), an
emphasis that continued at the recent renaissance of emotions as especially psychology was seen as a
science of healing (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) and the study of psychological problems
(Fredrickson and Branigan, 2001). Although only a limited number of researchers study positive
emotions, the area is regarded as a developing topic with great potentials for future research (Bonanno
and Mayne, 2001; Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000).
However, one area that has received considerable attention for several years is Subjective-Well-Being
(SWB). Quality-of-Life is part of this psychological field of research which includes people’s cognitive
and affective evaluation of their life (Diener, 2000). Quality-of-Life or Life-Satisfaction-Judgment as the
phenomenon is called in the corresponding research area that stems out of psychology (Frisch, Clark,
Rouse, Rudd, Paweleck, Greenstone and Kopplin, 2003), is the subjective, cognitive evaluation of the
quality of experiences regarding life as a whole. Quality-of-Life research therefore broadens consumer
satisfaction to human happiness or life satisfaction (Shaw, 1997).
SWB is concerned with “how and why people experience lives in positive ways, including both cognitive
judgments and affective reactions” (Diener, 1984, p.542). The research area has thrived in the last decade
by examining topics such as life satisfaction, happiness and morale (Diener and Lucas, 2000). Interest in
this field of psychological research reflects the growing concern for individual values and subjective
views on individual lives in a society that realises that social indicators like GDP are not sufficient to
describe peoples wellbeing (Diener, Suh, Lucas and Smith, 1999), as well as a realisation of the
overwhelming dominance of research into mental illnesses and negative emotional states, as criticised by
Seligman (1998).
245
Problems Regarding the Measurement of Quality-of-Life and Satisfaction
The following section explores potential problems in the measurement of Quality-of-Life. These are
closely related to problems encountered in the measurement of consumer satisfaction (Diener and Fujita,
1995; Peterson and Wilson, 1992), as QOL can be considered an expansion of satisfaction from consumer
satisfaction to life satisfaction (see above) (Shaw, 1997).
Although measurement research is supposed to be a fundamental activity the area is surprisingly
neglected in the entire social sciences (DeVellis, 1991) as well as in marketing. Diener and Fujita (1995)
and Peterson and Wilson (1992) (the latter in a satisfaction context) belong to a small group of
researchers who investigate problems inherent in measuring Quality-of-Life and satisfaction, most
notably the consistent negative skewness of obtained results. The resulting ceiling effects decrease the
possibility of uncovering group differences and limit the suitability of commonly used data analysis
techniques (Diener and Fujita, 1995).
Diener and Fujita (1995) and Peterson and Wilson (1992) mention a variety of possible explanations for
negatively skewed (life-) satisfaction scales, e.g.: the use of high-end, extreme anchors, an increase in the
number of available answer categories, or the influence of social desirability. However, all presented
explanations produce only inconsistent results and, at best, somewhat reduce the negative skewness
(Diener and Fujita, 1995; Maggino and D’Andrea, 2003; Peterson and Wilson, 1992).
When measuring QOL (either as domain satisfaction or as global life satisfaction), the Delighted-Terrible
Scale (D-T Scale) (Andrews and Withey, 1976) appears to be a popular choice (Bramston, Pretty and
Chipuer, 2002; Sirgy, Rahtz, Cicic and Underwood, 2000). The D-T Scale is deemed to lead to favourable
results in terms of validity and reliability (Andrews and Withey, 1976; Westbrook, 1980). It also
somewhat reduces observed negative skewness (Westbrook, 1980). However, as e.g. Sirgy et.al’s (2000)
study shows, results still lean towards the upper, more positive end of the available answer categories
(e.g. mean for ”How do you feel about your life as a whole” = 5.76 out of 7).
Additionally, the D-T scale is generally assumed to be an interval scale (Bramston et al., 2002; Sirgy et
al., 2000; Westbrook, 1980), although it contains emotional words which are later transformed to a
numeric scale (e.g. from 1 - 7 ). This implies that the researcher assumes the interval from e.g. mostly
satisfied to pleased is exactly the same than the one from pleased to delighted, although it is not known,
whether respondents do indeed regard the steps between these two categories as equal.
Given that the D-T Scale was developed almost 30 years ago, a rethinking of included terms might also
be warranted.
The next section will introduce Rasch Measurement Theory (RMT) (Rasch, 1960/80), an alternative to
the Classical Test Theory (CTT) (e.g. Nunnally and Bernstein, 1994) upon which the majority of existing
measurement scales are based (Embretson and Reise, 2000; Hambleton 1991). While some features of
RMT make it especially suitable to overcome the skewness inherent in current QOL and satisfaction
scales, the theory was designed to improve the quality of measurement in social sciences in general and
therefore provides a valuable opportunity for the entire field of QOL and marketing.
Georg Rasch and the Rasch Model (RM)
Rasch Measurement Theory (RMT) was developed in 1960 by George Rasch (1960/80). The Rasch
Model follows mathematical and scientific rules of measurement and aims to introduce rigid rules of
measurement - similar to physics - into social sciences (Wright, 1997). The following paragraphs provide
a brief summary of the development process as it provides an insight into Rasch’s life and the ideas that
build the theoretical foundations of the model.
246
Georg Rasch was trained as a mathematician at the University of Copenhagen, but due to a lack of
suitable positions, started to work as a consultant for the University Department for Hygiene and the
Danish Serum Institute (Andersen and Olsen, 2001). In order to receive a formal education in statistics,
Rasch got a scholarship to study with R. Frisch in Oslo (who would later be awarded the first Nobel Price
in economics) and R.A. Fisher in London (Andersen and Olsen, 2001; Olsen, 2003; Wright, 1980). After
his return to Denmark, he was regarded as person with the best knowledge of Fisher’s work (Andersen
and Olsen, 2001).
Georg Rasch developed the model, which would become known as Rasch Model during the 1950s and
finally published it in the book Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests in 1960
(Andersen and Olsen, 2001; Olsen, 2003; Rasch, 1960/80; Wright, 1980; Wright, 1997). Two projects
have formed the basis for the development of the Rasch Model: work on an intelligence test for the
Danish Department of Defence and investigations regarding the long-term effect of special education that
so-called slow-readers received. When working on the first project, Rasch was interested whether “it was
possible to define the degree of difficulty of a test in a unique way or if this was only possible within a
certain subpopulation” (Andersen and Olsen, 2001, p.9). In the slow-reader study Rasch was aiming to
“develop a new method where the individual could be measured independent of which particular reading
test had been used for testing the child” (Andersen and Olsen, 2001, p.10). Both aspects clearly
influenced the development of the final model.
In the development process of his model, as has been common practice throughout Rasch’s career, he
“used graphical methods to understand the nature of a set of data and then transferred his findings to a
mathematical and a statistical formulation of the model” (Andersen and Olsen, 2001, p.12). According to
an interview David Andrich conducted with Georg Rasch in 1979, Rasch first used the multiplicative
Poisson distribution because he thought it was a mathematically good idea.
“I had chosen the multiplicative Poisson for the reading tests because it seemed a good idea
mathematically, if it would work. It turned out that it did. Then I wanted to have some
motivation for using it, and not only the excuse that it worked. In order to do so, I imitated
the proof of a theorem concerning a large number of dichotomous events, each of them
having a small probability. Under conditions which can be specified easily, including that
these probabilities be small, the number of events becomes Poisson distributed. I imitated the
proof, but in doing so I took care that the imitation ended up with the multiplicative Poisson
model, that is, I made sure that there was a personal factor entering into each of the small
probabilities for the dichotomous outcome. The probabilities for the dichotomous case
should therefore be of the form λ/ (1 + λ), and the λ would have a factor that was personal
through all of the items and each item, of course would have its own parameter and then I
had my new model” (cited in Wright, 1980, p. xiii).
Rasch referred to his model as Models for Measurement in order to “stress that the models’ main property
was to measure, and that they therefore could be used to solve all sorts of measurement problems within
social sciences” (Andersen and Olsen, 2001, p.21).
The mathematical formulation of the Rasch Model is:
where:
Pni (xni= 1/βn, δi ) = e βv – δi / [1+ e βv – δi]
Pni (xni= 1/βn, δi ) = the probability that person n with ability βn scores correct (x = 1) on item i
δi = the item location parameter of item i
βn = the person parameter for person n
In 1962 Rasch was appointed Professor of Statistics at the University of Copenhagen (Olsen, 2003). He
kept working on his model until his death in 1980 (Andersen and Olsen, 2001; Wright, 1980). During and
247
after Rasch’s life the properties of the model were explored and the model itself was expanded. A major
development was the extension to models that can deal with ordered response categories (Andrich, 1978).
However, the following discussion is mainly limited to the model described in the equation above.
Important Characteristics of the Rasch Measurement Theory
The following discussion provides a brief summary of the theoretical properties of the Rasch Model and
outlines the process involved when constructing a measure applying RMT. Detailed discussions can be
found in Andrich (1988), Bond and Fox (2001), Fischer and Molenaar (1995) and Wright and Stone
(1979; 2004).
Rasch Models belong to the family of logit models. The mathematical model is based “on a probabilistic
relation between any item’s difficulty and any person’s ability” (Bond and Fox, 2001, p.199). The
technique is regarded is a probabilistic alternative to the deterministic Guttman scaling (Andrich, 1982;
Salzberger, Sinkovics and Schlegelmilch, 1999; Wright, 1997). “A person having greater ability than
another should have the greater probability of solving any item of the type in question and similarly, one
item being more difficult than another one means that for any person the probability of solving the second
item correctly is the greater one” (Rasch, 1960/80, p.117).
It has to be noted, that the terms difficulty and ability are most appropriate in an educational context.
However, from the history of the development of the Rasch Model, it is clear that Rasch Models were
developed for measurement in social science in general. The term ability represents the person location
parameter and is used when referring to the amount of a particular latent trait a person possesses (e.g. how
intelligent, happy, satisfied, extrovert, etc. a person is). The difficulty of an item is the degree of the latent
trait in question that is expressed by that item - how hard it is to endorse an item, or how extreme the item
is. The higher the ability - intensity of the latent trait a person possesses, e.g. the more affect towards the
experience/object the person has - the more likely is it that the subject will agree to/endorse a more
difficult item - more extreme rating of that latent trait, e.g. a more extreme affective item) on the Rasch
scale.
As the Rasch Model aims to introduce rigid rules of measurement into social sciences (Wright, 1997), it
specifies what data has to look like to constitute measurement. The question whether measurement is
accomplished is left to empiricism (Salzberger et al., 1999, Wright, 1997). The mathematical
characteristics of the Rasch Model also allow the transformation from commonly observed binary or
ordinal answer patterns (e.g. Likert type data), into measures on an equal-interval scale (Peck, 2000).
Important Theoretical Characteristics
The theoretical merits of the Rasch Model are the subject of numerous studies (see for example
http://www.rasch.org). “Mathematical analysis shows the Rasch Model to be statistically strong. It has
estimators for its parameters, Bn and Di that are sufficient, consistent, efficient and unbiased” (Wright,
1977, p.3). The Rasch Model builds on Campbell’s, Fisher and Levy’s and Kolmogorov’s work and is the
only measurement model that realizes the necessities for Thurstone’s New Measures (Wright, 1997).
Separability, Sufficiency, Specific Objectivity and Latent Additivity are four key characteristics of the
Rasch Model that have received considerable attention. A brief explanation of these can be found in Table
1.
248
Characteristic Explanation
… means that person and item effects can be isolated from each other.
Separability
Following from this characteristic it is also possible to estimate the item
parameters without knowing the distribution of the person parameter (Rost,
2001).
… means that “the sum of responses in a certain category is a sufficient
Sufficiency
statistic for estimating the person’s ability or trait value to respond in that
category” (Rost, 2001, p.27). It is identical with the requirement that person
measures are sample-free (Linacre, 1992).
… the term invented by Georg Rasch (Rasch, 1977) and extensively
Specific
explored and used by Rasch researchers - combines elements of Separability
Objectivity
and Sufficiency. It has been called the crowning achievement of Georg
Rasch’s work (Wright, 1988) and is one of the most important theoretical
merits of the Rasch Model. The concept describes that item parameters do
not depend on characteristics of persons taking the test and person
parameters do not depend on items that are chosen from an item pool. It
follows that resulting parameters also do not depend on the samples’ mean
location or its dispersion (Fischer and Molenaar, 1995).
… is related to Specific Objectivity. It is the requirement that “whenever
Latent
specific objectivity holds for a set of items and a set of persons, then there
Additivity
exists a representation of the model where the person and the item parameter
are connected by addition or subtraction” (Rost, 2001, p.28). Latent
Additivity is also connected to Conjoint Measurement. Rasch Modelling is
credited with fitting the requirements of Conjoint Measurement as specified
by Luce and Tukey (1964) in their groundbreaking paper (Bond and Fox,
2001; Perline, Wright and Wainer, 1979; Wright, 1997).
Constructing a Measure using Rasch Modelling
When constructing a measure using Rasch Modelling, the dimension of interest - the latent trait - needs to
be defined. The latent trait is the dimension on which items and persons can then be projected (Wright
and Stone, 1979). A latent trait is therefore represented by a “single dimension along which items can be
located in terms of their difficulty, δ and persons can be located in terms of their ability, β” (Andrich and
Luo, 2003, p.60).
In order to determine item difficulty and person ability, raw scores, which represent observations of the
latent trait, have to be transformed into measurements using a mathematical model, the Rasch Model
(Andrich, 1988) as described in the Rasch Model Formula above.
The Rasch Model represents a theoretical idealization of the inter-relationship of the data and neither
items nor persons will ever fit it perfectly (Bond and Fox, 2001). Each items’ fit and each persons’ fit is
examined individually, in order to determine whether items/persons depart unacceptably from the model
(Bond and Fox, 2001).
In latent trait theory, the main emphasis is on determining if a unidimensional structure is achieved by a
set of items expressed by tests of fit, as described above (Andrich, 1982). However, when it is of interest
to determine how well a measure separates the persons measured, a Person Separation Index is computed
to calculate the internal consistency for the data (Peck, 2000). This is analogous to measuring reliability in
Classic Test Theory (Andrich and Luo, 2003; Peck, 2000).
249
Overall, if observations of items and persons (observed values) fit Rasch’s mathematical model (expected
values) sufficiently, unidimensionality is achieved in the sense that “the single difficulty/ability
continuum is sufficient to explain the patterns of item/person performances” (Bond and Fox, 2001, p.29).
Although the fit of single items is of particular importance (as discussed above), a chi-square statistic is
also provided to determine the fit of the overall model statistically.
RMT is extensively used in educational measurement and a number of other areas in social science
(Soutar and Monroe, 2001), but has been almost entirely ignored in marketing and only recently gained
some attention (e.g. Salzberger, 2000; Salzberger et al., 1999; Salzberger, 1999; Soutar, Bell and Wallis,
1990; Soutar and Cornish-Ward, 1997; Soutar and Monroe, 2001). Applications of RMT in marketing
include the investigation of cross-cultural transferability of the ethnocentrism scale (Salzberger et al.
1999) and patterns of ownership (e.g. of durable goods or financial assets) (Soutar et al., 1990; Soutar and
Cornish-Ward, 1997) or people’s recreation patterns (Soutar and Ryan, 1999). Recently, Ganglmair and
Lawson (2002; 2003) have used Rasch Modelling to develop a scale measuring Affective Response to
Consumption (ARC), an extension to satisfaction that includes highly positive emotional terms. To the
researcher’s knowledge, no study interested in QOL measurement has employed Rasch Modelling.
WHY RASCH MODELLING MIGHT HELP TO IMPROVE QOL MEASUREMENT?
The previous section outlined theoretical merits of the Rasch Model that enable an improvement of
quality of measurement scales in general. The current section will summarize practical advantages when
constructing a measure with Rasch Modelling. As no empirical example in the context of QOL is
available, a closely related parallel – ARC - is used to illustrate the rich contrasts achieved with this
alternative measurement approach in comparison to measures developed with Classical Test Theory.
When trying to overcome the current shortcoming of negative skewness in QOL scales, two practical
aspects of Rasch Modelling are considered particularly useful: the process of item selection and reliability
issues.
By linking back to the research tradition of Thurstone and Guttman (Andrich, 1988; Engelhard, Jr., 1990;
Wright and Stone, 2004) the Rasch Model requires researchers to generate items that show different
intensity levels of the construct in question, e.g. QOL (Andrich, 1988; Salzberger et al., 1999; Salzberger,
1999; Salzberger, 2000; Wright and Stone, 1979). A useful answer pattern on a Rasch scale is easier
accomplished if item difficulty and person ability spread widely (Andrich, 1982). In RMT the emphasis
shifts from measuring one point in the construct towards measuring the entire dimension of interest. This
emphasis, on investigating the entire breadth of the construct can be found throughout the Rasch
Modelling process. Rasch Software programs like RUMM2010 (Andrich et al., 2001) provide indices and
visual displays that help to establish whether items spread sufficiently along the continuum as opposed to
clump together on one point of the dimension. This enables the researcher to visualize if and where
additional items are necessary to cover the entire dimension of the construct, including extreme positions.
When investigating the concept of ARC it has been found that items included in traditional satisfaction
measures tend to cluster together and therefore do not provide sufficient discrimination amongst
respondents.
As discussed, RMT is based on a strict mathematical model of a theoretical relationship with items and
respondents being projected on the same dimension. The model represents an ideal form and the
researcher is interested which items or persons derive more than expected from the ideal model (Bond and
Fox, 2001). Item and person fit in relation to the model are computed (Wright, 1977) and the items’
observed fit to the model is taken to investigate unidimensionality (Soutar and Monroe, 2001). It is
therefore the researchers task to determine, whether a satisfactory fit is still accomplished (Andrich and
Luo, 2003; Peck, 2000; Salzberger, 2002).
250
Rasch Modelling increases the richness of the results as terms and phrases that represent strongly
different aspects of the negative-positive QOL continuum can be included. The graphics provided by
Rasch computer software help to visualize if e.g. two items are really perceived as having different
intensity (difficulty) by respondents. Due to the mathematical merits of the Rasch Model, results (in
logits) are presented on a truly interval scale and the difference between each answer category can be
determined.
It is expected that graphic results from the related ARC construct can be presented in order to emphasize
these characteristics.
CONCLUSION
Overall, RMT does not only provide theoretical merits that support the development of better scales and
increase the standard of measurement in marketing, it also has practical, process oriented characteristics,
test statistics and visual display options that make it particularly suited to measure concepts like QOL.
The newly developed concept of Affective Response to Consumption has shown favourable results in a
satisfaction/positive affect context. Initial further research will re-investigate the existing D-T Scale
(Andrew and Whitey, 1976) regarding the suitability of the chosen items in general and their spread along
the QOL continuum. Additionally, the concept of ARC will be expanded towards QOL and a new scale,
suitable to investigate global QOL or domains thereof, will be developed. For the second research stage,
extensive item generation is necessary to make sure that potential items are not restricted to those already
included in currently used scale, e.g. the D-T scale as additional, emotionally stronger terms are expected
to reduce the skewness of results and increase discrimination among respondents.
REFERENCES
Andersen, E. B. and Olsen, L. W. (2001). The Life of Georg Rasch as a Mathematician and as a
Statistician, in A. Boomsma, M. A. van Duijn and T. A. Snijders (eds), Lecture Notes in Statistics:
Essays on Item Response Theory , Springer Verlag, New York, pp. 3–24.
Andrews, F. M. and Withey, S. B. (1976). Social Indicators of WellBeing Americas Perception of Quality
of Life , Plenum Press, New York.
Andrich, D. (1978). A Rating Formulation for Ordered Response Categories, Psychometrica 43(4): 561–
573.
Andrich, D. (1982). Using Latent Trait Measurement Models to Analyse Attitudinal Data: A Synthesis of
Viewpoints, in D. Spearit (ed.), The Improvement of Measurement in Education and Psychology ,
Australian Council for Educational Research, Melbourne, pp. 89–126.
Andrich, D. (1988). Rasch Models for Measurement, Sage University Paper Series on Quantitative
Applications in Social Science, California.
Andrich, D. and Luo, G. (2003). Modern Measurement and Analysis in Social Science, Unit Materials,
School of Education, Murdoch University, Perth.
Andrich, D., Sheridan, B. and Luo, G. (2001). RUMM2010: Rasch Unidimensional Measurement Models
[Computer Software].
251
Bonanno, G. A. and Mayne, T. J. (2001). The future of emotion research, in T. J. Mayne and G. A.
Bonanno (eds), Emotions: Current Issues and Future Directions, The Guilford Press, New York, pp.
398–410.
Bond, T. G. and Fox, C. M. (eds) (2001). Applying the Rasch Model , Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Mahwah, New Jersey.
Bramston, P., Pretty, G. and Chipuer, H. (2002). Unravelling Subjective Quality of Life: An Investigation
of Individual and Community Determinants, Social Indicators Research 59: 261–274.
Churchill, Jr., G. A. (1979). A Paradigm for Developing Better Measures of Marketing Constructs,
Journal of Marketing Research 16(February): 64–73.
DeVellis, R. F. (1991). Scale Development: theory and application , Sage Publications, Beverly Hills.
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective Well-Being, Psychological Bulletin 95(3): 542–575.
Diener, E. (2000). Subjective Well-Being: The Science of Happiness and a Proposal for a National Index,
American Psychologist 55(1): 34–43.
Diener, E. and Fujita, F. (1995). Methodological Pitfalls and Solutions in Satisfaction Research, in J. M.
Sirgy and C. A. Samli (eds), New Dimensions in Marketing/Quality of Life Research , Quorum
Books, Westport, pp. 27–46.
Diener, E. and Lucas, R. E. (2000). Emotional well-being, in M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland (
Handbook of Emotions (2nd edition), The Guilford Press, New York, pp. 325–337.
eds),
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E. and Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective Well-Being: Three Decades of
Progress, Psychological Bulletin 125(2): 276–302.
Embretson, S. E. and Reise, S. P. (2000). Item Response Theory for Psychologists , Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Mahwah, New Jersey.
Engelhard, Jr., G. (1990). History of Measurement, Rasch Measurement Transactions 4(4): 126–127.
Online at http://www.rasch.org/rmt/rmt44.htm.
Finn, A. and Kayande, U. (1997). Reliability Assessment and Optimization of Marketing Measures,
Journal of Marketing Research 34(May): 262–275.
Fischer, G. H. and Molenaar, I. W. (1995). Rasch Models, Recent Developments and Applications,
Springer Verlag, New York.
Fredrickson, B. L. and Branigan, C. (2001). Positive emotions, in T. J. Mayne and G. A. Bonanno (eds),
Emotions: Current Issues and Future Directions, The Guilford Press, New York, pp. 123–151.
Frisch, M. B., Clark, M. P., Rouse, S. V., Rudd, M. D., Paweleck, J.
Ganglmair, A. and Lawson, R. (2002). Conceptualising affective satisfaction, in R. N. Shaw, S. Adam
and H McDonald (eds), Proceedings of the 2002 Australia and New Zealand Marketing Academy
Conference (ANZMAC 2002, Deakin University, Melbourne. CD ROM.
252
Ganglmair, A. and Lawson, R. (2003). Measuring Affective Response to Consumption Using Rasch
Modeling, Journal of Customer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behaviour, 16: 198210.
Greenstone, A. and Kopplin, D. A. (2003). Predictive validity and sensitivity to change in quality of life
assessment and life satisfaction, in M. J. Sirgy, D. Rahtz and A. C. Samli (eds), Advances in
Quality-of-Life Theory and Research , Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dortrecht, pp. 191–210.
Hambleton, R. K. (1991). Fundamentals of Item Response Theory, Sage Publications, Newbury Park,
California.
Linacre, J. M. (1992). Why Fuss about Statistical Sufficiency, Rasch Measurement Transactions 6(3):
230. online at http://www.rasch.org/rmt/rmt63c.htm.
Luce, R. D. and Tukey, J. W. (1964). Simultaneous Conjoint Measurement: A New Type of Fundamental
Measurement, Journal of Mathematical Psychology 1(1): 1–27.
Maggino, F. and D’Andrea, S. S. (2003). Different Scales for Different Survey Methods: Validation in
Measuring The Quality of University Life, in M. J. Sirgy, D. Rahtz and A. C. Samli (eds),
Advances in Quality-of-Life Theory and Research , Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dortrecht, pp.
233–256.
McLemee, S. (2003). Getting Emotional, The Chronicle of Higher Education 49(24 (Feb.21, 2003)).
Available online: http://chronicle.com/free/v49/i24/24a01401.htm.
Nunnally, J. C. and Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric Theory , McGraw Hill, New York.
Olsen, L. W. (2003). Essays on Georg Rasch and his contributions to statistics, Institute of
Economics, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen. Available online:
http://www.rasch.org/olsen.pdf.
Peck, B. (2000). Ethnicity: A Study of Participation, Aspirations and Achievement in Education, A thesis
submitted in part completion of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Education, Murdoch
University, Perth.
Perline, R., Wright, B. D. and Wainer, H. (1979). The Rasch Model as Additive Conjoint Measurement,
Applied Psychological Measurement 3(2): 237–255. Available online:
http://www.rasch.org/memo24.htm.
Peterson, R. A. and Wilson, W. R. (1992). Measuring Customer Satisfaction: Fact and Artifact, Journal of
the Academy of Marketing Science 20(1): 61–71.
Rasch, G. (1960/80). Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests; With a Foreword
and an Afterword by Benjamin D. Wright., MESA Press, Chicago.
Rasch, G. (1977). On Specific Objectivity: An Attempt at Formalizing the Request for Generality and
Validity of Scientific Statements, Danish Yearbook of Philosophy 14: 58–93. available online at
http://209.238.26.90/memo18.htm.
Rossiter, J. R. (2002). The C-OAR-SE procedure for scale development in marketing, International
Journal of Research in Marketing 19(4): 305–335.
253
Rost, J. (2001). The Growing Family of Rasch Models, in A. Boomsma, M. A. van Duijn and T. A.
Snijders (eds), Lecture Notes in Statistics: Essays on Item Response Theory, Springer Verlag, New
York, pp. 3–24.
Salzberger, T. (1999). How the Rasch model may shift our perspective of measurement in marketing
research, in J. Cadeaux and M. Uncles (eds), Proceedings of the 1999 Australia and New Zealand
Marketing Academy Conference(ANZMAC) Sydney , p. CD ROM.
Salzberger, T. (2000). An alternative way of establishing measurement in marketing research - its
implications for scale development and validity, in A. O’Cass (ed.), Proceedings of the 2000
Australia and New Zealand marketing Academy Conference(ANZMAC) , Gold Coast.
Salzberger, T. (2002). Personal communication [email] received on 26.09.2002.
Salzberger, T., Sinkovics, R. R. and Schlegelmilch, B. B. (1999). Data Equivalence in Cross-Cultural
Research: A Comparison of classical Test Theory and Latent Trait Theory Based Approaches,
Australasian Marketing Journal 7(2): 23–38.
Seligman, M. E. (1998). Building Human Strength: Psychology’s Forgotten Mission, APA Monitor 29(1).
available online at http://www.apa.org/monitor/jan98/pres.html.
Seligman, M. E. and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive Psychology, American Psychologist 55(1): 5–
14.
Shaw, E. H. (1997). New Dimensions in marketing/Quality-of-Life Research: Book Review, Journal of
Macromarketing 17(1): 132–135.
Sirgy, M. J., Rahtz, D. R., Cicic, M. and Underwood, R. (2000). A Method for Assessing Residents’
Satisfaction with Community-Based Services: A Quality-of-Life Perspective, Social Indicators
Research 49: 279–316.
Soutar, G. N., Bell, R. C. and Wallis, Y. M. (1990). Consumer Acquisition Patterns for Durable Goods: A
Rasch Analysis, Asia Pacific International Journal of Marketing 2(1): 31–19.
Soutar, G. N. and Cornish-Ward, S. P. (1997). Ownership Patterns for Durable Goods and Financial
Assets: a Rasch Analysis, Applied Economics 29(7): 903–911.
Soutar, G. N. and Monroe, G. (2001). Rasch modelling: An alternative to summated scales, ANZAM
2001 - Closing the Divide, Auckland.
Voss, K. E., Stem, Jr., D. E. and Fotopoulos, S. (2000). A Comment on the Relationship between
Coefficient Alpha and Scale Characteristics, Marketing Letters 11(2): 177–191.
Westbrook, R. A. (1980). A Rating Scale for Measuring Product/Service Satisfaction, Journal of
Marketing 44(Fall): 68–72.
Wright, B. (1980). Foreword to Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests, in G.
Rasch (ed.), Probabilistic
Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests, MESA Press, Chicago, pp. ix–xix.
254
Wright, B. D. (1977). Solving Measurement Problems with the Rasch Model, Journal of Educational
Measurement 14(2): 97–116. available online at: http://www.rasch.org/memo42.htm.
Wright, B. D. (1988). Georg Rasch and Measurement, Informal Remarks by Ben Wright at the Inaugural
Meeting of the AERA Rasch Measurement SIG, New Orlins, 1998, Rasch Measurement
Transactions 2(3). available online at http://www.rasch.org/rmt/rmt23a.htm.
Wright, B. D. (1992). IRT in the 1990s: Which Models Work Best, Ben Wright’s opening remarks in his
invited debate with Ron Hambleton, Session 11.05, AERA Annual Meeting 1992, Rasch
Measurement Transactions 6(1): 196–200. available online at
http://www.rasch.org/rmt/rmt61a.htm.
Wright, B. D. (1997). Fundamental Measurement for Psychology, MESA Research Memorandum 64.
available online at http://www.rasch.org/memo64.htm.
Wright, B. D. and Stone, M. H. (1979). Best Test Design, MESA Press, Chicago.
255
__________________________________________________
Native People and Quality-of-Life: A Macromarketing
Perspective
Alf H. Walle, Erskine College
Abstract
The perspectives of macromarketing provide useful tools for dealing with quality-of-life issues involving
Native people who are increasingly impacted by intrusions from the outside world. In this paper, models
of quality-of-life that stem from macromarketing are discussed in terms of parallel perspectives that are
emerging in the field of Native studies. Both macromarketing and Native studies focus upon providing
viable and workable alternatives to the “dominant social paradigm.” These similarities facilitate a
melding of these two streams of thought in ways that more effectively serve Native, indigenous, and
traditional people who are involved with marketing strategies and tactics.
Beyond Generic Measures
Increasingly, decision makers are considering the needs and vulnerabilities of “indigenous”, “Native”,
and “traditional” people7 who are impacted by economic development, business, and marketing activities.
The tools that are used in such situations have often been generic in nature; by this I mean that the same
basic methods are used to analyze many different peoples in spite of their differences. As a result of this
strategy of investigation, measures such as “social indicators” (that have been created to serve within the
mainstream, industrialized world) might be inappropriately employed when investigating small, rural, and
culturally distinctive groups.
Ethical and practical problems arise when those who frame policies and strategies involving atypical
populations do not adequately consider their distinctiveness. Native cultures are distinct, but these
differences may not be adequately recognized and acknowledged when generic methods of analysis are
employed.
Unfortunately, furthermore, many policy makers have incorrectly assumed that Native cultures and other
small scale societies are doomed to extinction. Those who embrace this perspective tend to feel little
ethical responsible to protect and nurture Native, indigenous, or traditional cultures because they appear
to be inevitably and irrevocably doomed to oblivion. This belief is typically based on the premise that the
forces and influences of what has come to be called “global culture” are destined to remake the entire
world in its image. Although those who embrace this global perspective may feel a moral commitment to
cushion the pain of change and cultural extinction, on many occasions little ethical thought has been
geared towards preventing such developments.
In recent decades, however this view of inevitable extinction has softened. As Wiessener has observed:
Contrary to widely held expectations, they [indigenous people] and their cultures have
survived and are making their presence known in the Americas, Asia, Africa, Australia,
the Pacific, and even on the launching pads of colonization, the continent of Europe
(1999 60.)
7
The distinctions between these terms will be dealt with below.
256
Such ideas are reflective of macromarketing thoughts; thus I once wrote in the Journal of Macromarketing
in regard to the limitations of global theories:
…if Afro-American culture can resist 400 years of depravity, forced illiteracy, and slave
drivers beating down traditions, can other cultures survive the onslaught of Coke and
Pepsi? And if today’s cultures can survive such relatively trivial threats, will global
marketing apply only in some rather specializes cases where homogeneous products are
easily translated into different lifestyles? (Walle 1985 87.)
Although the example cited above concerns a traditional group that does not qualify as Native, the basic
point is well taken: cultures, traditions, and social groups have an ability to survive even when they face
great pressures. Because the survival of indigenous cultures and peoples is increasingly recognized,
dealing with these groups in equitable ways that enhance their quality-of-life emerges as an important
ethical and practical consideration.
This kind of orientation already has a high profile in fields such as Native studies that seek to more fully
reflect the unique and distinct needs of the indigenous community and other ethnic minorities. These
initiatives are (1) focused towards the uniqueness of Native8 communities and (2) they seek to help Native
people achieve equity, self-determinism, and an enhanced quality-of-life. Macromarketing is poised to
provide leadership in these important areas.
As indicated above, the business disciplines have often employed inappropriate global models when
evaluating Native and other “small scale” societies, the opportunities they provide, their vulnerabilities,
and their strengths. Global models tend to employ generic quality-of-life indicators that are used as
surrogate measures. Although these tools are “objective” and quantifiable, they are often criticized for
being culture bound and for not being relevant to the needs of Native cultures and their members. Thus,
while the percentage of families with a flush toilet in a community or the percentage of people with fulltime, cash-paying jobs may be appropriate measures of quality of life within the industrialized world, they
might be inappropriate measures when analyzing the quality of life of rural communities that rely upon a
subsistence way of life.
Progress, however, is being made in this area; macromarketer Mark Peterson, for example, is currently
grappling with the fact that members of different cultures evaluate the same quality-of-life criteria
indicators in different ways (2004.) Perspectives such as Peterson’s are pointing marcromarketing
towards a more flexible and sophisticated means of analyzing quality-of-life; these sensitive and flexible
methods can more appropriately evaluate specific social groups no matter how distinctive they may be.
By merging the perspectives of contemporary macromarketing with parallel theories, practices, and
orientations that are being developed within disciplines such as Native studies, this paper suggests ways
in which these diverse intellectual streams can be merged in a productive and synergistic manner.
As we shall see, these initiatives within macromarketing are similar to the perspectives of those who
study Native communities and/or serve as their advocates. Native studies specialists point to the
distinctiveness of specific Native people and they affirm that these people need to be evaluated on their
own terms, not with reference to the vague and undifferentiated measures of a global age.
Thus, both marcomarketing and Native studies are evolving in a manner that views people in terms of
their uniqueness and distinctiveness. Although macromarketing and Native studies derive from different
8
This paper tends to deal with a variety of issues that are emerging in the field of Native studies. As a result,
parallels between macromarketing and Native studies are the focus. The discussions provided may be useful when
thinking about other groups, such as indigenous and traditional people.
257
intellectual traditions, they and their perspectives are converging. This situation provides a role for
macromarketing (largely and historically a strategic discipline) to help Native studies (largely and
historically a humanistic discipline) to more fully and effectively deal with the intrusion of outside forces
into small, atypical communities. This paper deals with the exciting possibilities and challenges that are
created by the collaboration of these two fields.
A Defining of Terms
In this paper, a number of different terms, including “Native”, “indigenous”, and “traditional”, are used in
largely interchangeable ways. Each will be discussed in regard to their subtle differences as well as their
similarities.
The term “Native” (with a capital N) refers to a person who is descended from the historic (or prehistoric)
peoples who are associated with a region: typically, those who lived in a region immediately before the
era of Western expansion. American Indians, the Maori of New Zealand, and Native Hawaiians are
representative examples of Native people. The term “native” (with a small n) refers to someone from an
area who is descended from later immigrants. Thus, a White person from Michigan would be a “native”
of Michigan. A member of the Chippewa tribe of Michigan, in contrast, would be a “Native.” The term
“Native” is often used by people to describe themselves. Many academic programs are called “Native
studies” (although other more specific titles, such as “American Indian Studies”, “Cherokee Studies”,
“Maori studies”, etc. may identify such programs in a more focused manner.
The term “Indigenous”, in contrast, often implies or suggests patterns of hurtful treatment. The term
often refers to the Natives of a region who find themselves to be a distinct minority that faces some kind
of discrimination or hardship due to their ethnic/racial identity. Thus, the “Convention Concerning
Indigenous and Tribal Populations in Independent Countries” states in part:
[Indigenous people are]…tribal peoples in independent countries whose social, cultural
and economic conditions distinguish them form other sections of the national community,
and whose status is regulated wholly or partially by their own customs or traditions or by
specific laws and regulations….[or people] who, irrespective of their legal status, retain
some or all of their own social, economic, cultural, and political institutions…
(International Labor Organization 1989.)
As Sigfreid Wiessner has observed:
…indigenous people are best described as groups traditionally regarded, and selfdefined, as descendents of the original inhabitants of the lands…These people are and
desire to be culturally, socially and/or economically distinct from the dominant groups in
society, at whose hands they have suffered, in past or present, a pervasive pattern of
subjugation, marginalization, dispossession, exclusion, and dispossession (1999 60)
According to this formal definition, not all “Native” people are “indigenous.” The people of Iceland, for
example, certainly fit the criteria of a “Native” people because they are descended from the first human
inhabitants of the region. Native Icelanders, however, control their government and they are not
persecuted or treated as second class citizens. They are in not a minority group and they are not
discriminated against in any systematic manner. Thus, Icelanders, do not fit the criteria of “indigenous”
(as described in the formal definition above) even though they are undisputedly Native.
Besides the terms “Native” and “indigenous”, a third term is “traditional people.” Traditional people may
or may not be descended from the original population of a region. And, perhaps, traditional people do not
face discrimination and hardship because of their identity. But these people still possess a culture that
258
makes them distinct. This difference, furthermore, is recognized both by the group and by outsiders. A
classic example of non-Native traditional people is Appalachian hill folk. They are descendents of White
immigrants of the 18th century, so they are not Native. They are not systematically discriminated against
in a way that reflects the historic plight of many indigenous people. Nevertheless, traditional people share
many of the concerns and pressures that are faced by Native and indigenous people.
Some Native and indigenous people, furthermore, simultaneously fit the definition of “traditional people”
because they live a life that is traditional and distinct from the mainstream population. Thus, “traditional
people” refers to lifestyle, “Native people” refers to genetically circumscribed groups that maintain a
distinctiveness, while “indigenous people” is a term that denotes ramifications, such as those associated
with discrimination.
While this paper centers (as a matter of convenience) upon Native people, much of the discussion is
equally applicable to other types of people including traditional and indigenous populations. Unless there
is a specific and unavoidable reason to do so, the discussions will be phrased in a broad enough manner to
encompass all of these groups. By doing so, the applicability of this paper is usefully expanded.
Quality-Of-Life and Marketing
This paper deals with quality of life issues involving Native people and other distinctive populations
(such as indigenous and traditional people.) The distinctions between these groups and their similarities
were discussed above. With that task completed, issues related to the impact of marketing upon their
quality-of-life is discussed. The role of macromarketing in such analyses is emphasized.
The field of marketing and macromarketing is increasingly concerned with quality-of-life issues. The
work of Joe Sirgy has been especially influential in this regard (Sirgy 2001, Sirgy and Cornwell 2001,
Sirgy and Rahtz, Cicic, and Underwood 2000, Lee, Sirgy, Larsen, and Wright 2002.) According to
Sirgy,
In addition to the ethics test, researchers have developed the quality-of-life (QOL) concept as
a tool with which to further encourage the application of ethics in marketing. The key
marketing objective of increasing short-term profitability sometimes falls in conflict with the
marketing ethicist’s objective of protecting the consumer (Lindquist and Sirgy 2003 527).
Initiatives regarding quality-of-life that exist within marketing thought are important because they
acknowledge how the efforts and impacts of marketing can either enhance or detract from the quality-oflife that people experience. Increasingly, Native studies scholars seek to use a similar perspective when
envisioning the relationship of Native people to the outside world. Native groups are often viewed as
being negatively impacted by the economic relationships with which they are involved. The goal of such
research (and advocacy related to it) is to evaluate the disruptions triggered by outside impact, mitigate
the resulting stress, and (where possible) insure Native people are treated in an equitable manner.
As Mark Peterson’s work demonstrates, macromarketing is also developing ways to view quality-of-life
in a culturally specific (not a generic) manner; this trend allows the field of macromarketing to play a role
in projects that involve particular Native people who are in contact with the larger world.
Marketing, as typically envisioned, is the discipline that deals with consumers and their well-being. As a
result, it should come as little surprise that discussions regarding quality-of-life and marketing typically
center upon the act of consumption and its ramifications. Nevertheless, the issues of concern to marketers
are broader than that. Thus, Sirgy observes:
259
Quality-of-Life marketing seeks to resolve the conflict by guiding marketers to seek the
best interests of consumers and other publics (Lindquist and Sirgy 2003 527, emphasis
mine.)
As Sirgy’s comment demonstrates, quality-of-life measures involving marketing can be used to evaluate
the influence of marketing behavior upon any group of people that is affected in some way by marketing
behavior and its impacts. This focus transcends those who are part of the patron-client dyad.
The idea of externalities (those who exist outside of a buyer-seller relationship, but are impacted by it)
demonstrates that the processes of marketing (and the environments created by it) often affect people who
are not involved in any way with marketing activities. A full consideration of quality-of-life issues that
involve marketing must take these people into account. Native studies scholars also seek to evaluate the
impacts of outside intervention in order to help people choose viable and equitable strategies of economic
development. Because macromarketing expands beyond the patron-client dyad, it is uniquely poised as a
business discipline that can help Native studies scholars to examine issues of quality-of-life that that are
linked to marketing in some manner.
The well-being of people who are willingly a part of the patron/client relationship also
needs to be evaluated with reference to how marketing impacts quality-of-life. This is
especially true because Native people may not totally envision the complete impact of a
marketing relationship upon them and, therefore, they may need advice in order to fully
understand its likely influence. Because macromarketing has long embraced a broad and
robust perspective regarding quality-of-life, it is well suited to aid Native people better
understand the implications of strategies that are available to them.
When dealing with quality-of-life issues, Sirgy and Lee argue that there are “significant public policy and
managerial implications” involved in quality-of-life issues. They enumerate them as:
1. Public policy makers can identify areas of improvement
2. Policy makers can develop a closer link between their specific public policies and well being
of impacted stakeholders, and
3. Quality-of-life issues and factors can be measured over time. (Sirgy and Lee 2004 38,
wording adjusted, implications not changed.)
As indicated above, decision makers often need specialized models that are developed with particular
Native cultures in mind. As Mark Peterson’s work (2001, 2004) demonstrates, quality-of-life evaluations
differ cross-culturally. Based on this observation, Peterson argues that particular populations should have
a role in determining how quality-of-life data that is gathered, evaluated, and interpreted. He states:
The results of this study suggest that both French and the U.S. groups can be described as
having heterogeneous perspectives in their ranking of the QOL dimensions based on
importance. In other words, no QOL dimension is universally perceived as being the
most important…. This study seeks to demonstrate the viability of using citizen
perspectives on QOL dimensions in deriving [their]…relative importance (Peterson 2004
40.)
Peterson’s research indicates that people from different cultures will view and evaluate quality-of-life
measures in distinct ways. If significant differences can be seen when comparing France and the United
States (two industrialized Western nations) consider how much difference will exist between mainstream
business people from an industrialized Western country vs. members of a Native culture that reside in a
rural area and practice a traditional way of life.
260
In any event, “marketing quality-of-life issues” is an umbrella term that extends to include the
examination of all stakeholders who are impacted in any way by the marketing and consumption of goods
and/or services. Quality-of-life evaluations, furthermore, consider both (1) “externalities”: impacted
groups that are not involved in the marketing process and (2) those people who are actively involved with
marketing and related activities.
As discussed below, these perspectives can help macromarketing to work with Native people who seek
self-determinism, hope to gain parity with others, and wish to enjoy a high quality-of-life. This type of
collaborative work can emerge as an area where macromarketing has a significant role to play in policy
making. The fact that macromarketing has long been interested in economic development makes the field
an especially appropriate tool to use in such circumstances.
Macromarketing and the Dominant Paradigm
Macromarketing is concerned with providing alternatives to the dominant social paradigm in ways that
parallel the evolution of Native studies. Much of this work is associated with William E. Kilbourne
(Kilbourne, W. McDonagh, and A. Prothero 1997, Kilbourne, W. and Beckman, S. C. 1998, , W. , S. C.
Beckman, A. Lewis, and Y. van Dam 2001, and Kilbourne, W. S. C. Beckman, and E. Thelen 2002.)
Even though this research stream in macromarketing is primarily concerned with the physical
environment, the underlying theme of these efforts demonstrates that the intellectual thrust of
macromarketing fits in well with attempts to help Native people (and policies involving them) to develop
models that expand beyond conventional ways of thinking that reflect modern, Western, and global
perspectives. By transcending the dominant social paradigm, more appropriate and equitable ways to
envision Native people and the challenges they face can be developed.
Sirgy and Lee provide a useful means of invigorating such analysis with a strong quality-of-life focus. As
will be recalled from the discussions above, they list three issues that need to be discussed regarding
quality-of-life (Sirgy and Lee 2004 38)
They are:
1 Identifying areas of improvement
2 Finding links between policies and the well-being of specific stakeholders
3. Measuring quality-of-life over time.
Each is briefly discussed:
Identify areas of improvement
Native people often complain that the existing relationships between themselves and the larger
community are in need of significant improvement. On many occasions, however, Native people and/or
their advocates do not frame these concerns in a manner that can be adequately addressed by decision
makers, especially those with a business background.
Macromarketing, however, is an applied strategic science that is concerned with quality-of-life issues in
ways that mesh with the perspectives of Native studies. In addition, as we have seen, macromarketing
(Peterson 2004) is evolving in order to view people on their own terms, not merely with reference to
generic, “one size fits all” methods of analysis. As a result of this trend, macromarketing can play a vital
role in helping Native people to identify and articulate areas that they believe are in need of improvement.
261
The dominant 4 Ps marketing management approach9, focuses upon serving customers through
manipulating certain controllable variables that are available to the organization. Although many benefits
potentially derive from this method, it tends to draw attention away from everything but the seller/buyer
dyad. As a result, other considerations (such as the quality-of-life of various stakeholders not directly
impacted by marketing) are easily eclipsed.
Alternative paradigms from the history of marketing thought, however, may prove useful as Native
people seek to explore the impact of marketing. The “institutional approach”, for example, centers upon
institutions and organizations that are involved with and/or impacted by the marketing process. These
institutions are viewed as having their own needs that are distinct from the requirements and wishes of
customers. Nonetheless, the needs of these institutions must be satisfied in order for customers to be
most effectively served. Although the institutional approach does not ignore customers or discount their
importance, it does recognize that satisfying customers is a complicated process that involves various
institutions, whose needs and vulnerabilities must also be recognized and addressed.
Following this lead, macromarketers can adapt the institutional approach in ways that serve the needs of
Native people by envisioning their cultures and societies as institutions. These Native “institutions” are
typically distinct from the social institutions of the mainstream world. Classic marketing theory
emphasizes that the character and demands of the institutions that are involved in the marketing process
need to be addressed.
Those who specialize in examining Native cultures and serving as their advocates are equally concerned
with Native institutions. By adapting the institutional approach of marketing and by viewing Native
cultures with reference to it, macromarketers can usefully examine the types of cultural institutions
embraced by Native people and how their needs can be most effectively and equitable addressed. By
recognizing these needs and acting accordingly, the impact of marketing strategies involving Native
people can be more effectively modeled.
The cultural institutions of many Native cultures, for example, are small scale and rural. These
characteristics are obvious to even the most casual observer. Looking deeper, more profound and subtle
differences can be seen. The institutions of Native people often deal with intellectual property rights in a
manner that is very different from that of the Western nations. Native cultural institutions are often
intimately bound to a particular ecological niche. Many Native people harbor sacred traditions; their roles
and importance should be taken into account when strategies involving the marketing of this heritage are
being developed. In general, the cultural institutions employed by marketing or impacted by it need to be
considered when marketing strategies are being planned, implemented, and evaluated.
Many Native people, for example, market their traditions in the form of cultural tourism. Folklorists and
Native studies specialists have long grappled with the fact that this arrangement often results in
significant stress within Native communities that can, for example, lead to an increase in dysfunctional
behavior, such as substance abuse. By viewing the tourism marketing relationship with an eye towards
the cultural institutions involved and the pressures faced by them, macormarketers can provide a better
understanding regarding how outside intervention impacts these social groups. Addressing these issues in
9
Readers with a non-marketing background will be interested to know that the dominant approach to the discipline
as it now exists was introduced in 1960 with the publication of E. Jerome McCarthy’s Basic Marketing: A
Managerial Approach. McCarthy’s approach divided marketing into (1) choosing a target market and (2)
manipulating the controllable variables available to the firm to satisfy it. The list of controllable variable presented
by McCarthy includes “Product”, “Price”, “Place” (distribution), and “Promotion.” Because each of these
controllable variables starts with the letter P, the approach is commonly called 4 Ps marketing management. This
basic method has become almost universal within marketing.
262
such a manner facilitates Native studies specialists joining with macromarketers in order to craft business
relationships that enhance the quality-of-life enjoyed by the Native community.
By viewing Native cultures (both individually and collectively) as institutions with specific characteristics
and needs, macromarketers can more effectively envision ways to tailor marketing relationships around
the needs, vulnerabilities, and desires of specific Native people. Since this approach recognizes the
uniqueness of Native cultures, it provides a way of avoiding an overly generic and homogeneous global
approach. This manner of envisioning Native cultures and the impact of the outside world upon them
parallels the methods of analysis that are currently employed within Native studies. By envisioning the
impact of marketing from this perspective, macromarketing can tap its heritage as a strategic science in a
way that facilitates collaborating with Native studies, a humanistic area of thought. By working in this
collaborative fashion, macromarketing and Native studies can join forces to improve the situation faced
by Native people.
Finding links between policies and the well being of specific stakeholders
Increasingly, macromarketing has begun to address quality-of-life issues in terms of the local and
subjective needs that are felt by specific peoples. This initiative creates a situation where macromarketing
can combine business theory and cultural studies in order to model the full impact of change upon Native
communities. Macromarketing can be especially effective in this area because (as we discussed above)
the field is transcending generic measures of quality-of-life, and is increasingly considering the subjective
measures experienced and articulated by specific peoples.
As mentioned above, Native cultures and their patterns of life can be envisioned as cultural “institutions.”
This perspective allows a modified marketing institutional approach to be used when dealing with specific
Native people and their heritage. In doing so, cultures are viewed as overarching institutions that have
needs that must be acknowledged and satisfied if the quality-of-life of the community is to be enhanced.
By reframing the institutional approach in this manner, the perspectives of marketing can be usefully
merged with Native studies.
When viewing the well-being of cultures that serve (in part) as marketing institutions, the value of this
institutional model is amplified because cultural institutions serve a wide variety of functions. Such a
perspective allows macromarketing and Native studies to merge their various visions of functionalism in
useful ways. Functionalism, a historic method of marketing analysis, deals with functions that are served
by the marketing process. The functionalism used by Native studies, in contrast, stems from the
theoretical social sciences and deals with a wider and more general array of functions that are performed
by cultures and their social institutions. While the focus of marketing is typically narrower than that of
humanistic disciplines, such as Native studies, the basic orientation is the same. In both cases, patterns of
behavior and response are discussed in terms of functions that are served.
Many Native studies scholars are indebted to structural/functional methods that have arisen in the social
sciences. Social structures are explained in terms of the roles they have played in the past. A key aspect
of functional theory is the premise that if one part of the structure changes, other aspects will be
transformed accordingly. Thus, altering one component of a system often triggers inadvertent and
unintended transformations throughout the entire structure. These unplanned changes need to be
anticipated and, where necessary, mitigated as marketing strategies and tactics are developed and
employed. Even scholars who embrace a poststrucutral stance tend to embrace such structural
perspectives.
Consider a Native culture that is being asked to market some aspect of its heritage. The process of
marketing is likely to change the way people relate to those traditions. Thus, through the process of being
marketed to outsiders, the function of traditions may evolve from serving the members of the community
263
to attracting revenues from the outside. The new function created by marketing needs to be
acknowledged. Perhaps the old and new functions compete with each other. Such a situation needs to be
addressed. If the new function overshadows the old, the resulting impacts will need to be considered. If,
for example, traditional Native crafts and folklore are transformed into marketable commodities, the
impact of this change needs to be considered. Native studies professionals and folklorists have long been
concerned with these issues, but they tend to deal with these changes from a humanistic, not a strategic
perspective. Macromarketing can help devise strategies and tactics to use in such circumstances.
By adapting the institutional and functional approaches, macromarketing can help to analyze the
connection between the well-being of Native peoples and specific decisions involving the marketing
process. Because these approaches from marketing have parallels that influence Native studies scholars,
a broad and multi-disciplinary perspective can be developed. Both marketing and Native studies embrace
methods for assessing the impact of change that are related to the functions of Native cultural institutions.
The similarity between these approaches creates a common ground that can facilitate scholars from the
Liberal Arts and macromarketing working together in fruitful ways. Such joint efforts can enhance the
well-being of Native communities that are involved in marketing arrangements. By exploring how
various functions of society may be altered by marketing relationships, the evolving situation faced by
Native people can be more effectively envisioned and more robust strategies can be developed in order to
enhance the quality-of-life of Native people.
Measuring quality-of-life over time.
Native people and their advocates are vitally concerned with long-term trends, not merely with the
quality-of-life that exists at a particular point of time. Macromarketing thought involving quality-of-life
is also concerned with a long term perspective. As a result, macromarketing is poised to help those
involved in Native studies to deal with issues involving the evolution of quality-of-life over time.
Macromarketing is well equipped to serve in this collaborative role because it is a strategic science that
has developed tools for analyzing how marketing relationships impact quality-of-life. While
macromarketing possesses a theoretic perspective, however, it often lacks the historic framework that is
essential when transitions over time and their significance are being explored.
Those involved in Native studies, in contrast, possess a strong historical perspective, but they have not,
typically, been trained in the strategic sciences. By combining forces, a more complete picture of the
impact of marketing upon Native quality-of-life can be presented. This more robust approach can lead to
useful perspectives regarding how in the long term marketing relationships potentially impact the quality
of life of Native people.
One illustrative example of this approach is provided by Harold Napoleon’s Yunarag: The Way of Being
Human (1996.) Napoleon, a Native Alaskan, examines the impacts of outside intervention upon his
community over a period of many years. Although not specifically concerned with marketing and its
effects, Napoleon does demonstrate that when significant and unmitigated change from the outside takes
place, the quality-of-life of the people may be in jeopardy.
Napoleon specifically observes that the quality-of-life experienced by people often declines although
conventional social indicators might point in another direction. Thus, Napoleon documents that even
though the material well-being of Native people improved due to contact with the outside world, the
overall quality-of-life declined (as demonstrated by a rapid rise in dysfunctional behaviors.) Thus,
Napoleon provides a useful analysis regarding shifts in quality-of-life that occur over time due to external
forces impinging upon Native people.
264
Macromarketing needs to accurately perceive how changes in quality-of-life over time might be triggered
by outside interventions, especially those associated (directly or indirectly) with marketing. This is
especially true in an era when Native people often seek to enhance their well-being through economic
relationships with the outside world. Although social scientists and humanists have been quick to
recognize the mixed blessings associated with economic development, the strategic sciences (such as
marketing) have often lacked the tools needed to address these issues.
The perspectives of marcomarketing can be used in order to address the impact of changes that, over time,
are triggered by outside intervention. By following this course of analysis, marcomarketing can join with
Native studies in order to better envision the impacts of change and how the negative impacts of
economic activities can be mitigated.
Conclusion
Macromarketing often needs to ally itself with other disciplines outside of business. By doing so, it can
provide unique services that no other strategic discipline is able to duplicate. Macromarketing has often
been relegated to the fringes of mainstream marketing. As a result, it has not exerted the influence that it
could have and should have. By bringing its strategic orientation to bear on the problems faced by other
disciplines, such as Native studies, macromarketing can play a more vital and prominent role in areas
such as economic development. Hopefully, macromarketers, while maintaining their status as marketers
will collaborate with other scholars and practitioners who possess parallel perspectives. Doing so can
usefully expand the role of the field in areas where our perspectives are wanted and needed.
Those who are involved with Native studies seek appropriate methods and perspectives in order to insure
that economic development enhances (and does not erode) the quality of life enjoyed by local
communities. The perspectives of macromarketing provide an array of useful tools of significant value
when examining and modeling how marketing can impact quality-of-life. These tools are useful when
addressing quality-of-life issues of (1) those who are involved with marketing relationships and (2) those
who are external to marketing relationships but are impacted by them.
This paper has discussed how macromarketing deals with quality-of-life issues and how these
perspectives can be of use to those involved in Native studies. Both macromarketing and Native studies
offer insights by providing alternatives to the “dominant social paradigm.” By combining these two
research streams, both fields will benefit from the power of synergism. The resulting, more robust,
perspectives will prove to be invaluable when strategies of development involving Native people are
being developed, refined, discussed, and mitigated.
REFERENCES
International Labor Organization (1989) Convention Concerning Indigenous and Tribal Populations in
Independent Countries quoted from Kokini, Te Puni (1994) Mana Tangata: Draft Declaration of
the Rights of Indigenous People 1993. (Wellington, New Zealand.) WWW version consulted P.
1.
Kilbourne, W. McDonagh, and A. Prothero (1997) Sustainable Consumption and the Quality of Life: A
Macromarketing Challenge to the Dominant Social Paradigm Journal of Macromarketing 17 (1)
2-24.)
Kilbourne, W. and Beckman, S. C. (1998) “Review and critical assessment of research on marketing and
the environment” Journal of Marketing Management 14 (6)
265
Killbourne, W. , S. C. Beckman, A. Lewis, and Y. van Dam (2001) “A Mulitnational Examination of the
Role of the Dominant Social Paradigm in the Environmental Attitudes of University Students,
Environment and Behavior 33 (2) 209-28.
Kilbourne, W. S. C. Beckman, and E. Thelen (2002) “The Role of the Dominant Social Paradigm in
Environmental Attitudes: A Mulit-national Examionation Journal of Business Research 55 (3)
193-204
Lee, Dong-Jin and M. Joseph Sirgy, Val Larsen, and Newell D. Wright (2002) “Developing a Subjective
Measure of Consumer Well Being” Journal of Macromarketing 22 (2) 158-69.
Lindguist, Jay D. and M. Joseph Sirgy (2003) Shopper, Buyer, and Consumer Behavior: Theory,
Marketing, and Public Policy Implications 2nd edition . (Cincinnati:Atomicdog.)
Napoleon, Harold (1996) Yunarag: The Way of Being Human (Fairbanks, Alaska: Alaska Native
Knowledge Network).
Peterson, Mark (2001) “Two Approaches For Deriving Weights for Objective QOL Dimensions” ISQLS
Conference, Washington, D. C. November.
Peterson, Mark (2004) “Comparing French and US Perspectives on the Relative Importance of Qualityof-life Dimensions” Macromarketing Scholarship and Education for a Global Century Papers of
the 29th Annual Macromarketing Seminar. 40-3.
Pirages, D. C. and Ehrlich, R. P (1974) Ark ll: Social Response to Environmental Imperatives (San
Francisco: Freeman.) 513-32.
Posey, Darrell and Dutfield, Graham (1996) Beyond Intellectual Property: Toward Traditional Resource
Rights for Indigenous People and Local Communities (Ottawa, Ontario: International
Development Resaerch Center)
Sirgy, M. Joseph (2001) Handbook of Quality-Of-Life Research: An Ethical Marketing Perspective
(Dordecht, the Netherlands: Kluver Acadamic Publishers.)
Sirgy, M. Joseph and Terri Cornwell (2001) “Further Validation of the Sirgy et al’s Measure of
Community Quality-of-life” Social Indicators Research 56 1215-43.
Sirgy, Joe, Don Rahtz, Muris Cicic, and Robert Underwood (2000) “A Method for Assessing Resident’s
Satisifaction With Community-Based Services: A Quality-of-life Perspective Social Indicators
Research 49 279-316.
Sirgy, M. Joseph and Lee, Don-Jin (2004) “Macro Measures of Consumer Well-Being (CWB): A Critical
Analysis and a Research Agenda” Macromarketing Scholarship and Education for a Global
Century Papers of the 29th Annual Macromarketing Seminar. 32-40.
Walle, Alf H. (1985) Review of Theodore Levitt’s The Marketing Imagination Journal of
Macromarketing V 5 # 1 pp. 85-8.
Walle, Alf (1999) Cultural Tourism: A Strategic Focus (Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press.)
Wiessner, Siegfried (1999) “Rights and Status of Indigeneous Peoples: A Global and Comparative and
International Legal Analysis” Harvard Human Rights Journal v 12 Spring
266
Appendix
Quality-of-Life: Native Perspectives
Native peoples are increasingly envisioning themselves as vested interest groups that are distinct from the
larger dominant cultures with which they interact. Native people are also envisioning themselves as
cultural entities that will not fade into extinction as a result of contact with the outside world. This view
is a reversal of earlier social-Darwinistic perspectives that suggested, for example, that the process of
globalization is destined to destroy local traditions and replace them with an increased cultural
homogeneity.
Native people do not deny that their cultures are being transformed, nor that they are borrowing the tools
and techniques the modern world. Advocates of Native people, however, do affirm that Native cultures
will survive as unique and as “going concerns” not as mere vestiges of another era.
These perspectives suggest a variety of strategies to use when Native people interact with the larger
community. Key issues to be examined here involve,
1. Controlling research that takes place among Natives populations
2. Focusing upon cultural disruption and their impacts
3. Envisioning quality-of-life in ecological terms
4. Dealing with the negative impacts of contact with the outside world.
5. Devising ways to mitigate negative impacts of contact.
Although other important consideration could be listed and discussed, this paper concentrates upon these
five representative and illustrative issues.
IN THE FINAL PAPER, THESE ISSUES WILL BE MORE FULLY DISCUSSED AND RELATED TO
THE MACROMARKETING LITERATURE.
Controlling research that takes place among Natives
Increasingly Native people are becoming concerned with intellectual property rights and the fact that the
methods of protecting rights that have developed in the Western world may not adequately guard the
heritage of the Native community. A vibrant literature on this subject is growing. It includes Posey and
Dutfield (1996 Battiste and Henderson (2000) Paterson and Darjala (2001) and Brascoupe and Mann
(2001.) As a result of this initiative, many Native people are becoming increasingly hesitant to allow
outsiders to research them and/or to allow outsiders to have access to traditional knowledge.
A key document that demonstrates this concern is Linda Smith’s Decolonializing Methodologies (1999.)
Smith’s work demonstrates fears harbored by many Native people in regard to research that is concerns
them. Her work, furthermore, documents ways in which Native people are choosing to control the
process of research and the access of outsiders to Native knowledge.
Native people who seek to protect their traditions need both legal advice and the participation of
marketers who can anticipate the likely impacts associated with various strategies involving Native
knowledge. Marcromarketing, being geared towards dealing with both (1) those who participate with
outside economic activity and (2) those who are external to it, has a leadership role to play in this area.
Cultural disruptions and their impacts.
Those who are involved in economic development have long been concerned with the potential hurtful
impacts of outside intervention and intrusion (Appell 1977.) Recent work in social psychology further
emphasizes this concern (Salzman 2001.) The Native community has, likewise, addressed these issues
(Maynard 1996, Harrison 1996, Napoleon 1996.) In 2004, the author of the paper published a book on
267
mental illness, Native societies, and rapid change triggered by the intrusion of outside forces
(Anonymous 2004.) A wide literature is growing on this issue: the citations above are merely illustrative.
The theories and methods of macromarketing has long been concerned with development issues including
its impact of indigenous populations. Models of macromarketing, for example, have served the cultural
tourism industry (Walle 1999.) By building upon this work and other perspectives of macromarketing,
the field can play a vital role in helping Native people to rationally decide upon strategies and policies. In
doing so, macromarketing can help Native people to analyze how economic activities involving
marketing will impact Native cultures and their members if they are either active participants or exist in
an external relationship.
Envisioning Quality-of-Life in Ecological Terms
Increasingly, there is a recognition that Native cultures have evolved in a particular ecological setting.
This has resulted in a better understanding regarding the potential negative impact that can occur through
disturbing that environment and/or altering the people’s relationship to it. The work of Oscar Kwagaley
has been particularly prominent in this area. (Kawagley 1995, personal communications 2002-5.) Other
organizatgions, such as the United States Army Corps of Engineers has also recognized the importance of
Native knowledge and the relationship of Native people to the land (1998.)
It is also recognized that Native people have subtle knowledge of their environment and that outsiders
often seek it out. When viewing such activities, it is important to keep in mind that a people’s emotional
well-being is often connected with their relationship to the environment. As a result, when marketing
impacts the land or the Native peoples’ access to it, decision makers need to consider the full implications
of their proposed actions. In many cases, unanticipated costs that cannot be measured in terms of dollars
and cents will be incurred. Macromarketers can play a vital role by creating models that facilitate
predicting and measuring these costs in terms that can be appreciated and acted upon by decision makers.
All too often, theoretical social scientists (who have little understanding of the strategic sciences) conduct
social impact assessments. When this happens, the resulting documents may not be expressed in ways
that can effectively guide decisions makers. Macromarketers have a unique background as a strategic
science that embraces a broad and multi-dimensional focus. This orientation can make the insights and
methods of macromarketing useful in such contexts.
Evaluating the Negative Impacts of Contact with the Outside World
Native people have long complained that economic initiatives that involve them often fail to take the
negative impacts of a decision into account. The vast literature on this subject is so great that it
overwhelms an ability to provide a sampling. Peterson’s (2001, 2004) work regarding evaluating qualityof-life in terms that are uniquely geared around specific populations provides macromarketing with a
means of helping to address this important issue.
Devising Ways to Mitigate the Negative Impacts of Contact.
Basically, those who are concerned with Native people need to consider the potentially negative impacts
of outside intervention, such as initiatives involving marketing. Macromarketing (which focuses upon
quality-of-life issues as well as economic development) is poised to provide leadership in this area.
Macromarketing is in a position to meld its perspectives with those of Native studies in order to create a
powerful perspective that will enhance both disciplines in synergistic ways.
Those involved with Native studies typically seek to offer alternatives to what can be called the
“dominant social paradigm” that has been defined as “the values, metaphorical beliefs, institutions, habits,
etc. that collectively provide social lenses through which individuals and groups interpret their social
world” (Pirages and Ehrlich 1974 7.) Those involved in Native studies envision the dominant social
paradigm that is typically used by those in business and economic development to be largely blind to the
unique needs of Native people. As a result, they suggest that even ethical and well-meaning decision
268
makers can easily reach decisions that inadvertently hurt Native people. Alert to this potential, Native
studies scholars seek to provide alternative models and measures that more accurately deal with the issues
that face the Native community.
Representative Issues
X
Native people and marketing traditions
Traditions have multiple purposes. Some can be sold, others cannot. Arrangements often need to reflect
the culture. X
Trauma involving selling traditions
X
Macromarketing can help frame these discussions
Native People and Intellectual Property
Ownership and marketing. Different types of ownership. The needs of view marketing of products with
the context of both local and international ownershiup Macromarketing has a role to play as intellectual
property rights are redefined.
Native People and the Environment
X
The Aldersonian Model and Quality of Life
Post-sturcutral = distinctiveness of groups from mainstream.
Structural = understanding the inherent structure of group.
Alderson structural. Value to studying native people
Both structural and poststructureal oppose dominant paradigm, but do so in different ways
269
Does Society Consider Marketing to Be a Science2
and Is There an Association Between Scientificity
and Benefit to Society?
Philip Barnes, University of Otago, New Zealand
Ben Wooliscroft, University of Otago, New Zealand
Abstract
Is marketing a science? That debate has gone on for many years. Marketing scholars have for many years
been concerned with whether marketing can attain the status of a science or not. The earliest marketing
academics set out to categorise marketing goods, marketing functions , and marketing institutions like a
botanist collecting specimens for the categorisation of that science (Sheth, Gardner and Garrett, 1986).
Katrichis and Stevens (2001) sought to clarify the status of marketing using Science2 the social
designation of science, with a sample of university professors, who it could be argued have considerable
self interest in the answers. This paper replicates and extends the research of Katrichis and Stevens
(2001) with a sample taken from the general population.
Introduction
Brown’s (1948) address to the American Marketing Association acted as springboard to a caveat
regarding to the scientificity of marketing. Katrichis and Stevens (2001) noted hitherto Brown’s speech
marketing theorists showed little interest in developing the discipline beyond a merely descriptive
explanation of phenomena. Brown (1948) was one of the first to comment on the need for marketing
theorists to employ a more analytical approach to marketing experimentation. The same year as this
speech, Alderson and Cox proposed a program, which addressed the need for marketing theory
development to elevate above the level of an empirical art and “establish truly scientific criteria for setting
up hypotheses and selecting the facts by means of which to test them” (1948, p.139).
There are opposing views to those of Brown (1948), and Alderson and Cox’s (1948), which Katrichis and
Stevens (2001) advocate as being triggered by Vaile’s (1949) reply. In this response, Vaile remarked that
marketing can never become a science, “but is an art which utilises scientific theories generated by other
disciplines in its application” (1949, p.521). From these original footings, the debate over the scientificity
of marketing continues to the present day.
During this fifty-year debate there have been numerous arguments put forward as to why marketing is, or
should be, a science (Anderson 1983; Arndt 1982; Bagozzi 1975; Hunt 1991; Lambe, Spekman, and Hunt
2000; Kolter and Levy 1969). However, among numerous theorists one common theme prevails, which is
the belief that it is desirable for marketing to gain the status of science. The debate contains four main
benefits associated with marketing becoming a science, namely:
1. Required in order to expand the scope of marketing
2. Required for the marketing discipline’s survival
3. Required for the advancement of the marketing discipline
4. Required for the marketing discipline to overcome its tarnished reputation
270
This replication and of Katrichis and Stevens' (2001) study has three objectives. Firstly, this study will
attempt, based on Anderson’s Science2 definition, to answer the question ‘is marketing a Science2 using a
sample from the general population? Secondly, this study will investigate if society awards scientific
status to those fields of inquiry that produce knowledge products that benefit the interests of society.
Thirdly, an expansion to the original study investigates societies' level of knowledge, or opinons, in
relation to the contributions of marketing.
The Logical Empiricist Perceptive of Science: Science1
There are a number of definitions of science and it is possible, based on Anderson’s (1983) work, to place
them into two streams. The first stream contains the logical empiricist view of science. Logical
empiricism is characterised by inductive statistical methods. Science1 begins with observations, and its
theories are justified by further observations providing probabilistic support for conclusions.
Sheth, Gardner and Garrett (1998) suggest that it is generally agreed that a Science1 contains four parts
that are outlined by Buzzell as,
1.
2.
3.
4.
a classified and systemised body of knowledge,
organised around one or more central theories and a number of general principles,
usually expressed in quantitative terms,
knowledge which permits the prediction and, under some circumstances, the control of future
events (1963, p.33).
Buzzell (1963) implies that the above criteria are generally accepted, however there are a number of
science definitions that are contrary to Buzzell's requirements.
One argument of particular importance is that of Kuhn (1970), which states that an essential element of
scientific progress is the breach of accepted scientific norms. Feyerabend (1981) illustrates that
historically violations of scientific norms have led to significant advances in scientific knowledge.
Anderson (1983) infers that Feyerbend (1981) and Kuhn (1970) consider there to be no general principles
of scientific practice. Rather that “knowledge claims are unique to each specific research area”
(Anderson, 1983, p.24). This can be seen to be linked to Popper’s (1962) idea that a discipline’s output is
more important than its subject mater.
Evaluating Marketing Using Science1
Looking more closely at the Science1 definition there are few academic arguments claiming marketing to
be a science (Hunt, 1991; members of the marketing science institute). Buzzell points out that there is “no
central theory; furthermore, there are very few accepted principles, and our ability to predict is limited
indeed” (1963, p.33). There were been attempts to create a general theory of marketing before Buzzell's
paper (c.f. Alderson, 1957; Nicosia, 1962). Post-1963, a number of theorists have tried, including Bartels
(1968), Hunt (2000) and Bagozzi (1984), unfortunately no one central theory has been widely accepted
and thus Buzzell’s (1963) view presently persists (Sheth et al 1988).
Hunt argues “withholding the label science until a discipline has central theories is unreasonable”(1991,
p.17). Basing his view around a similar analogy, that car is still a car before the engine is started.
Hutchinson, along with others, holds an opposing opinion.
There is a real reason, however, why the field of marketing has been slow to develop a unique body of
theory. It is a simple one: Marketing is not a science, it is rather an art or practice, and as such much more
closely resembles engineering, medicine, and architecture than it does physics, chemistry, or biology
(1952, p.289).
271
Hutchinson (1952) is therefore suggesting that engineers and architects do not study physics, and medical
practitioners do not study physics, chemistry and biology. In fact, it is likely that they do, making
engineering, medicine and architecture somewhat scientific and as marketing closely represents these arts
or practices it must also be somewhat scientific – perhaps best described as an applied science or
technology.
Brown (1948) noted a possible explanation for the slow marketing theory development in his appeal to
the American Marketing Association and Buzzell agrees when stating, “for most of the 50 years since the
beginnings of concentrated efforts to study marketing, our emphasis has been predominantly on fact
gathering”(1963, p.33). It could also be argued that there has been a concentrated effort on making the
marketing practitioner more profitable without focusing on theory development. Hutchinson maybe
incorrect as marketing is a rather young academic discipline (Gummeson, 2001). Buzzell’s (1963) science
definition does not include marketing as a science without a central theory of marketing. It should be
noted that in the view of Sheth et al the topic of a “general theory of marketing has not received sufficient
attention by marketing theorists” (1988, p.15). Is a general theory of marketing possible? If it is not,
marketing, under Buzzell’s widely agreed definition of science, can never be a science.
The Social Designation of Science: Science2
The second definition of science represents the social designation of science, Science2. Anderson (1983)
suggests that, via social designation, science is whatever society chooses to call science, with the related
proposition that society will grant scientific status to those disciplines that contribute to the best interest
of society. This definition of science is in keeping with the marketing discipline’s focus on consumer (or
potential consumer) perceptions of products, in this case the product being the marketing discipline.
Like Katrichis and Stevens' (2001) study, the rationale for this study stems directly from Anderson’s
(1983) work.
Evaluating Marketing Using Science2
Does marketing gain the stature of science under the Science2 definition, does society consider marketing
to be a science? Hunt considers this definition to be “irrationalistic” (1991, p.17). However, Hunt
suggests that the “label ‘science’ signifies that a discipline has ‘arrived’ in the eyes of other scientists”
(1991, p.17). Could it then be inferred that the label ‘science’ can only be allocated to a discipline when
scientists consider the discipline mature enough. Then does this not make other scientists the measure of
science?
The knowledge-claims of the non-sciences have as much epistemological warrant as the sciences
(Feyerbend, 1975). Feyerabend claims historical records demonstrate, “there is not a single rule, however
plausible, and however firmly grounded in epistemology, that is not violated at some time or another”
(1975, p.23).
Anderson (1983) argues that society is the measure of science rather than scientists. Madsen’s (1974)
Science2 conceptualisation of science emphasises the importance of social interaction, it,
suggests that society bestows a high epistemological status on science because it values its knowledge
products, and because it believes that science generally functions in the best interests of society as a
whole (Anderson 1983, p.26).
One might speculate that marketing theorists desire the marketing field of inquiry to gain scientific stature
to elevate its somewhat tarnished reputation among the general public (Farmer 1987, 1977, 1967). It is
clear for the creditability of sciences in general that withholding the label science does hold merit,
especially in relation to the label’s corresponding benefits. This also presents a possible explanation for
272
the high level of objection to marketing becoming a science amongst the current scientific disciplines. If
marketing was to become a science, marketing’s tarnished reputation could then rub off on the
creditability of the scientific status in general and in-turn downgrade the general public’s view of current
science disciplines.
According to Anderson the goal of science is to “ensure that its knowledge is dispersed to the greater
society so that it can serve to benefit in society’s best interests” (1983, p.26). In addition Anderson
suggests that the enhancement of the science disciplines relies to a certain degree on the achievement of
this goal when commenting “the more beneficial the knowledge products of a discipline, the more likely
the flow of resources for further advancement of its knowledge base”(1983, p.26). The question then has
to be asked - does marketing act in the best interest of society or is it simply exploiting it?
Some (one of the reviewers of this paper) suggest that consumer behavior may be seen as contributing to
society while marketing strategy is more likely to be seen as detrimental to society. It is questionable
whether this distinction would hold for society, as annecdotally many consider consider consumer
behavior to be a tool of business used to maniuplate consumers into buying things that they don’t want.
Marketing’s Contribution to Society
Katrichis and Stevens' (2001) findings indicate that there is statistically significant (p<.001) difference
between the responses of marketing professors and professors from other disciplines in relation to
marketing’s contributions to society. From a list of twenty-four disciplines, marketing professors ranked
marketing’s contribution 11th as opposed to the sample of non-marketing professors who ranked it 22nd.
Kasper (1993; cited in Wilkie and Moore, 1999) provides support for these findings in commenting that
studies have shown that the less familiar a person is with the marketing field, the more likely he or she
will equate marketing with advertising or selling.
Marketing is commonly seen as a soft area that contributes very little, if not negatively, to society (Wilkie
and Moore, 1999). However, one only needs to look at where marketing is derived from to understand
how truly complex the field really is. The complexity of the marketing discipline makes it extremely
difficult, if not impossible to measure its contributions. One reason for the difficulty relates to the
question ‘what is marketing?’ There is no single universally accepted definition of marketing (Sheth et
al, 1998). Therefore, how one measures the contributions of marketing is dependent on how marketing is
defined. Is it really possible to define marketing in a widely accepted way?
Even if we as marketers define marketing, it still may not be possible to quantify the contributions of
marketing. For example look at this definition proposed by the American Marketing Association,
“marketing is the process of planning, promoting and distribution of ideas, goods and services to create
exchanges that satisfy individuals and organisational objectives,” (AMA, 1985; cited in Hosley and Wee,
1988, p.44; emphasis added). The key word in the above definitions is process; marketing is not a time
bound single function but contributes to almost every aspect of getting the right product to the right
consumers, at the right time (Wilkie and Moore, 1999). For the purpose of this study, the generally
accepted list of marketing’s contributions provided by Wilkie and Moore (1983) in the Journal of
Marketing is considered to be sufficient.
Even using the Wilkie and Moore (1983) list of marketing’s contributions, it is impossible to single out
the contributions of marketing to any individual aspect of the marketing process, as each aspect
contributes something that adds to the aggregate benefits experienced by society. Furthermore, a large
number of these benefits are not measurable in monetary terms, such as improvements to living standards.
Across time the accumulation of billions of purchase occasions magnifies the benefits to society (Wilkie
and Moore, 1999). Therefore, Wilkie and Moore (1999) suggest that it may be beneficial to use the
economic measure of ‘utility’ when referring to the contributions of the marketing discipline.
273
Wiklie and Moore (1999) advocate that marketing provides four types of utility:
Form utility- dealing with operations and productions, the creation of the product.
Place utility- the value added by providing goods where buyers need them.
Time utility - activities to ensure customers can obtain and consume goods when they are needed.
Possession utility- allows customers to use goods for the desired purposes.
Although marketing provides all four forms of utility, consumers benefit most from product usage
(possession utility) and because marketers benefit from the payment for the purchase, they tend to focus
on the purchase process and overlook other elements required in providing possession utility. This might
explain why marketing is commonly associated with advertising and sales and the other contributions of
marketing are overlooked.
Discussion on the Results of Katrichis and Stevens Paper
The data of Katrichis and Stevens study indicated that, “at least in some sense, all the surveyed disciplines
could be seen as sciences and could be seen as contributing positively to peoples’ lives” (2001, p.225).
(Refer to Appendix A; for a list of the study’s disciplines).
The Result of the Marketing Sample
The marketing sample consisting of 101 professors, rated the marketing discipline to be significantly
more scientific and contribute more to society than the sample of 102 non-marketing professors. In
comparison to the non-marketing sample the marketing sample provided a significantly higher rating for
the scientificity of finance (p<.001) and personnel management (p<.001). Conversely, the marketing
sample provided a significantly lower rating for the scientificity of geology (p<.001), computer science
(p<.001), and history (p.<.005) (Katrichis and Stevens, 2001). Katrichis and Stevens suggest that this
underrating of history by marketing professors, may help to explain the lamentations of some within the
discipline regarding the shortage of historical research conducted in marketing.
A comparison of the ratings generated by the two samples presents a large amount of disagreement with
regard to contribution to peoples’ lives. The marketing sample’s rating of marketing’s contribution to
people’s lives generated a rank eleven places higher then their non-marketing peers. Marketing received a
rank of eleventh from the marketing sample, as opposed to a rank of twenty-second from the nonmarketing sample, exceeding only astronomy and astrology (Katrichis and Stevens 2001). The marketing
sample rated the contributions of computer science (p<.05), economics (p<.01), statistics (p<.10) and
astronomy (p<.10) below the non-marketing sample and the contribution of religion (p<10) higher than
the non-marketing sample. The higher rating of marketing by the marketing sample is likely to be
influenced by two factors. Firstly, marketing professors are more likely to be educated about the
contributions of the marketing discipline (Kasper, 1993; cited in Wilkie and Moore 1999). Secondly, it is
likely that they consider their home field to be important and will be more generous in the ranking of
marketing discipline (Farmer 1983).
The Results of the Non-Marketing Sample
With regard to scientific status, the non-marketing sample perceived the marketing discipline as mediocre
(ranked 22nd), indicating that marketing’s scientificity more closely resembles the twenty-fourth ranked
astrology as opposed to the top ranked physics. As to be expected, the two samples generated different
ratings on the scientific status of some disciplines. However, the results present a high level of
correlation. Katrichis and Stevens (2001) point out that with the exception of personnel management, no
discipline is ranked differently by more than two places.
274
Katrichis and Stevens' Major Conclusions and Limitations
Only a small correlation was present between a disciplines’ perceived contribution to society and its
socially designated scientific status (only five out the twenty-four disciplines presented positive
correlations between scientific status and contribution). Katrichis and Stevens (2001) suggest that these
results indicate that marketing’s quest to gain scientific status still has a long way to go; even using
Anderson’s (1983) concept of Science2. This study presents little empirical support for Anderson’s (1983)
hypothesis that Science2 is based on a disciplines perceived contribution to society, indicating a need for
further research. However, inferring that the responses used in this study represent the socially
designation of science is a limitation of the paper, as university professors do not represent society.
A result of Katrichis and Stevens' (2001) study is the suggestion that society (university professors)
awards scientific status to those disciplines typically associated with Science1. With only a small
correlation to their perceived contribution to society, especially in the case of geology and astronomy that
are perceived to have little impact on peoples’ lives.
The major conclusion of Katrichis and Stevens' (2001) study looks at the future of marketing, suggesting
that a decision needs to be made in relation to the primary goal of marketing. Is the primary goal of
marketing to produce knowledge products that are perceived to benefit society? Or if possible achieve
scientific status?
Within the limitations and conclusions of the paper, Katrichis and Stevens outline that the results of the
study need to be interpreted with care as university professors have been used as a proxy for the general
public. The reasoning behind this proxy is twofold; firstly, it is suggested that professors are likely to be
“more familiar with the distinction between sciences and non-sciences so the question would seem less
strange” (Katrichis and Stevens 2001, p.224). For this reason it can be argued that this sample is more
educated than the general public, therefore, the perceptions of university lecturers’ might be different than
that of the general public. However, the general public may hold a more extreme view, with distinct
delimitations between scientific and non-scientific disciplines. Alternatively, logical empiricism may have
permeated academic thought in that the perceptions of university lecturers maybe more extreme than the
general publics. To the extent they view the scientificity of disciplines based on the Science1 concept
because that is what is expected of them.
Secondly, Katrichis and Stevens (2001) indicated that they used university professors as a proxy as they
might be considered society’s opinion leaders with their perceptions being the leading indicators for the
rest of society. However, given the political situation within universities it is impossible to determine the
driving forces behind the responses of professors and therefore can they be related to professors acting as
opinion leaders? It does seem that the reason university professors were used, is because they provided a
convenient sample frame.
Katrichis and Stevens have implied that the responses of the university professors represent society in
stating “it appears that the rest of society (non-marketing professors) views what we are doing far
differently than we ourselves (marketing professors) view it”(2001, p.229; emphasis, and brackets added)
and “society (non-marketing professors) awards the highest scientific status to those disciplines typically
associated with Science1” (2001, p.230; emphasis and brackets added). Some of the limitations of this
study are clearly outlined; one possible improvement is changing the sample frame to the general public
as this removes the bias that is associated with using university professors as a proxy for the general
public. This study attempts to address this problem by using a sample frame consisting of members of the
Dunedin public. Although, the Dunedin public certainly does not represent western society as a whole, the
results should be more representative in comparison with the results gained from a sample of university
professors.
275
Hypotheses
Hypothesis 1: Marketing is a Science2
This hypothesis is set forth to find whether the marketing discipline is considered by members of the
public to be a Science2 or not.
Hypothesis 2: is good for society
Anderson (1983) postulates that the label Science2 is bestowed on disciplines which are perceived to
produce knowledge of benefit for society. A simple correlation of the scientificity of disciplines versus
their perceived contribution to society will indicate whether this hypothesis is supported.
Hypothesis 3: Those disciplines from the schools of science in universities will have the highest
scientificity score
This hypothesis suggests that the traditional disciplines of the hard sciences will be recognised as more
scientific by the public, i.e. physics, chemistry, etc. A proposition that follows this hypothesis is that:
P3: The sample will identify groups of disciplines in a similar way to the groupings made by
universities in placing disciplines in divisions or schools.
The Instrument
The use of questionnaires was decided upon and a modified version of Katrichis and Steven’s instrument
was employed.
The questionnaire was made up of five questions; with questions one and two taking a considerable
amount of time to complete, approximately seven minutes. Twenty-eight disciplines were included in the
study, it should be noted however, four more than the Katrichis and Stevens (2001) study.
Question 1 of the Instrument
The first question provided information on how scientific respondents considered each discipline. They
were asked to rate each discipline on a scale of 1-7, with 1 representing “not-at-all scientific” and 7
representing “completely scientific”. Included in the disciplines was astrology, a discipline generally
considered as completely unscientific.
Question 2 of the Instrument
The second question determined the positive contribution of each discipline to society. Again utilising a
1-7 scale, respondents were asked to rate the perceived positive impact of each discipline. 1 denoted
“contributes nothing to peoples lives” and 7 denoted “contributes a great deal to peoples lives”.
Question 3 and 4 of the Instrument
Questions three and four related to the demographics of the respondents.
Question 5 of the Instrument
The final question provided data on what respondents consider as contributions of marketing. Seven
contributions of marketing were given, based on Wilkie and Moore (1999), and one non-marketing
contribution. The non-marketing contribution was included so the validation of the question could be
gauged. If a large proportion of respondents wrongly identified the non-marketing contribution as a
marketing contribution, this might indicate that marketing is perceived as marketing management. Or, that
there was a degree of subjects satisficing in their answer patterns, particularly toward the end of the
survey.
276
Order of Presentation Bias
For questions 1 and 2 of the instrument, the disciplines were listed in a random order, with the original
order reversed for half the questionnaires in attempt to avoid presentation order affects.
The Sample
Those eligible for inclusion in the sample were all residents of Dunedin, a university city of
approximately 130 000 residents, including 20 000 students, in the south of New Zealand. Five hundred
and fifty questionnaires were distributed and 154 returned (28% return rate). Of those who returned the
surveys 49.3% were male and the distribution of ages of the respondents was similar to the city’s age
distribution.
RESULTS
In order to assess if the Dunedin public perceived any of the disciplines to be completely scientific or
completely non-scientific, the raw data averages were calculated, and two sample t-tests were performed.
The first of the sample t-tests showed that, at least in an absolute sense, all the listed disciplines are
somewhat scientific, as all twenty-eight disciplines were rated significantly greater than one (p<0.001),
which denoted “not at all scientific”. The second sample t-test showed that, at least in an absolute sense,
none of the twenty-eight disciplines were rated completely scientific, as each received a mean rating of
significantly less than seven (p<0.001), which denoted “completely scientific”.
The relative scientificity was analysed. In doing this, the mean ratings of the data were standardised using
the same standardisation process that Katrichis and Stevens (2001) implemented. This was required so
that the results of this study can be compared to work of Katrichis and Stevens (2001). The
standardisation process consisted of finding the mean rating of the raw data for each discipline, and then
computing the average of all the raw data means. Each disciplines’ raw data means had the average of all
the raw data means subtracted, resulting in the standardised mean for each discipline.
Table 1 ranks the surveyed disciplines using the mean rating they received in relation to how scientific the
sample perceive the disciplines. Marketing achieved the rank of 16th, from the sample of Dunedin
residents. This ranking indicates that Dunedin public perceive marketing’s scientificity to be low, as the
majority of disciplines were ranked higher than marketing. From the same list of disciplines, the Katrichis
and Stevens (2001) sample of non-marketing university professors ranked marketing in similar regard at
15th.
Katrichis and Stevens' (2001) non-marketing sample generated a smaller range in their mean ratings. The
distribution range produced by the sample of non-marketing professors’ is 2.513 as opposed to 4.269 for
the sample of Dunedin’s public. This suggests that the sample of Dunedin residents is more representative
of the general public, as it is explained within the limitations of Katrichis and Stevens (2001) study, that
the general public hold a more extreme view, with distinct delimitations between science and non-science.
The results of both studies show remarkable concurrence with each other. Although there is variance in
the order, both, the four highest and lowest ranked disciplines are the same. Furthermore, with the
exception of statistics (three places different), and education (four places different), no discipline is
ranked differently by more than two places. A possible explanation for the difference in ranking of
statistics is that university professors are more likely to implement statistical analysis. While the
difference in educations ranking might arise because University professors’ work in the education
profession, it should be expected that they hold a differing perception of educations scientificity as they
are much more involved with education.
277
Table 1: Standardised Ratings Comparing the Scientificity of Disciplines.
95% Confidence
DUNEDIN RESIDENTS
Interval
DISCIPLINE
NON-MARKETING
UNIVERSITY
PROFESSORS
Standardised
Ranking Rating
Mean LL
UL
Rating Rating
Chemistry
1
2.29
6.331 6.158
6.5
1.212
2
Physics
2
2.21
6.247 6.074
6.42
1.265
1
Biology
3
1.82
5.857 5.660
6.05
1.023
3
Astronomy
4
1.67
5.708 5.479
5.94
1.004
4
Engineering
5
1.38
5.418 5.207
5.63
0.854
7=
Geology
6
1.32
5.357 5.141
5.57
0.862
5
Computer Pro
7
1.04
5.078 4.843
5.31
0.854
7=
Nutrition
8
0.94
4.980 4.753
5.21
0.392
9
Statistics
9
0.67
4.704 4.441
4.97
0.86
6
Psychology
10
0.24
4.281 4.032
4.53
0.108
11
Education
11
-0.25
3.791 3.546
4.04
-0.497 16
Economics
12
-0.31
3.725 3.486
3.96
173
10
Finance
13
-0.41
3.625 3.362
3.89
-0.118 13
Accounting
14
-0.49
3.552 3.276
3.83
-0.059 12
Sociology
15
-0.63
3.407 3.183
3.63
-0.059 14
Marketing
16
-0.75
3.288 3.077
3.5
-0.153 15
Philosophy
17
-0.87
3.169 2.912
3.43
-0.597 18
History
18
-0.99
3.046 2.779
3.31
-0.528 17
Music
19
-1.15
2.883 2.627
3.14
-0.722 20
Personnel Mant 20
-1.25
2.792 2.567
3.02
-0.72
19
Literature
21
-1.41
2.632 2.390
2.87
-1.301 22
Astrology
22
-1.51
2.529 2.239
2.82
-1.628 24
Journalism
23
-1.6
2.438 2.232
2.64
-0.969 21
Religion
24
-1.97
2.072 1.853
2.29
-1.301 23
Source for the Non-Marketing Sample Results: Katrichis and Stevens, 2001, p.226
The results for hypothesis one found some support for marketing as being a science in absolute terms,
though with little support relative to other disciplines examined.
Disciplines' contribution to society
Question two of the instrument was used in order to assess society’s perception of marketing’s
contribution to society. The raw data averages were calculated, and t-tests were performed. The t-tests
showed that all the listed disciplines are perceived to have some positive impact to society, as all twentyeight disciplines were rated significantly greater than one (p<.001), which denoted “Contributes nothing
to peoples lives”.
The sample produced a non-standardised mean rating for marketing of 4.08; this is around the mid point
of the one to seven scale. This mean rating on its own provides little insight into how the contributions of
marketing are perceived in relation to other disciplines. To answer hypothesis one, marketing’s relative
contribution to peoples’ lives was assessed identically to relative scientificity, including the
278
standardisation of the data for the purpose of comparing the results of this study with the work of
Katrichis and Stevens (2001).
Unlike the concurrence between the two samples in relation to scientificity, Table 2 identifies large
dissimilarity between the rankings of the disciplines with respect to contribution to peoples’ lives. The
sample of Dunedin residents ranked the marketing discipline 20th, which is two positions greater than the
sample of non-marketing university professors used by Katrichis and Stevens (2001). Marketing’s
ranking of 20th suggests that Dunedin residents do not perceive marketing to contribute very positively to
society.
Table 2: Standardised Ratings of Disciplines’ Contributions to Peoples’ lives.
DISCIPLINE
DUNEDIN
RESIDENTS
Education
Nutrition
Engineering
Music
Chemistry
Biology
Literature
Computer Prog
Finance
Physics
Economics
History
Psychology
Journalism
Accounting
Geology
Sociology
Personnel Mant
Religion
Marketing
Philosophy
Statistics
Astronomy
Astrology
Ranking
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
95% Confidence
Interval
NON-MARKETING
UNIVERSITY
PROFESSORS
Standardised
Rating
1.635
1.228
1.072
0.838
0.761
0.709
0.684
0.575
0.514
0.391
0.220
-0.017
-0.156
-0.189
-0.267
-0.343
-0.362
-0.379
-0.379
-0.479
-0.494
-0.581
-0.838
-2.223
Mean
6.188
5.765
5.638
5.367
5.304
5.253
5.221
5.121
5.067
4.939
4.764
4.680
4.510
4.380
4.360
4.284
4.221
4.189
4.187
4.180
4.074
4.053
3.993
3.944
LL UL Rating
5.989 6.387 0.629
5.574 5.956 0.534
5.447 5.828 0.824
5.128 5.605 0.585
5.083 5.526 0.655
5.036 5.471 0.699
4.992 5.451 0.446
4.909 5.333 0.561
4.815 5.319 -0.228
4.695 5.183 0.348
4.514 5.013 -0.017
4.459 4.902 -0.69
4.252 4.768 -0.023
4.143 4.617 0.197
4.118 4.602 -0.328
4.043 4.525 -0.298
3.977 4.466 -0.325
3.946 4.433 -0.529
3.908 4.465 0.049
3.927 4.433 -0.554
3.810 4.337 -0.343
3.813 4.293 -0.24
3.730 4.256 -0.592
3.698 4.189 -2.033
Ranking
4
7
1
5
3
2
8
6
15
9
12
13
14
10
19
17
18
21
11
22
20
16
23
24
H2: There is a correlation between a disciplines contribution to society and its socially designated
scientific status.
279
Two methods of analysis were used to determine whether a relationship exists between ratings of
scientificity and contribution to people’s lives. Firstly, paired sample correlations were conducted with
the results presented in Table 3.
Table 3: Paired Samples Correlations of Scientificity and Contributions
DUNEDIN SAMPLE NON_MARKETING
UNIVERSITY PROFESSORS
Discipline
Physics
Sales
Chemistry
Biology
Astronomy
Marketing
Engineering
Statistics
Geology
Computer Programming
Advertising
Economics
Nutrition
Finance
Psychology
Logistics
Accounting
Sociology
Personnel Management
Education
History
Philosophy
Music
Journalism
New Product Development
Literature
Religion
Astrology
Average Correlation
Correlation
0.242
0.176
0.142
0.173
0.258
0.214
0.236
0.084
0.193
0.203
0.256
0.151
0.275
0.146
0.347
0.315
0.270
0.355
0.281
0.263
0.136
0.313
0.317
0.288
0.342
0.041
0.361
0.405
0.242
Sig. <
.050
.050
.100
.050
.050
.050
.050
n.s
.050
.050
.050
.100
.001
.100
.001
.001
.001
.001
.001
.001
n.s
.001
.001
.001
.001
n.s
.001
.001
Correlation
-0.038
N/A
0.26
0.315
0.238
0.218
0.146
0.008
0.338
0.199
N/A
0.305
0.338
0.277
0.386
N/A
0.308
0.247
0.163
0.432
0.12
0.251
0.053
-0.004
N/A
-0.063
-0.004
0.317
0.200
Sig.<
n.s
N/A
.010
.001
.050
.050
n.s
n.s
.001
.050
N/A
.100
.001
.010
.001
N/A
.010
.050
.100
.001
n.s
.010
n.s
n.s
N/A
n.s
n.s
.001
The paired correlations for the sample of Dunedin residents showed for all but three of the disciplines
(statistics, history and literature), with at least a 90 percent level of confidence that there is a small
positive correlation between the disciplines' scientificity and its perceived contribution. Therefore, there
is evidence to reject the null hypothesis.
The sample of Dunedin residents provided a higher average correlation when compared to Katrichis and
Stevens' (2001) sample of non-marketing university professors. This provides some indication that the
Dunedin residents provide stronger support for the concept of Science2. Comparing the two samples
280
based on the average correlation is somewhat limited. However, because the raw data is not available for
the sample of non-marketing university professors no statistical test is possible.
Figure One: Dunedin Sample Contribution versus Scientificity
Figure Two: Katrichis and Stevens' non marketing sample Contribution versus Scientificity
Source: Katrichis and Stevens, 2001, 226-228.
281
Figures One and Two graphically represent the relationship between scientificity and contribution to
peoples’ lives of the two samples under study. The figures indicate that mainly those disciplines
accredited with Science1 are also accredited with Science2 indicating that there might not be a difference
between the concepts. The top right hand quadrant of both figures is of particular importance, as this
quadrant scored highest on both, the question of scientificity and contributions to people’s lives (these
disciplines are considered as Sceince2) However, it does appear that for both samples most of the
disciplines in this top right quadrant are also Science1 indicating that for both samples logical empiricism
is the perceived view of science.
What is marketings' contribution to society?
Question 5 of the instrument was used to gain data for answering the research problem; what does society
consider marketings' contribution to be?
The first part of this question asked respondents if they perceived branding and trademarks as a
contribution of marketing. Table 4 presents that the majority of respondents (87%) considered branding
and trademarks to be a contribution of marketing.
The second part of this question asked respondents if they perceived advertising and promotion to be a
contribution of marketing. Table 4 presents that the majority of respondents (90%) considered advertising
and promotion to be a contribution of marketing.
The third part this question used the term logistics and this might have been somewhat ambiguous, which
might account for why only 64 percent of respondents perceived distribution and logistics to be a
contribution of marketing. However, it is also likely that this function of marketing is less likely to be
considered a contribution of marketing as consumers will be less educated about it (Kasper, 1993; cited in
Wilkie and Moore 1999). Table 4 illustrates the findings generated by this part of question five.
The fourth part of this question asked respondents if they perceived increased competition to be a
contribution of marketing. Table 4 shows that the majority of respondents (84%) considered increased
competition to be a contribution of marketing.
The fifth part of this question asked respondents if they perceived financial management to be a
contribution of marketing. Table 4 shows that only 54 percent of respondents considered financial
management to be a contribution of marketing. Wilkie and Moore (1999) do not advocate financial
management to be a contribution of marketing. This part the question was asked so the validity of the
results could be gauged. A higher than expected proportion of respondents answered yes to this question,
indicating that the possible leading nature of this question has influenced the responses. However, this
question received the lowest support, which was expected. Therefore, it does appear that question five
passes the test for face validity. It should be noted that a possible explanation for the higher than expected
proportion of respondents answering yes, is that marketing is viewed as marketing management, which
contains a financial management function.
The sixth part of this question asked respondents if they perceived post purchase support to be a
contribution of marketing. Table 4 shows that 67 percent of respondents considered posted purchase
support as a contribution of marketing.
The seventh part of this question asked respondents if they perceived pleasant environments for buyers to
be a contribution of marketing. Table 4 shows that 79 percent of respondents considered pleasant
environment for buyers as a contribution of marketing.
282
The last part of this question asked respondents if they perceived reduced buyer time requirements to be a
contribution of marketing. Table 4 shows that only 57 percent of respondents considered reduced buyer
time requirements to a contribution of marketing.
Table 4: What is Marketings' contribution?
Is this a Contribution of Marketing
Branding and Trademarks
Advertising and Promotion
Distribution and Logistics
Increased Competition
Financial Management
Post-Purchase Support
Pleasant Environment for Buyers
Reduced Buyer Time Requirements
Yes%
86.90
90.34
63.50
83.67
55.94
67.36
78.62
57.25
No%
13.10
9.66
36.50
16.33
44.06
32.64
21.38
42.75
Contrary to the belief of some authors, (Farmer, 1967, 1977, 1987) it does appear that a large proportion
of Dunedin’s public are aware of the contributions of marketing, with the possible exception of reduced
buyer time requirements. However, what this survey question did not explore is the nature of these
contributions. For example, advertising is considered a contribution of marketing, yet in aggregate it
might be perceived to contribute negativity to society.
Research Proposition 3
P3:Dunedin public will group disciplines based on how The University of Otago groups them, which is
by divisions.
Question one asked respondents to rate how scientific they perceived each of twenty-eight disciplines.
Table 5, the results of a factor analysis of those responses, illustrates that although there are discrepancies,
it does appears that the underlying factor influencing the respondents’ ratings for scientificity is that of the
division it is grouped in by The University of Otago.
Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.
Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.
Rotation converged in 27 iterations.
Other Interesting Findings
In order to determine if gender influenced how respondents answered the questions, an independent
samples t-test was conducted; with only one impact relating to gender being present. With a 99.9 percent
level of confidence, there is a difference between how males and females rated astrology’s positive
contribution to society. Females rated astrology’s positive contribution significantly higher than males.
CONCLUSION
Marketing can be seen to be relatively not scientific according to the sample of Dunedin residents,
however in absolute terms it is somewhat scientific. There is some evidence that the public use divisions,
or schools, as indicators of the degree to which a discipline is scientific.
Certainly there is considerable work to be done marketing the benefits of marketing to society if we are to
believe the benefits attributed to marketing by Wilkie and Moore (1999) are those that the public should
associate us, as marketers, with.
283
Table 5: Factor Analysis of Instrument Question One: Scientificity
Rotated Component Matrix
Component
1
2
3
4
5
6
Division
How scientific is Finance
0.817
0.150
0.182
0.181
0.072
0.000
Commerce
How scientific is Accounting
0.795
0.113
0.215
0.297
0.028
0.025
Commerce
How scientific is Logistics
0.763
0.197
0.161
0.145
0.069
0.057
Commerce
0.671
0.422
-0.067 0.036
-0.124 0.036
Science/
Commerce
How scientific is Economics
0.669
0.110
0.214
0.369
0.032
Commerce
How scientific is Engineering
0.495
0.499
0.257
0.066
-0.228 0.175
Applied Science
How scientific is Computer
Programming
0.490
0.413
-0.046 0.238
-0.049 0.410
Commerce/
Science
How scientific is Chemistry
0.091
0.882
-0.106 0.044
0.157
-0.050 Science
How scientific is Biology
0.020
0.882
-0.083 0.077
0.020
0.017
Science
How scientific is Physics
0.178
0.828
-0.086 -0.043 -0.210 0.004
Science
How scientific is Astronomy
0.128
0.753
-0.039 0.077
Science
How scientific is Geology
0.418
0.655
0.135
-0.111 0.065
-0.036 Science
How scientific is Nutrition
0.325
0.430
0.217
0.157
0.247
0.094
0.241
-0.050 0.824
0.014
0.106
Humanities/
-0.139 Arts
How scientific is Education
0.296
0.056
0.713
0.242
0.088
0.145
Arts
How scientific is Journalism
0.194
-0.088 0.690
0.301
0.327
0.053
Arts
How scientific is Literature
0.059
-0.130 0.679
0.132
0.386
0.259
Arts
How scientific is Religion
-0.268 0.022
0.444
0.240
0.542
-0.055 Arts
How scientific is Marketing
0.173
0.087
0.086
0.844
0.051
0.264
How scientific is Sales
0.167
0.087
0.089
0.820
0.061
-0.061 Commerce
How scientific is Advertising
0.272
-0.057 0.230
0.766
0.208
0.012
How scientific is Personnel
Management
0.450
0.026
0.331
0.494
0.153
0.049
How scientific is Sociology
0.395
0.067
0.267
0.412
0.366
-0.011 Social Science
How scientific is Philosophy
0.031
-0.012 0.271
0.053
0.719
0.125
Humanities
How scientific is Psychology
0.356
0.140
-0.096 0.120
0.687
0.057
Social Science
How scientific is Music
0.104
-0.084 0.327
0.108
0.617
-0.021 Arts
How scientific is Astrology
-0.239 -0.221 0.063
0.019
0.592
-0.032 Arts
How scientific is New Product
Development
0.097
0.084
0.085
0.912
How scienfitic is Statistics
0.082
How scientific is History
284
0.120
0.061
Applied Science
Commerce
Commerce
Commerce
N/A
0.017
0.099
REFERENCES
Alderson, W. and R. Cox, 1948. Towards a theory of marketing. Journal of Marketing 13 (October): 137152.
Anderson, P.F. (1983), Marketing, Scientific Progress, and Scientific Method. Journal of Marketing 47
(Fall): 18-31.
Arndt, J. 1982. The Conceptual Domain of Marketing: Evaluation of Shelby Hunt’s Three Dichotomies
Model. European Journal of Marketing 16 (1): 27-36.
Bagozzi, R.P. 1975. Marketing as Exchange. Journal of Marketin 39: 32-39.
Bagozzi, R.P. 1984. A Prospectus for Theory Construction in Marketing. Journal of Marketing 48: 11-29.
Bartels, R. 1968. The General Theory of Marketing. Journal of Marketing 32: 29-33.
Buzzell, R.D. 1963. Is Marketing a Science?. Harvard Business Review 41 (January/February): 32-40.
Farmer, R.N. 1967. Would You Want Your Daughter to Marry a Marketing Man?. Journal of Marketing
31: 1-3.
Farmer, R.N. 1977. Would You Want Your Son to Marry a Marketing Lady?. Journal of Marketing 41:
15-18.
Farmer, R.N. 1987. Would You Want Your Granddaughter to Marry a Taiwanese Marketing Man?.
Journal of Marketing 51 (October): 111-116.
Feyerabend, P. 1981. More Clothes From the Emperor’s Bargain Basement. British Journal for the
Philosophy of Science 32: 57-94.
Gummeson, E. 2001. Are current research approaches in marketing leading us astray?Marketing Theory
1(1): 1-27.
Hosley, S. and C.H. Wee, 1988. Marketing and Economic Development. Journal of Macromarketing.8
(1): 43-53.
Hunt, S.D. (1983). General Theories and the Fundamental Explananda of Marketing. Journal of
Marketing 47: 9-17.
Hunt, S.D. 1991. Modern Marketing Theory: Critical Issues in the Philosophy of Marketing Scienc. Ohio:
South-Western Publishing Co.
Hutchinson, K.D. 1952. Marketing as a Science: An Appraisal. Journal of Marketing 16: 286-293.
Katrichis, J.M. and K.M. Stevens, 2001. Marketing and the Social Designation of Science. Proceedings of
the 26th Annual Macromarketing Conference 222-235.
Kuhn, T. 1972. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions (2nd Ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lambe, C.J., R.E. Spekman, and S.D. Hunt, 2000, Interimistic Relational Exchange: Conceptualization
and Propositional Development. Academy of Marketing Science Journal 28 (2): 212-225.
Popper. K. 1962. Conjectures and Refutations. New York: Harper.
Sheth, J.N, D.M Gardner, and D.E Garrett, 1998. Marketing Theory: Evolution and Evaluation. Canada:
John Wily and Sons Inc.
Vaile, R.S. 1949. Towards a Theory of Marketing, Comment. Journal of Marketing 13: 520-22.
Wilkie, W.L, and E.S. Moore, 1999. Marketing’s Contributions to Society. Journal of Marketing. 63: 198218.
285
Future of Marketing
Session Organizer & Moderator
Roger Dickinson, University of Texas at Arlington
Panel
Robert W, Nason, Michigan State University
John O’Shaughnessy, Columbia University
Clifford J. Shultz II, Arizona State University
William L. Wilkie, University of Notre Dame
For Perspective:
Pre2005 AMA definition of Marketing
“Marketing is the process of planning and executing conception,
pricing, promotion and distribution of goods, ideas and services to
create exchanges that satisfy individual and organizational goals."
2005 AMA definition of Marketing
“Marketing is an organizational function and a set of processes
for creating, communicating and delivering value to customers
and for managing customer relationships in ways that benefit the
organization and its stakeholders.”
286
__________________________________________________
Social Capital and Co-production in the Marketplace
Susie Pryor, University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Sanford Grossbart, University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Abstract
This paper uses a social capital (SC) framework to theorize about co-production by marketers and
consumers and the socio-commercial nature of the marketplace in a downtown retail area (DRA).
Specifically, we adopt Portes’ conceptualization of SC as referring to actors’ abilities to secure benefits
via membership in networks and other social entities. This ability emanates from the consummatory and
instrumental motives of actors who provide or facilitate these benefits (Portes1998). In the marketing
field, co-production is typically depicted in terms of situations in which a customer and a firm’s
employees interact and participate in joint production of goods and services (Benapudi and Leone 2003;
Bowen 1986; Chase 1988; Gersuny and Rosengren 1973; Lengnick Hall 1996; Meuter and Bitner 1998;
Vargo and Lusch 2004) or consumers or a marketer and consumers jointly produce marketing activities,
functions, and meanings (cf., Muniz and O’Guinn 2001; Penaloza 2000; Schouten and McAlexander
1995).
Introduction
Our focus is on co-production among marketers and consumers that is related to SC (i.e., social capitalrelated co-production or SCRC). Expectations about SC differ from those regarding a dyadic exchange
that is not embedded in a larger social structure (Portes 1998). Likewise, SCRC differs from coproduction that is based on personal gain or oriented to another person but is not linked to a larger social
entity. Thus, our interest in SCRC extends beyond a managerial concern with self-service or a consumer’s
joint involvement with a marketer to create goods or services for her/himself.
We argue that SCRC encompasses a wider set of marketing activities and levels of the marketplace than
is described in prior work; creates outcomes for other marketers and consumers; and affects the
marketplace’s meanings, offerings, and uses (e.g., as a political, social, cultural, and commercial space).
In particular, our interest is in marketers’ and consumers’ SCRC of retailing activities and functions
(customer service, display, entertainment, etc.; Levy and Weitz 2004) and less commercially-focused
activities (e.g., parades and holiday celebrations). This SCRC may be for one or more stores in parts or all
of the DRA. We use Portes’ (1998) conception of SC to theorize about SCRC and its proximal benefits
and more general positive and negative consequences.
Portes’ (1998) theorizing about SC is an extension of earlier work on SC and economic behavior in
immigrant communities (Portes and Sensenbrenner 1993). His conception of SC overlaps with that of
other scholars, including Coleman (1980), Putnam (2000), and Fukuyama (1999), but Portes’ views differ
in breadth, focus, and clarity. For example, Portes notes that Coleman’s definition of SC is vague (1998,
p. 5). Coleman defines SC by its function as a variety of entities with two common elements, which
consist of some aspect of social structures that facilitates certain individual or corporate actions of actors
within the structure (p. 98). He argues that social relations form capital resources for individuals via
processes such as establishing obligations; expectations and trustworthiness; channels for information
dissemination; and norms and sanctions (pp. 103-4). Coleman notes that three forms of SC can be
287
valuable resources: trustworthiness that underlies a system of reciprocity; information that reduces search
time and costs and allows actors to take action; and norms and sanctions that guide behavior and insure
observance of norms. Similarly, Putnam and Fukuyama’s definitions lack the specificity necessary to
theorize about SCRC in the marketplace. Putnam defines social capital as stocks of social trust, norms and
networks that people can draw upon to solve common problems, and Fukuyama as stocks of social
values. In contrast, Portes stresses the distinction between the resources related to SC and the ability to
obtain them via membership in different social structures. He argues that equating SC with the resources
obtained via SC can lead to tautological reasoning. Consequently, we use Portes and Sensenbrenner’s
(1993) and Portes’ (1998) more comprehensive conceptions to theorize about SCRC and develop this
paper’s research propositions (P1-P5).
We use a SC framework because its relevance in the marketplace is indirectly reflected by reports on its
role in other facets of community life (Flora and Flora 1993; Putnam 1993; Woolcock 1998). Insights
about SC also help reconcile differences between narrow and broad views of motives and relationships in
the marketplace. Narrow views describe agents who seek self-interest via individual actions (transactions)
in a social organization (market) with an exchange nexus (Lichbach and Seligmann 2000). In contrast,
broad views stress how individuals’ desires, institutions, and collective action in the marketplace are
related to the ties among actors, negotiation of acceptable demands in social life, and consciousness of the
larger social aggregate’s outlook (Douglas and Ney 1998). Moreover, SC sheds light on how marketplace
meanings and activities may be embedded in larger social structures. SC is an aspect of social
embeddedness i.e., the contextualization of economic activity in on-going patterns of social relations that
affect interactions among individuals and organizations in the marketplace (Coleman 1988; Polanyi
1944).
These matters raise issues about tensions between market logic and communal relations. Our views
contrast with the idea that markets homogenize, suppress, and operate on or against consumers (Kozinets
2002). We suggest that consumers and marketers may jointly affect a marketplace’s functioning and
meaning and both reduce and heighten market-community tensions. Our discussion is structured in terms
of components of the SC framework (sources, benefits, and consequences) and related propositions.
Sources and Benefits of SCRC
Portes (1998) suggests that SC’s sources lie in consummatory and instrumental motives (or expectations)
that may be appropriated by members of a network or collectivity. Consummatory motives concern the
expression and maintenance of basic values, commitments, and orientations that guide the affairs of a
network or collectivity. Such motives may lead individuals to help others due to value introjection (e.g.,
norms or obligations) or a sense of bounded solidarity and identification (e.g., their actual or perceived
shared membership or awareness of their common situation or fate). Value introjection operates via
socialization into consensually established beliefs. Bounded solidarity is increased by factors related to
members’ sense of external opposition (distinctiveness of traits that distinguish them from the general
population; amount of prejudice associated with their traits; and likelihood of exit from the situation) and
social construction (members’ collective identity and common memory; Portes and Sensenbrenner 1993).
Instrumental motives concern means-ends relationships, as reflected in expected reciprocity and
enforceable trust. Individuals may help others due to expected reciprocity, albeit not necessarily in the
same form or from the person they aid, e.g., they may help others to gain group approval. Enforceable
trust involves the confidence that repayment is insured by the sanctioning capacity of the network or
collectivity. It operates in terms of particularistic sanctions and rewards that are linked to group
memberships and closure. It is positively related to outside discrimination and negatively related to
options outside the group for securing social honor and economic opportunity. It is also enhanced by the
collectivity’s ability to confer unique awards on its members, monitor their behaviors, and publicize the
identities of deviants (Portes and Sensenbrenner 1993).
288
Due to consummatory and instrumental motives, SC functions as a non-monetary form of capital.
Scholars note SC’ s capital-like qualities. They suggest that it may be appropriable by the actors involved
(Coleman 1988); convertible to other advantages (e.g., money and favors; Bourdieu 1985); and a
complement or substitute for other assets (e.g., compensate for lack of funds via social connections or
lower transaction costs; Anheier, Gerhands, and Romo 1985). They also observe that SC may be
increased via use, diminished by non-maintenance and contextual changes (cf., Sandefur and Laumann
1998), and endowed to new parties or withheld from excluded parties (Hechter 1987).
Implications for motives for SCRC. The prior considerations and research in marketing support the
expectation the marketers and consumers have diverse consummatory and instrumental motives for many
types of SCRC in a DRA. This supposition is indirectly supported by evidence that normative
commitment, which is associated with moral obligation toward the group and professional duty, is
positively related to co-production in a life insurance sales agents’ organization (Gruen, Summers, and
Acito 2000). Similarly, some consumers may be jointly involved with marketers or other consumers in
DRA activities due to value introjection, e.g., they may express their patriotic, religious, political, or
ethical values in holiday events, parades, protests, etc.
External and internal factors are also apt to affect marketers’ and consumers’ feelings of in-group or
bounded solidarity and identification. For instance, DRA merchants and shoppers may feel that they are
subject to prejudice because they have social characteristics that distinguish them from other marketers
and consumers in the population. This expectation is consistent with evidence of the cohesive effects and
joint activity among brand community members due to derision and ridicule (Kozinets 2001).
A sense of identification due to recognition of common circumstances and common opposition also
seems to exist in other contexts in which co-production exists. For example, SCRC due to bounded
solidarity appears to occur among consumers: who sense their shared opposition to marketplace forces
(Kozinets 2002); contest a marketer (Muniz and O’Guinn 2001); are in brand communities that coalesce
due to competition between brands (O’Guinn and Muniz 2000); are brand devotees who work together
due to necessity, have a common enemy or cause, and face similar challenges and threats; deal with
derision, ridicule, and strong rivalry; and share history and marketplace abandonment (Muniz and Schau
unpublished). Likewise, in DRA contexts, awareness of common circumstances and opposition are apt to
arise due to threats from the openings of mass merchandisers, chain stores, and other retail complexes in
the city.
In addition, marketers’ and consumers’ direct or vicarious participation in DRA activities and events may
elevate their feelings of collective identity and common memory. This likelihood is indirectly supported
by evidence of participation in and memories of community activities in other consumer contexts
(Kozinets 2002; Schouten and McAlexander 1995). For similar reasons, consummatory motives
associated with bounded solidarity may underlie marketers’ efforts to help or facilitate help for other
DRA marketers. In other words, they may engage in SCRC with other marketers due their sense of
identification with business people in the area. This expectation is indirectly supported by indications that
affective commitment, which is focused on positive emotional attachment to a professional marketers’
organization, is positively related to members’ involvement in co-production activities in the group
(Gruen, Summers, and Acito 2000). Likewise, some consumers may engage in joint efforts with
marketers or other consumers due to their identification with the DRA as a community symbol or sense of
connection with others who patronize area stores (Grossbart and Pryor 2002).
Except for suggestions that consumers and individual marketers jointly build brand communities
(McAlexander, Schouten, and Koenig 2002), there are few reports on co-production among consumers
and marketers due to bounded solidarity. However, there is evidence of bonding, communitas, and co-
289
production among consumers and marketers in a temporary river rafting setting (Arnould and Price 1993).
Indirect evidence of apparent bounded solidarity among consumers includes reports on how identification
and bonding are rooted in common consumer experiences; sense of connection to other brand owners;
consciousness of kind, responsibility to other members, and co-production in brand communities; and
socially cohesive elements in subcultures of consumption (McAlexander, Schouten, and Koenig 2002;
Muniz and O’Guinn 2001; Muniz and Schau, unpublished; Schouten and McAlexander 1995). SCRC
among consumers and marketers based on bounded solidarity is also likely when distinctions between
marketers’ representatives and consumers become blurred at events for brand communities, consumption
subcultures, and other subcultures (Kates 2002, 2004).
Instrumental motives are also apt to underlie SCRC in a DRA. For instance, motives linked to reciprocity
should lead marketers to co-produce marketing activities with other marketers because they expect to
receive recognition or unspecified future benefits, i.e., generalized reciprocity, from other marketers
(Gruen, Summers, and Acito 2000) or from consumers. Consumers may also engage in SCRC with
marketers because they expect to get recognition or favored treatment, e.g., respect, awards, and status
privileges from retailers or other DRA patrons. Similarly, consumers may co-produce marketing activities
with other consumers in the expectation of receiving recognition from those involved or other marketers
or consumers.
External and internal factors are also apt to elevate SCRC related to enforceable trust among marketers
and consumers. In terms of external factors, this SCRC may be elevated by the possible outside
discrimination faced by DRA marketers and patrons and a lack of options for obtaining social honors and
economic opportunities outside the DRA. In terms of internal factors, enforceable trust and SC among
marketers and consumers are likely to be enhanced by their abilities to confer awards, monitor behaviors,
and publicize the identities of deviants. These abilities should be more evident among marketers than
consumers, because marketers are fewer in number, more organized (e.g., in professional groups), and
have a more continuous presence in the DRA. The prior logic is consistent with the following expectation
about motives for SCRC.
P1: Marketers and consumers have consummatory and instrumental motives for SCRC
in the marketplace.
Implications for SCRC on multiple levels. An added implication of the prior reasoning is that SCRC is
apt to occur at multiple levels. On the micro-level, SCRC may involve a relatively few marketers and/ or
consumers and one or two stores. (Retailer A, who was recommended by retailer C, may help retailer B
find a vendor for store fixtures; a customer and her friend may help a retailer decorate her/ his store; or a
retailer A and a consumer may help a retailer B unload merchandise.) On a more meso-level, SCRC may
involve a larger set of marketers and/ or consumers and set of stores. (After a fire, a retailer may be
helped by other retailers who are also located on a side street; or consumers and bar owners may
cooperate to create and promote a charity event.) On the macro-level, SCRC may involve a larger and
more diffuse collectivity of marketers and/ or consumers and most or all DRA area and its stores. (All
DRA retailers and many consumers may stage a DRA Halloween event.)
This paper envisions marketplace relationships as forms of social organization that involve the interplay
of individual and collective-level factors. Through this interplay, marketers and consumers affect and are
affected by outcomes that foster and constrain their individual and collective views and behaviors
(Lichbach and Seligmann 2000). A SC perspective offers a useful basis for investigating this interplay, in
terms of the pattern of relationships and resource linkages among actors (marketers and consumers); the
social mechanisms that sustain and regulate their actual and imagined relationships (e.g., expressive and
instrumental motives, norms, and sanctions); and the socio-cultural milieu in which they exist (Lin 2001).
290
Moreover, SC has research value because it links micro- and more general-levels of social analysis, by
shifting the focus from the behavior of individual agents to the pattern of relations among agents, social
units, and institutions (Flora and Flora 1993; Schuller, Baron, and Field 2000, p. 35). The multi-level
(individual and collective) factors and different motives (instrumental and consummatory) in the SCRC
framework provide useful bases for examining possible diversity and similarity in marketplace views and
relationships. Investigating these matters helps us address tensions about the roles of agency and
collectivity in narrower and broader views on social order and marketplace relationships. This logic leads
to the following proposition.
P2: SCRC by marketers and consumers in the marketplace occurs on micro-, meso-, and
macro-levels.
Implications for benefits from SCRC on multiple levels. Portes (1998) and others highlight specific
benefits of SC (e.g., sympathetic treatment, information, moral and material support, brokering, and
allocative and adaptive efficiencies; Burt 1992, 1997; Lin 2001; Nahapiet and Goshal 1998). The varied
benefits of SC in other contexts indirectly support the idea that marketers and consumers may be jointly
involved in a wide set of marketing and other activities due to SCRC. The benefits may include shared
information and help with retailing functions at multiple levels ranging from a store to the whole DRA.
As implied by the prior examples, there should be benefits on multiple levels.
P3: SCRC by marketers and consumers in the marketplace creates benefits on micro-, meso-,
and macro-levels.
Consequences of SCRC
Portes (1998) and other researchers note that SC may have other general positive and negative
consequences (Bourdieu 1985; Putnam 1993). Norm observance or social control often is listed as a
positive consequence of SC. It is reflected in varied ways, including rule enforcement among actors who
try to maintain discipline and foster compliance. It lessens the need for formal or overt controls. It is
often rooted in bounded solidarity and enforceable trust. However, Portes notes that SC’s effects may not
be complementary and may be in conflict. For instance, social controls may conflict with benefits from
SC that stem from the ability to evade existing norms. Thus, a larger collectivity’s ability to enforce rules
may be undermined by networks that facilitate evasion of rules for private benefit. Portes suggests that
this potential inconsistency underscores the need to avoid moralizing statements about the virtues of SC,
social relationships, social controls, and collective sanctions. Instead, we should consider how actors may
gain from or be disadvantaged by SC’s less desirable consequences.
He highlights four negative consequences that occur because the factors that create appropriable SC may
also constrain or direct individuals’ actions away from positive consequences (Portes and Sensenbrenner
1993). First, outsiders may be excluded if strong ties, bounded solidarity, and trust that benefit members
also restrict outsiders from access to economic benefits. Second, more affluent members’ initiatives may
be constrained by excessive claims by others who free ride on bonds and norms for mutual assistance.
Third, the social controls and demands for conformity may restrict individual freedoms and outside
contacts and encourage young and independent-oriented persons to leave. Fourth, downward leveling
norms maybe discourage individual success, because it undermines cohesion, particularly in groups in
which solidarity is based on a sense of common adversity and opposition to the mainstream or external
society.
Implications for consequences on multiple levels. As in other contexts, there should be positive and
negative consequences of SCRC in a DRA. Consistent with our prior reasoning, these outcomes should
occur on micro, meso-, and macro-levels. For example, in positive terms, retailers may have less need to
monitor their peers or rely on formal controls and contracts due to the social control and compliance that
291
results from SCRC in the DRA. Consumers may also save the time and costs that might otherwise be
given to checking the adequacy of DRA marketers’ offerings and prices. SCRC may also create public
goods (e.g., when it produces parades, parking, and DRA beautification).
The possible meaning(s) of the DRA to marketers and consumers may be an added consequence of these
elements. For example, beyond its role in fulfilling economic and consumption related functions, the
DRA may also be seen as a multi-faceted locus of political, social, and cultural activities and meanings,
and as source of civic pride for many marketers and consumers. Alternatively, such possible social,
cultural, and civic activities and meanings may have negative connotations for some marketers and
consumers. (A proposition about marketplace meanings is in the next section.)
There also may be other negative consequences. For instance, the norms, bonds, and expectations that
underlie SCRC may act as barriers to new stores and, thereby, disadvantage some consumers or some
marketers who might benefit from retail agglomeration effects that result from more stores offering goods
in the same product categories. If similar factors lead marketers to favor local shoppers, nonresident
consumers who shop in the DRA may also be disadvantaged. Successful merchants may be hampered by
excessive requests for favors and support for charities. Retailers’ employees and consumers may face
sanctions if they patronize non-DRA stores. Retailers may also restrict their mixes of goods and services
to avoid harm to other retailers in the DRA.
The prior examples illustrate possible positive and negative consequences of SCRC, including tensions
between consumers, between marketers, and between consumers and marketers. Although it is not
possible to predict specific consequences, these examples suggest that outcomes are likely to occur at
different levels of aggregation.
P4: SCRC by marketers and consumers in the marketplace has positive and negative
consequences on micro-, meso-, and macro-levels.
Implications for marketplace meanings. The prior discussion includes examples of how marketplace
meanings may both shape and emerge from SCRC among marketers and consumers. The likelihood that
these meanings may be social, cultural, economic, political, and consumption-related is consistent with
views that suggest that: the creation of meaning is negotiated through the symbolism of the marketplace
and is a basis for identification for those involved (McAlexander, Schouten and Koenig 2002);
postmodern consumers are active links in the continual production and reproduction of images and
symbolic meanings (Firat, Dholakia, and Venkatesh 1995); and socio-cultural factors are central, not
peripheral, features of market processes (Granovetter 1985).
Marketers and consumers may not agree about some meanings or they may agree about some meanings
but disagree about their valences (e.g., they may see a DRA as a site for night entertainment and
socializing but disagree about its desirability). More generally, identical interpretations are unlikely,
because: the market reflects tensions within and between the meanings and values of marketers and
consumers (Penaloza 2000); monolithic meanings may not entirely dominate or preclude the individual
expression and distinction in the marketplace (Kates 2002); and different actors in the marketplace may
have different views because, like a consumption object, the marketplace has polysemic symbolic
qualities that enable variation in interpretation and use (Holt 1997).
Thus, SCRC is likely to produce both shared and contested meanings about the marketplace. Yet, the
existence of varied meanings does not preclude tolerance or reconciliation of differences. Research
suggests that SC can lead to depersonalization of issues and concentration on common goals and interests
rather than divisiveness (Flora and Flora 1993). Moreover, marketplace meanings (like brand meanings)
292
may be accepted, institutionalized to a certain extent, and sufficiently ambiguous or malleable to assume
socially acceptable variations (Kates 2004).
P5: SCRC by marketers and consumers contributes to shared and contested economic, social,
political, cultural, and consumption-related meanings about the marketplace.
REFERENCES
Anheier, H. K., J. Gerhards, and F. P. Romo. 1995. Forms of capital and social structure in Cultural
Fields: Examining Bourdieu’s Social Topography, American Journal of Sociology, 100: 859-903.
Arnould, Eric J. and Linda L. Price. 1993. River magic: Extraordinary experience and the
extended service encounter, Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (June): 24-45.
Benapudi, Neeli and Robert P. Leone. 2003. Psychological implications of customer
participation in co-production, Journal of Marketing, 67 (January): 14-28.
Bourdieu, Pierre 1985. The forms of capital. In Handbook of Theory and Research for the
Sociology of Education, edited by J. G. Richardson, 241-58. New York: Greenwood, 241-58.
Bowen, David E. 1986. Managing customers as human resources in service organizations,
Human Resource Management, 25 (3): 371-383.
Burt, Ronald S. 1992. Structural holes: The social structure of competition, Cambridge, Mass.:
Harvard University Press.
----------- 1997. The contingent value of social capital, Administrative Science Quarterly, June:
339-365.
Chase, Richard B. 1988. The service vactory, Academy of Management Executive, 2 (3): 191-196.
Coleman, James S. 1988. Social capital in the creation of human capital, American Journal of
Sociology, 94: S95-121.
----------- 1990. Foundations of social theory, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Douglas, Mary and Steven Ney. 1998. Missing persons: A critique of personhood in the social
sciences. Berkley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Firat, A. Fuat, Nikhilesh Dholakia, and Alladi Venkatesh. 1995. Marketing in a postmodern
world, European Journal of Marketing, 29 (1): 40-56.
Flora, Cornelia Butler and Jan L. Flora. 1993. Entrepreneurial social infrastructure: A necessary
ingredient, The Annals of the American Academy of Political & Social Science, 529: 48-58.
Fukuyama, Francis. 1999. The great disruption, The Atlantic Monthly. 283: 55-80.
Gersuny, C and W. R. Rosengren. 1973. The service society. Cambridge, MA: Schenkman Press.
293
Granovetter, Mark. 1985. Economic action and social structure: The problem of embeddedness,
American Journal of Sociology, 91 (3): 481-510.
Grossbart, Sanford and Susie Pryor. 2002. Communal influences and social capital among
retailers and consumers,” in Proceedings of the Annual Macromarketing Conference,
Macromarketing in the Asian Pacific Century, 220-225, University of New South Wales
Sydney, Australia.
Gruen, Thomas W., John O. Summers, and Frank Acito. 2000. Relationship marketing activities,
commitment, and membership behaviors in professional associations, Journal of Marketing,
64: 34-49.
Hechter, Michael. 1987. Principles of group solidarity, Berkeley: University of California Press.
Holt, Douglas B. 1997. Poststructuralist lifestyle analysis: Conceptualizing the social patterning of
consumption in postmodernity, Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (4): 326-350.
Kates, Steven M. 2004. The dynamics of brand legitimacy: An interpretive study in the gay
men’s community. Journal of Consumer Research 31 (September): 455-464.
----------- 2002. The protean quality of subcultural consumption: An ethnographic account of gay
consumers. Journal of Consumer Research 29:3 (December): 383-399.
Kozinets, Robert V. 2001. Utopian enterprise: Articulating the meanings of Star Trek’s culture of
consumption. Journal of Consumer Research 28 (June): 67-88.
----------- 2002. Can consumers escape the market? Emancipatory illuminations from burning
man. Journal of Consumer Research 29: 20-38.
Lengnick-Hall, Cynthia A. 1996. Customer contributions to quality: A different view of the
customer-oriented firm. The Academy of Management Review, 21 (3): 791-824.
Levy, Michael and Barton Weitz. 2004. Retailing management. 5th edition. Boston: Irwin
McGraw-Hill.
Lichbach, Mark I. and Adam B. Seligmann. 2000. Market and community. University Park, PA:
The Pennsylvania State University Press.
Lin, Nan 2001. Social capital: A theory of social structure and action. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
McAlexander, James H., John W. Schouten, and Harold F. Koenig. 2002. Building brand community.
Journal of Marketing, 66 (January): 38-54.
Meuter, Matthew L. and Mary Jo Bitner 1998. Self-service technologies: Extending service
frameworks and identifying issues for research. American Marketing Association,
Winter: 12-19.
Muniz, Albert M. and Thomas C. O’Guinn. 2001. Brand community. Journal of Consumer
Research, 27 (March): 412-432.
294
----------- and Hope Jensen Schau. Unpublished. When the consumer becomes the marketer: The
abandoned Apple Newton brand community, 1-44.
Nahapiet, Janine and Sumantra Ghoshal. 1998. Social capital, intellectual capital, and the organizational
advantage. Academy of Management Review, 23 (2): 242-266.
O’Guinn, Thomas C. and Albert M. Muniz. 2000. Correlates of brand communal affiliation strength
in high technology products, paper presented to Association for Consumer Research
Annual Conference, Salt Lake City, UT.
Peñaloza, Lisa. 2000.The commodification of the American west: Marketers’ production of
cultural meanings at the trade show. Journal of Marketing 64 (October): 82-109.
Polanyi, Karl. 1944. The great transformation. Boston: Beacon Press.
Portes, Alejandro. 1998. Social capital: Its origins and applications in modern sociology. Annual
Review of Sociology 24: 1-24.
----------- and Julia Sensenbrenner. 1993. Embeddedness and immigration: Notes on the social
determinants of economic action. American Journal of Sociology, 98 (6): 1320-50.
Putnam, Robert. 1993. Making democracy work: Civic traditions in modern Italy. Princeton:
Princeton University Press.
----------- 2000. Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New York: Simon and
Schuster.
Sandefur, R. L and E. O. Laumann. 1998. A paradigm for social capital. Rationality and Society
,
10: 481-501.
Schouten, John W. and James H. McAlexander. 1995. Subcultures of consumption: An ethnography of
the new bikers. Journal of Consumer Research 22: 1 (June): 43-61.
Vargo, Stephen L. and Robert F. Lusch. 2004. Evolving to a new dominant logic for marketing.
Journal of Marketing 68 (1): 1-17.
Woolcock, Michael. 1998. Social capital and economic development: Toward a theoretical
synthesis and political framework. Theory and Society 27: 151-208.
295
__________________________________________________
Ants, Brand Loyalty, Variety Seeking,
Market Topography,
and Aggregate Adaptive Optimization
Ben Wooliscroft, Marketing Department, University of Otago, NZ
Tim Molteno, Physics Department, University of Otago, NZ
Abstract
Brand loyalty is widely seen as having decreased, particularly since World War II. At an aggregate level
it can be seen to be healthy for a society, or group within society, to variety seek, in relation to the rate of
change of the market environment.
Introduction
Brand loyalty, or the lack of it, has been an important topic for a number of years and it regularly features
among the top priorities for the MSI. This paper provides a framework for analyzing variety seeking
behavior — or the resistance to loyalty — in consumers, at an aggregate level, and suggests that,
depending on the market environment, brand loyalty may be an un-natural state, in a changing market
place. The work of Alderson (1957; 1965) and others has been based in the systems paradigm where
individuals can be considered as members of systems which have purpose beyond that of the sum of the
individuals goals. This paper continues that work by considering one of the outcomes of the system which
is not consciously known by its members — adaptive optimization to maximize the return on purchases.
Considerable research has been undertaken into adaptive optimization in computer and biological
sciences (Davidor and Schwefel, 1992). It has much potential to be applied to the problems of the
marketplace, particularly at an aggregate level. This paper is the result of a physicist and a social scientist
having lunch together regularly and ‘talking shop’, as such it should be considered in the social physics
stream of research with a focus on behavior and not the cognitive processes involved in variety seeking.
Brand Loyalty and Variety Seeking
In spite of the fact that customers are satisfied with their experiences with a product their satisfaction
rating does not predict repurchase (Barlow and Maul, 2000; Hennig-Thurau and Klee, 1997). They
continue to exhibit variety seeking behavior.
While there has been prior research on variety seeking, it has focused on individual cognitive processes
involved in variety seeking (McAlister, 1982), deriving a taxonomy of variety seeking behavior by
individuals (McAlister and Pessemier, 1982), or the factors involved in variety seeking and how they can
be taken advantage of by managers (Van Trijp, Hoyer and Inman, 1996). A proposed explanation, at an
aggregate level, of this phenomena is developed below through the use of a metaphorical illustration.
The Ant Metaphor: Finding Their Way
Ants walk across the land in lines, but every now and then one breaks from the group and seeks out an
alternative path. The benefit of this pattern of behavior is that the member of the group who breaks away
296
can find superior routes and report them to the group, or confirm that the main path is the current
optimum. Should the landscape, or topography, change, for example a land slip or the arrival of a anteater
who resides along the previous optimum path, this behavior will help the group find a new optimum path
between two points.
It can be seen, from computer simulations of adaptive optimization (Bonabeau, Dorigo and Theraulaz,
2000; Deneubourg, Goss, Franks, Sendova-Franks, Detrain and Chŕetien, 1990; Dorigo and
Gambardellaon, 1992), that the more frequently the topography of the landscape changes the more
frequently an ant should break from the accepted path and seek another, if the optimum path is to be used
as frequently as is ideal for the colony’s benefit.
The Optimum Path in the Market Place
How does a person find their way in the market place? They are faced with countless decisions, at many
levels. There has even been suggestions that too much choice leads to less satisfaction for consumers
(Easterbrook, 2003; Schwartz, 2003). How does the consumer, or in this case group of consumers,
maximize the returns on their purchases, i.e. how do they ensure they’re following the best path through
the market?
If a person is satisfied with a brand of a certain product, say muesli, will they continue to purchase that
one brand again and again without trying other brands? Perhaps at an individual level, but at an aggregate
level we see that there is variety seeking behavior (Ehrenberg, 1964). Consumers are not loyal to their
chosen brands. Like the ants we seek variety, perhaps only to check that we are on the right path — still
getting the best muesli. How much variety we seek is dependant on the market place and its topographic
change.
Topography and the Marketplace
Topography, or the science of measuring the surface of a given environment, provides us a way of
recognizing how much change is occurring in a landscape. It can also be used to measure change in the
market place, i.e. are new products arriving, are other products disappearing, are products changing their
quality, are new flavors being introduced, are prices changing in a way that effects relative value, etc..
The Relation between Topography and Variety Seeking
It is proposed that variety seeking will be seen to be a function of changes in the topography of the market
place. The higher the rate of change in the market place, the higher the rate of variety seeking within a
given group of people. Therefore, the more change in the market place the less brand loyalty, regardless
of the relative worths of the offerings.
Who Seeks Variety?
In aggregate analysis of ants and humans it can be seen that there are leaders in most things, including
variety seeking — market innovators or market leaders. There is no suggestion that the proposition of
topographic change and variety seeking behavior can be used to explain individual shopping behavior.
Rather it is a proposition related to behavioral evolutionary theory (Laland and Brown, 2002), which
predicts aggregate or system wide behavior. However it is expected that the higher the rate of topographic
change in the landscape, for ants, or in the market place, for consumers, the more members of a group that
will engage in variety seeking behavior.
297
CONCLUSION AND FUTURE RESEARCH
In some areas of loyalty study, i.e. membership of professional organizations, or unions, we would expect
considerably less turnover, due to the relatively stable topography of that market. In others, due to the
rapid and frequent changes in the market place topography, we would expect to see much variety seeking,
for example ice creams, muesli, computer hardware, etc.
Future research will revolve around measuring market topography and testing this proposition using
aggregate data. The authors welcome suggestions, particularly where significant data sets could be
accessed.
REFERENCES
Alderson, W. 1957. Marketing Behavior and Executive Action: A Functionalist Approach to
Marketing. Homewood: Richard D. Irwin Inc.
Alderson, W. 1965. Dynamic Marketing Behavior: A Functionalist Theory of Marketing. Homewood:
Richard D. Irwin Inc.
Barlow, J. and Maul, D. 2000. Emotional Value. San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler Publishers.
Bonabeau, E., Dorigo, M. and Theraulaz, G. 2000. Inspiration for optimization from social insect
behaviour. Nature 406: 39–42.
Davidor, Y. and Schwefel, H. 1992. An introduction to adaptive optimization algorithms based on
principles of natural evolution. In Dynamic, Genetic, and Chaotic Programming, edited by B.
Soucek and the IRIS Group, 183– 202. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Deneubourg, J. L., Goss, S., Franks, N., Sendova-Franks, A., Detrain, C. and Chretien, L. 1990. The
dynamics of collective sorting robot-like ants and ant-like robots. In Proceedings of the first
international conference on simulation of adaptive behavior on From animals to animats, 356–
363. MIT Press.
Dorigo, M. and Gambardellaon, L. M. 1992. Ant algorithms for discrete optimization. Artifical Life 5:
137–172.
Easterbrook, G. 2003. The Progress Paradox: How Life Gets Better While People Feel Worse. New
York: Random House Publishing Group.
Ehrenberg, A. S. C. 1964. Estimating the Proportion of Loyal Buyers. Journal of Marketing
Research 1(1): 56–59.
Hennig-Thurau, T. and Klee, A. 1997. The Impact of Customer Satisfaction and Relationship Quality
on Customer Retention: A Critical Reassessment and Model Development. Psychology and
Marketing 14 (3): 737–764.
Laland, K. N. and Brown, G. R. 2002. Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human
Behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
McAlister, L. 1982. A Dynamic Attribute Satiation Model of Variety-Seeking Behavior. Journal of
Consumer Research 9 (2): 141-150.
McAlister, L. and E. Pessemier. 1982. Variety Seeking Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Review. Journal
of Consumer Research 9 (3) 311-322.
Schwartz, B. 2003. The Paradox of Choice: Why More is Less. New York: HarperCollins
Publishers.
Van Trijp, H. C. M., W. D. Hoyer and J. J. Inman 1996. Why Switch? Product Category-Level
Explanations for True Variety-Seeking Behavior. Journal of Marketing Research 33 (3): 281-292.
298
Early Twenty-First Century Worldviews
and Macromarketing
Mark Peterson, University of Texas at Arlington
Stephen J. Terry, University of Texas at Arlington
Abstract
In order to provide better understanding of worldviews of the early twenty-first century that will likely
influence the actions of marketers and macromarketing thought in the coming years, three religious
worldviews (Buddhism, Christianity, and Islam) and three secular worldviews (Economism, Humanism,
and what we term Transcendental Globalization) are compared and contrasted on six dimensions.
Additionally, the concept of hope is used to better understand what these worldviews offer those who
adopt them. Prospects for these worldviews and for those adopting them conclude the analysis.
The postmodern world of the early twenty-first century is experiencing a crisis of meaning (Rohr 2001;
Smith 2003). Pestilence, and war continue to manifest themselves on the global scene. Instead of
soothing tensions among peoples of the world, religion is too frequently found to be inflaming such
tensions. In this milieu, marketers still pursue commercial relationships – only now on an increasingly
global scope. Accordingly, understanding major worldviews is increasingly important not only to
transnational policy makers, and political leaders, but to non-governmental organizations (NGOs),
marketers, and informed citizens who would like to inhabit a less threatening planet.
Klein (1997) and Mittelstaedt (2002) have reviewed the interplay of religion and markets. In every region
of the world, religion affects what is traded, how trade occurs, as well as when and where trade occurs.
Conversely, markets affect religion, as can be seen in developments in contraceptives that have led to the
Catholic Church’s condemnation of these, and to divided opinion about such contraceptives amongst the
laity of the Catholic Church. Kale (2004) has extended this discourse on markets and religion to include
globalization. Kale notes that the connection between spirituality, economics and globalization can be
seen in contemporary writings about the globalization phenomena, such as Friedman’s (1999) in which
globalization is said to emerge from people’s very souls and their deepest aspirations. Other researchers
have noted the globalization-spirituality linkage in the early writings of Teilhard and St. Ignatius of
Loyola who both envisioned a future when “the consciousness of all [would be] working together in a
task as great as the world itself” (Teilhard 1967, 285).
In the area of vulnerable consumers, macromarketing scholars have prescribed other-centered love as a
resource to improve the lives of impoverished consumers (Hill 2002). In the Judeo-Christian tradition,
such agape love is a compassionate love that is viewed as an extension of love of God and a response to
“love thy neighbor”. Past research suggests intrinsic religiosity is positively correlated with altruism.
In order to provide better understanding of worldviews of the early twenty-first century that will likely
influence the actions of marketers and macromarketing thought in the coming years, three religious
worldviews (Buddhism, Christianity, and Islam) and three secular worldviews (Economism, Humanism,
and a new worldview - Transcendental Globalization) are compared and contrasted. The six dimensions
of comparison for these worldviews are 1) material QOL, 2) the nature of man, 3) source of evil/suffering,
299
4) being in community, 5) what is worshipped?, and 6) afterlife. While the uniqueness of individual
worldviews is apparent at times (For example, Christianity’s view of man as inherently evil without the
redemption offered by a relationship with Jesus Christ.), similarity across some worldviews emerges in
our analysis (For example, the exalted regard given material QOL by Economism and Transcendental
Globalization.)
We incorporate Sciolo’s recent work on the topic of hope as a final piece in our analysis of these six
worldviews. Hope has recently been cited as an important component of what marketers offer consumers
through brands (MacInnis and de Mello 2005). Sciolo proposes that the principle offering of any religion
or system of spiritual understanding is hope. According to Sciolo’s theory, three dimensions of hope are
most important: 1) mastery (ultimate aims, positive future, empowerment), 2) attachment (trust, openness,
mystical experience), and 3) survival (care, recruitment, and terror management). The six worldviews are
reviewed using this lens of hope. Prospects for these worldviews and for those adopting them conclude
the analysis.
Buddhism
Material QOL
Buddhism has at its core the belief that desire or attachment is the root of all suffering, so focused
material pursuits or prosperity are naturally not looked upon favorably. As Renard notes, Buddhist
material life should be survivable but austere.
Nature of Man
Man is seen as a being that can realize enlightenment. Widespread human inclinations to focus on desires
and imbalances are symptoms of not being enlightened.
Source of Evil/Suffering
Suffering is seen as a universal aspect of human existence brought about by focus, attachment, or desire.
The Eightfold Noble Path, a Buddhist teaching, provides a guide for elimination of this attachment-based
suffering.
Being in Community
Early Buddhist missionary efforts have created many distinct Asian Buddhist nations and traditions.
Buddhism has a very well-defined culture and community, even if universalist tendencies are sometimes
present.
What is Worshipped?
Sometimes local deities are recognized, but these are not central to the Buddhist belief system.
Siddhartha Gautama (Buddha) is revered as a teacher, and sometimes lesser enlightened individuals are
recognized.
Afterlife
Nirvana is the ultimate goal of the Buddhist, but other afterlife states (heavens and hells) are sometimes
believed to exist. Nirvana is not described to have any attributes.
300
Christianity
Material QOL
Material pursuits and prosperity viewed as a blessing, but too much emphasis on this life is viewed
unfavorably. Certain ethical guidelines, similar to those of Judaism and Islam, should guide material
conduct.
Nature of Man
Man is on a lower level than God and angels. Individuals are capable of performing righteous acts with
God's assistance. Humanity is many times considered to be inherently evil or rebellious towards God.
The fall, or first act of disobedience to God, is viewed as particularly significant in determining man's
standing.
Source of Evil/Suffering
Man's own choice to disobey God is the recognized source of evil and suffering. Whether or not man is
considered to have an evil nature, the principal source of problems in the world is this choice, especially
the first such choice by Adam.
Being in Community
Christians are part of the universal church. Many doctrinal divisions exist, including broad Protestant and
Catholic divisions. Belief in Christ's death, Bible, and the afterlife provide a common link. Membership
is determined, with differing emphasis, by an individual's beliefs, profession, and baptism.
What is Worshipped?
One God existent in three persons (this is the "Trinity" doctrine) provides the center of worship. God the
father is similar to the Jewish concept. The Holy Spirit is more personal and close to individuals. Jesus
Christ is considered to be earthly incarnation of the Son of God but still on an equal plane with first two
persons.
Afterlife
The afterlife is seen as a place of judgment or reward. Differing emphasis is put on one's own deeds, but
an individual's relation to Christ's death figures prominently into judgment as a source of grace for bad
deeds.
301
Islam
Material QOL
As Kuran notes, material pursuits and prosperity are seen in a positive light (even Muhammad was
merchant), but should be in accordance with Islamic traditions, such as alms-giving and a prohibition on
interest.
Nature of Man
Capable of choosing good (Islam) over evil and good by nature. Created by God and situated on a lesser
plane than God and spirits.
Source of Evil/Suffering
Choosing to disobey God is the principal source of problems. A devil figure ("Iblis" or "Shaytan," a jinn)
contributes to evil along with many lesser jinn (evil spirits).
Being in Community
Muslims are a part of the umma, or universal Muslim community. Principal doctrinal divisions are
Sunnis and Shi'ites. All Muslims support the common links of the five pillars, Muhammad, and the
Koran.
What is Worshipped?
One God (this is an important facet of Islamic doctrine), described through Ninety-nine Beautiful Names.
Seen as merciful, all-powerful, close to individuals, and in control of all worldly happenings.
Afterlife
Day of judgment, person's good and bad deeds weighed. Paradise/Hell assigned accordingly. Certain
faiths, "Peoples of the Book" (Christianity, Judaism) typically seen as closer to salvation than members of
other faiths.
302
SECULAR WORLDVIEWS
Economism
(A variety of inputs have been used to compile a profile of Economism. Woehlring’s (2003) critique of
theological economics, and Friedman’s neo-liberal economic views are principal to our analysis. Frank’s
(2005) notes on the economics profession’s guiding tenet: self-interest, as well as Stalk’s (2005) update
on corporations
Material QOL
It is seen as the most important factor in society. From it, individuals are able to draw both purpose and
self-motivation. Wealth is viewed as a good influence in society, while poverty or lack from any source
is viewed negatively.
Nature of Man
Above all, humanity is seen as self-interested. This is not interpreted as “good” or “bad,” but rather leads
to the important conclusion that individuals will usually make their decisions in such a way as to
maximize their own personal satisfaction.
Source of Evil/Suffering
Most problems, and the barrier that holds mankind back from utopia, come from scarcity. Material want
and dissatisfaction are introduced because individuals are forced to forgo certain benefits that are
precluded by scarcity. Reductions in economic and political freedoms are also seen as sources of
dissatisfaction and scarcity.
Being in Community
An individual is part of the collection of all economic agents, who purchase from and provide goods and
services to other economic agents. People also exist as citizens in government, whose primary role
should be that of guarantor of economic rights (Friedman, 1962).
What is Worshipped?
Because deity is usually not considered to play a role in man’s life, individuals themselves and the
process of economic exchange and growth provide the unifying and all-important pieces of man’s
existence.
Afterlife
This period is typically assumed to be non-existent, or simply the legendary product of individual cultures
and backgrounds. It may have an affect on a person’s life and actions, but this is simply their own
reaction to their beliefs.
303
Humanism
Humanist Manifesto I; Noebel (1996)
Material QOL
It is seen as a practically important part of human existence, but one that should be of little significance to
each individual. As the “Humanist Manifesto I” notes, a socialist, equality-based society and the eventual
absence of material cares present the best path towards human progress.
Nature of Man
Life is entirely biological, and behavior is determined by environmental factors such as family, prominent
societal institutions, government. As Noebel notes, humans are seen as capable of forward evolution and,
eventually, self-perfection.
Source of Evil/Suffering
Ineffective societies and environments are the only sources of suffering. Evil is a concept that does not
necessarily have a standard definition or source, but can generally be recognized as societal influences
hindering the forward progression/evolution of man.
Being in Community
Man is a part of the universal, physical community. Political, intellectual, and economic institutions
provide a framework for the secular community. Mankind's evolution or progress can be aided through
these entities.
What is Worshipped?
No deities are believed to exist, although the society at large and the process of evolution (physical and
societal) can play similar roles as universal, all-controlling forces.
Afterlife
No afterlife is recognized, as physical death brings an end to all biological processes.
304
Transcendental Globalization
(Thomas P.M. Barnett’s book The Pentagon’s New Map serves as the foundation for this worldview
which poses four flows as being essential to the vitality of globalization. These flows are 1) goods and
services, 2) investments, 3) people (emigration/immigration), and 4) security. The developed world’s
military and para-military forces are proposed to be the needed agents for snuffing out regional trouble
spots that endanger the advance of globalization, such as the genocide that occurred in Rawanda, or the
Taliban’s nurturing of terrorist training camps. . While Barnett does not explicitly label globalization as
a worldview, he implicitly suggests it is one. The structure of this implicit suggestion is captured below.
On the same topic, Gray (2000) has described globalization as a religious delusion.
Material QOL
Material life is seen as a fundamental need that motivates humanity but in a smaller manner than in
Economism. The “good life” is an important goal, but it is not the purpose of life. A GDP per capita of
$10,000 (US) is seen as the threshold for obtaining an appropriate level of happiness. After this, Gains in
material QOL don’t correlate with happiness (Diener and Seligman 2004; Easterbrook 2005).
McDonald’s franchises in a country signify a society’s warring tendencies have been curbed (Friedman
1999).
Nature of Man
Man needs rules. Misalignment of rules triggers conflict and wars. With the right rules and with the right
alignment of rules, interconnectedness and prosperity can be realized and perpetuated. Man is a multifaceted being that has a capacity for interconnectedness and self-improvement. This capacity is what
primarily determines their actions and outlooks. Individuals can likewise be separated into two broad
descriptive categories, those who share a part of the mainstream, connected outlook on life and those who
do not.
Source of Evil/Suffering
Tribalism is the source of evil and suffering. Contributing to the formation of tribalism is fear and a crisis
of meaning. Evil is recognized as the condition of being cut off from globalization and opposing the
mainstream international community. Evil is recognized as the condition of being cut off from
globalization and the mainstream international community. Political and economic repression, virtually
non-existent in the larger community, are very much present in a cut-off sector of the planet, and they find
their source in the very fact that that sector of the population is isolated from the rest of the planet.
Being in Community
The mainstream globalizing “core” community of developed countries is expanding and provides the
basis for connectedness. Another, “gap” community of countries is detached from or not firmly
connected to the core. Here, individuals do not necessarily share common values across tribes or groups.
Humanity is separated into two broad groups that increasingly provide identity for individuals.
What is Worshipped?
Globalization, itself, is projected to have powers similar to a deity. It is unstoppable. It is increasingly
omnipresent. It brings salvation from tribalism and strife. It imparts a destiny of global harmony. It
paves the way for self-actualizing individuals. In these ways, globalization is suggested to be
305
transcendent, as well as offering transcendence to those who become part of the unfolding network of
connections around the planet.
Afterlife
Kant’s eternal peace awaits a world totally connected and mutually dependent. Individual afterlife is
generally considered to be unimportant.
REFERENCES
Barnett, Thomas P.M. 2004. The Pentagon’s new map. New York, NY: G.P. Putnam’s Sons.
Diener, E. and M. E. P. Seligman. 2004. Beyond money: Toward an economy of well-being.
Psychological Science in the Public Interest 5 (1): 1-31.
Easterbrook, G. 2005. The real truth about money. Time January 17 A32-A33.
Esposito, J. 2002. World religions today. New York: Oxford University Press.
Ferris, A. 2002. Religion and the Quality of Life. Journal of Happiness Studies 3: 199-215.
Frank, R. 2005. Economic Scene: If firmly believed, the theory that self-interest is the sole motivator
appears to be self-fulfilling. New York Times February 17, c2.
Friedman, M. 1962. Capitalism and freedom. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Friedman, T. 1999. The Lexus and the Olive Tree. New York: Anchor Press.
Gray, J. 2000. False Dawn: The Delusions of Global Capitalism, London: New Press.
Halverson, D. C. 1996. The compact guide to world religions. Minneapolis, MN: Bethany House
Publishers.
Hill, R. P. 2002. Compassionate Love, Agape, and Altruism: A New Framework for Understanding and
Supporting Impoverished Consumers. Journal of Macromarketing 22, (June): 19-31.
Humanist Manifesto I, from http://www.americanhumanist.org/about/manifesto1.html accessed February
16, 2005.
Johansson, S. 2002. Conceptualizing and measuring quality of life for national policy. Social
Indicators Research 58: 13-32.
Kale, S. 2004. Spirituality, Religion, and Globalization. Journal of Macromarketing 24 (December):
92-107.
Kuran, T. 1997. The genesis of Islamic economics: A chapter in the politics of Muslim identity. Social
Research 64: 301-338.
MacInnis, D. and G. E de Mello. 2005. The Concept of Hope and Its Relevance to Product Evaluation and
Choice. Journal of Marketing 69 (January) 1-14.
306
Mittelstaedt, J. 2002. A Framework for Understanding the Relationships between Religions and
Markets. Journal of Macromarketing 22 (June): 6-18.
Noebel, D. A. 1991. Understanding the Times. Manitou Springs, CO: Summit Press.
Novak, P. 1994. The World’s Wisdom: Sacred Texts of the World’s Religions. San Francisco, CA:
HarperSanFrancisco.
Renard, J. 2002. The Handy Religion Answer Book. New York, NY: Barnes & Noble Books.
Rohr, R. 2001. Hope against darkness: The transforming vision of Saint Francis in an age of anxiety.
Cincinnati, OH: St. Anthony Messenger Press.
Scioli, A. 2005. Hope in the Age of Anxiety. Book available at www.gainhope.com.
________ 2004. Hope and spirituality in an age of anxiety. Presentation at the 2004 International Society
for Quality of Life Studies (ISQOLS) Conference. Philadelphia, PA.
Sirgy, M. J 2002. The Psychology of Quality-of-Life. Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
_________2001. Handbook of Quality-of-Life Research: An Ethical Marketing Perspective.
Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Smith, H. 2003. Beyond the Postmodern Mind: The Place of Meaning in a Global Civilization. Wheaton,
IL: Quest Books.
________1991. The World’s Religions: Our Great Wisdom Traditions. San Francisco, CA:
HarperSanFrancisco.
Stalk, G. 2005. Warm and fuzzy doesn’t cut it. Wall Street Journal, February 15.
Teilhard de Chardin, P. 1967. Writings in time of war. Translated by B. Wall. New York: Harper & Row.
Woehrling, F. 2003. Toward a Theology of Economics: Arresting Congenital Scarcity by Developing
Exchanges. Journal of Markets and Morality 6, (Fall) 409-431.
307
308
Sometimes local deities are recognized,
but these are not central to the Buddhist
belief system. Siddhartha Gautama
(Buddha) is revered as a teacher, and
sometimes lesser enlightened individuals
are recognized.
What is
Worshipped?
Nirvana is the ultimate goal of the
Buddhist, but other afterlife levels
(heavens and hells) are sometimes
believed to exist. Nirvana is not
described to have any attributes.
Early Buddhist missionary efforts have
created many distinct Asian Buddhist
nations and traditions. Buddhism has a
very well-defined culture and
community, even if universalist
tendencies are sometimes present.
Being in
Community
Afterlife
Suffering seen as a universal aspect of
human existence brought about by
focus/attachment/desire. The Eightfold
Noble Path, a Buddhist teaching,
provides a guide for elimination of this
attachment-based suffering.
Man can be seen as a being that can
realize enlightenment. Widespread
human inclinations to focus on desires
and imbalances are symptoms of not
being enlightened.
Buddhism has at its core the belief that
desire/attachment is the root of all
suffering, so focused material pursuits or
prosperity are naturally not looked upon
favorably. As Renard notes, Buddhist
material life should be survivable but
austere.
Source of
Evil/Suffering
Nature of
Man
Material QOL
Buddhism
The afterlife is seen as a place of
judgment or reward. Differing emphasis
is put on one's own deeds, but an
individual' relation to Christ's death
figures prominently into judgment as a
source of grace for bad deeds.
One God existent in three persons (this is the
"Trinity" doctrine) provides the center of worship.
God the father is similar to the Jewish concept.
The Holy Spirit is more personal and close to
individuals. Jesus Christ considered to be earthly
incarnation of the Son of God but still on an equal
plane with first two persons.
Christians are part of the universal church
community. Many doctrinal divisions exist,
including broad Protestant and Catholic divisions.
Belief in Christ's death, Bible, and the afterlife
provide a common link. Membership is
determined, with differing emphasis, by an
individual's beliefs, profession, and baptism.
Man's own choice to disobey God is the
recognized source of evil and suffering.
Whether or not man is considered to
have an evil nature, the principal source
of problems in the world is this choice,
especially the first such choice by Adam.
Man is on a lower level than God and angels.
Individuals are capable of performing righteous
acts with God's assistance. Humanity is many
times considered to be inherently evil or
rebellious towards God. The fall, or first act of
disobedience to God, is viewed as particularly
significant in determining man's standing.
Material pursuits and prosperity viewed
as a blessing, but too much emphasis on
this life is viewed unfavorably. Certain
ethical guidelines, similar to those of
Judaism and Islam, should guide
material conduct.
Christianity
This period is typically assumed to be
nonexistent, or simply the legendary
product of individual cultures and
backgrounds. It may have an affect on a
person’s life and actions, but this is
simply their own reaction to their beliefs.
Because deity is usually not considered
to play a role in man’s life, individuals
themselves and the process of economic
exchange and growth provide the
unifying and all-important pieces of
man’s existence.
An individual is part of the collection of
all economic agents, who purchase from
and provide goods and services to other
economic agents. People also exist as
citizens in government, whose primary
role should be that of guarantor of
economic rights (Friedman, 1962).
Most problems, and the barrier that holds
mankind back from utopia, come from scarcity.
Material want and dissatisfaction are introduced
because individuals are forced to forgo certain
benefits that are precluded by scarcity. Reductions
in economic and political freedoms are also seen
as sources of dissatisfaction and scarcity.
Above all, humanity is seen as selfinterested. This is not interpreted as
“good” or “bad,” but rather leads to the
important conclusion that individuals
will usually make their decisions in such
a way as to maximize their own personal
satisfaction.
It is seen as the most important factor in
society. From it, individuals are able to
draw both purpose and self-motivation.
Wealth is viewed as a good influence in
society, while poverty or lack from any
source is viewed negatively.
Economism
No afterlife is recognized, as physical
death brings an end to all biological
processes.
No deities are believed to exist, although
the society at large and the process of
evolution (physical and societal) can
play similar roles as universal, allcontrolling forces.
Man is a part of the universal, physical
community. Political, intellectual, and
economic institutions provide a
framework for the secular community.
Mankind's evolution or progress can be
aided through these entities.
Ineffective societies and environments
are the only sources of suffering. Evil is
a concept that does not necessarily have
a standard definition or source, but can
generally be recognized as societal
influences hindering the forward
progression/evolution of man.
Life is entirely biological, and behavior
is determined by environmental factors
such as family, prominent societal
institutions, government. As Noebel
notes, humans are seen as capable of
forward evolution and, eventually, selfperfection.
It is seen as a practically important part
of human existence, but one that should
be of little significance to each
individual. As the Humanist Manifesto I
notes, a socialist, equality-based society
and the eventual absence of material
cares present the best path towards
human progress.
Humanism
Major Worldviews
Day of judgment, person's good and bad
deeds weighed. Paradise/Hell assigned
accordingly. Certain faiths, "Peoples of
the Book" (Christianity, Judaism)
typically seen as closer to salvation than
members of other faiths.
One God (this is an important facet of
Islamic doctrine), described through
Ninety-nine Beautiful Names. Seen as
merciful, all-powerful, close to
individuals, and in control of all worldly
happenings.
Muslims are a part of the umma, or
universal Muslim community. Principal
doctrinal divisions are Sunnis and
Shi'ites All Muslims support the
common links of the five pillars,
Muhammad, and the Koran.
Choosing to disobey God is the principal
source of problems. A devil figure
("Iblis" or "Shaytan," a jinn) contributes
to evil along with many lesser jinn (evil
spirits).
Capable of choosing good (Islam) over
evil and good by nature. Created by God
and situated on a lesser plane than God
and spirits.
As Kuran notes, material pursuits and
prosperity are seen in a positive light
(even Muhammad was merchant), but
should be in accordance with Islamic
traditions, such as alms-giving and a
prohibition on interest.
Islam
Kant's eternal peace awaits a world
totally connected and mutually
dependent. Individual afterlife is
generally considered to be unimportant.
Globalization, itself, is projected to have
powers of a deity. It is unstoppable. It is
increasingly omnipresent. It brings
salvation from tribalism and strife. It
imparts a destiny of global harmony. It
paves the way for self-actualizing
individuals.
The mainstream globalizing "core" community of
developed countries is expanding and provides the
basis for connectedness. Another, "gap"
community of countries is detached from or not
firmly connected to the core. Here, individuals do
not necessarily share common values across tribes
or groups.
Tribalism. Evil is recognized as the condition of
being cut off from globalization and opposing the
mainstream international community. Political and
economic repression, virtually non-existent in the
larger community, are very much present in
countries of the planet's gap - those cut off from
the developed core countries.
Man needs rules. Misalignment of rules triggers
conflict and wars. With the right rules and with the
right alignment of rules, interconnectedness and
prosperity can be realized and perpetuated.
Individuals can be separated into two broad
descriptive categories, those who endorse
connectedness and those who don't.
A GDP per capita of $10,000 (US) seen
as the threshold for obtaining an
appropriate level of happiness. After this,
gains in material QOL don't correlate
with happiness. McDonald's franchises in
a country signify a society's warring
tendencies have been curbed.
Transcendental
Globalization
Macromarketing Implications of Religious Fractionalization
and Religious Affiliation
John D. Mittelstaedt, Clemson University, USA
Robert A. Mittelstaedt, University of Nebraska, USA
Abstract
Macromarketing has demonstrated an interest in the effects of religion on marketing and marketing
systems, and the effects of marketing systems on religions and religious institutions. This paper expands
the discussion to include the religious distributions of nations, in an attempt to better understand the
effects of faith on the development of markets as social provisioning systems.
Introduction
In recent years there has been a growing interest among macromarketers in the effects of religious
organizations and religious beliefs on marketing systems, and the effects of marketing systems on religion
and religious beliefs. Mittelstaedt (2002) laid out a framework for understanding the avenues through
which religious beliefs and organizations affect when, how and where market exchanges occur. Klein
(1997) has examined the market implications of Catholic Social teaching on marketing systems, as well as
the role of religious organizations as market competitors. Hill (2002) considered love as a market
orientation, with implications for markets and market systems. Most recently, Kale (2004) explored the
effects of spirituality on a globalizing economy. Conceptually, then, great strides have been made in our
understanding of the relationship between matters of conviction and commerce.
To date, however, macromarketing scholars have set aside empirical questions of the effects of religion
on marketing, and on externalities of interest to macromarketers. Are there relationships between the
religious convictions of a country and its objective quality of life? Does the quality of governance vary
with religious demographics? Can religion explain long term economic growth? To the extent that each
of these is an important objective of an effective provisioning system, this paper intends to explore
relationships between the religious constitution of countries and the aspirations of well-functioning
marketing systems.
The purpose of this paper is to examine the relationships, at the level of nations, between distribution of
religious faith and the political, social and economic development of countries. Data collected by Barrett
and Johnson (2002) of faith affiliation is contrasted with the United Nations Human Development Index
(2004), estimates of political stability by the World Bank Institute (Kaufmann, Kraay and Mastruzzi
2003), and measures of 15- and 25-year economic growth (United Nations 2004. Results indicate that the
nature and distribution of religious groups affects both human development and political stability, but that
effects on long term economic growth are channeled through human development and political stability.
Consequences for macromarketing are considered.
Few countries are religious monoliths. For historic, cultural, political and economic reasons, people
follow different paths of spirituality and religious affiliation. Both the nature of religious belief and the
diversity of religious opinion affect the character and trajectory of political and social life in a country,
and as a consequence its economic and human development. To the former, Mittelstaedt (2002)
discussed the effects of religious affiliation on the social and economic life of countries, while Kilbourne,
309
et al. (1997) have addressed the latter by laying out the basic tenets of dominant social paradigms on a
country’s economic and market development. Within this context, we propose that the religious diversity
of a country moderates the effects of religious life on social, economic and market activities, while the
presence of a dominant religious community sets the tone for the dominant social paradigm.
Religious Fractionalization
What is fractionalization, and how is it the same or different from diversity? For our purposes we define
fractionalization as the diversity, in number and proportion, of religious communities within a society.
Both number and proportion are important in establishing the religious makeup of a country. A country
might be evenly spilt among three religious sects, and we would expect that each would then have an
influence on the cultural makeup of a country. Conversely, a country may be divided among three
religious sects, one constituting 80% of the population while the other two reflect only 20% of believers.
Here, the variety of religious belief would be the same, but the social and political implications of
religious affiliation would be different. Put another way, the diversity would be the same, but the
fractionalization of belief would vary.
To capture the fractionalization of religious belief within countries, a Herfindahl-Hirshman Index of
religious belief was calculated, as the sum of the squared probabilities of drawing at random members of
a religious group,
n
RF = 1 -
∑ (% RA )
i =1
i
2
,
where RF = religious fractionalization and RA is religious affiliation. In this form, 0 ≤ RF ≤ 1, where RF
= 1 indicates maximum religious homogeneity and RF = 0 indicates maximum religious heterogeneity.
Data were collected for each of 238 countries, based on religious survey data of Barrett and Johnson
(2002). Barrett and Johnson report proportional affiliation of adherents to 19 of the largest religions of
the world, as well as non-believers and atheists (see Table One). Table Two reports the most and least
religiously fractionalized countries in the world. The most religiously fractionalized country in the world
is the Central African Republic (20% Sunni, 20% Indigenous, 18% Roman Catholic, 14% Protestant,
12% Non-denominational Christian), followed by Estonia (30% non-religious, 20% Orthodox Christian,
17% Protestant, 10% Atheist, 3% Non-denominational Christian). Prior to its collapse, Yugoslavia was
the second most fractionalized country. The most religiously homogeneous country in the world is the
Western Sahara (99.4% Sunni Muslim). The popular example for religious homogeneity, Saudi Arabia,
is only 93.7% Sunni.
Note that both lists include countries large (e.g., United Kingdom) and small (Christmas Island), rich
(Italy) and poor (Somalia), stable (Australia) and unstable (Kenya). This suggests to us the following.
First, religious fractionalization may play a role in political-, economic- and human develop, but in
conjunction with other factors. Second, the effects of religious fractionalization could be affected by the
dominant religion of a nation. Given these conditions, the effects of religious fractionalization need to be
considered in the context of other variables.
Effects of Religious Fractionalization
Religious diversity is likely to have two effects of the social life of a nation. On the one hand, religious
diversity can undermine social cohesion in a country. Studies of the effects of ethnic and linguistic
fractionalization have found that as these forms of fractionalization increase, so too does political
instability (Barro and McCleary 2003). Alternatively, Richard Florida’s (2002) work on tolerance and
economic development in US cities finds that diversity leads to greater social tolerance, which leads to
310
increased economic development. Given these findings, we can expect religious fractionalization to have
an effect on economic development, but cannot predict, a priori, the direction of its effect. Religious
dominance (defined as 40% or more of a single religious faith) may play a moderating role, as well.
Three effects of religious fractionalization were measured. First, we considered the effects of religious
fractionalization on political stability, a necessary component for social and economic stability. Religious
fractionalization and religious dominance were regressed on the Kaufmann, Kraay and Mastruzzi’s
(2003) Index of Good Governance (see Table Three). This multi-item measure summarizes a number of
factors of good governance, and we expected that increased religious fractionalization would stress these
political indicators of governance.
Second, we considered the effects of religious fractionalization on quality of life. We regressed religious
fractionalization, religious dominance and governance on the United Nations Human Development Index,
a measure that reflects health, education and economic prosperity (see Table Four). We expected that
greater religious diversity would affect social tolerance, and that this would manifest itself in these
objective measures of quality of life.
Finally, we asked whether religious fractionalization had an effect on long term economic growth.
Because of the importance of political stability, and worker health and education on economic growth,
these were included as factors in assessing the direct and indirect effects of religious fractionalization on
long term economic growth. United Nations (2004) measures of annual economic growth from 19752002 and 1990-2002 were used assess the effects of religious-, political- and social capital for two
important periods of time. In 1973, the United States went off the gold standard, marking the beginning
of the modern era of globalization. In 1989 the Berlin Wall fell, bringing Eastern and Central Europe, as
well as large parts of Asia, into the global economy. In the wake of these changes, what role did religion
play?
Findings
Table Five reports the standardized β coefficients for the regressions described above. Religious
fractionalization has a significant, negative effect on good governance, indicating that the greater the
religious diversity of a country, the less likely it is to be stable, responsive or effective. Protestantism,
Anglicanism and Chinese folk religion, as dominant religions within a country, tend to increase the
effectiveness of governance, while Orthodoxy, Buddhism, Sunni and Shia Islam, as well as indigenous
beliefs, are negatively associated with good governance. In addition, religious fractionalization has a
negative effect on human development, as does dominant presence of indigenous religions. Good
governance and Orthodox Christianity, on the other hand, are associated with improved human
conditions. Finally, the effects of religious fractionalization on long term economic growth appear to be
absorbed by other factors, including good governance and human development. In the last quarter of the
20th Century, quality of life, in terms of education and health had the greatest effect on long term
economic growth, while in the period following the fall of Communism, effective political governance
was most important. Again, Orthodoxy was negatively associated with growth. Non-religiousness was
strongly associated with Globalization growth, while Hinduism and Buddhism were positively associated
with Post-Communist growth.
Implications and Future Directions
It appears that religion plays a role in shaping the political, economic and human development growth of
countries, but in different ways. Religious fractionalization appears to make it more difficult for nations
to develop politically, and to establish basic social foundations of healthcare and education. On the other
hand, some forms of religion, if dominant in a society, appear to affect governance, quality of life and
long term economic growth. Some are positive, while others negative. While not conclusive, these
311
findings indicate that understanding the role of religion in the market is important to understanding the
consequences of market activity – political, social and economic.
REFERENCES
Barrett, D.B. and T.M. Johnson. 2002. A statistical approach to the world’s religious adherents, 20002050 C.E., in Religions of the World: A Comprehensive Encyclopedia of Beliefs and Practices,
Volume I, J.G. Melton and M. Baumann (Ed.s). Santa Barbara, California: ABC CLIO.
Barro, R.J. and R.M. MeCleary. 2003. Religion and economic growth across countries. American
Sociological Review 68(5), 760-781.
Florida, R. 2002. The Rise of the Creative Class. New York: Perseus Books Group.
Grier, R. 1997. The effect of religion on economic development: a cross national study of 63 former
colonies. Kyklos 50(1), 47-62.
Hill. R.P. 2002. Compassionate love, agape and altruism: a new framework for understanding and
supporting impoverished consumers. Journal of Macromarketing 22 (June), 19-31.
Kaufmann, D., A. Kraay and M. Mastruzzi. 2003. Governance matters III: governance indicators for
1996-2002. World Bank Policy Research Working Paper #3106.
Kale. S. 2004. Spirituality, religion and globalization. Journal of Macromarketing 24(December), 92-107.
Kilbourne, W., P. McDonough and A. Prothero. 1997. Sustainable consumption and the quality of life: a
macromarketing challenge to the dominant social paradigm. Journal of Macromarketing 17 (June),
59-77.
Klein, T. 1987. Prophets and profits: a macromarketing perspective on “Economic justice for all: Catholic
social teaching and the U.S. economy. Journal of Macromarketing 7(Spring), 59-77.
Klein, T. 1997. The influence of religion on marketing institutions. Proceedings of the 22nd Annual
Macromarketing Conference, vol. 1, A.W. Falkenburg and T.L. Rittenburg (Ed.s). Bergen, Norway,
Norwegian School of Economics and Business Administration.
Mittelstaedt, J.D. 2002. A framework for understanding the relationships between religions and markets.
Journal of Macromarketing 22(June), 6-18.
United Nations. 2004. Human Development Report 2004: Cultural Liberty in Today’s Diverse World.
New York: United Nations Development Programme.
312
Table One: Religions of the World
Religion
Christian
Roman Catholic Christian
Non-Denominational Christians
Protestant Christian
Orthodox
Anglican
Marginalized Christians
Muslim
Sunni Muslim
Shia Muslim
Hindus
Non-Religious
Chinese Folk-Religionists
Buddhists
Ethnoreligionists
Atheists
Neoreligionists
Sikhs
Jews
Spiritists
Baha’is
Confucianists
Jains
Shintoists
Taoists
Zoroastrians
Other
Barrett and Johnson (2003)
% of World Population
17.5%
6.4%
5.7%
3.4%
0.8%
> 0.1%
18.3%
1.3%
13.4%
12.7%
6.4%
6.0%
3.8%
2.5%
1.7%
0.4%
0.2%
0.2%
0.1%
0.1%
0.1%
0.1%
>0.1%
>0.1%
>0.1%
313
Table Two: Most and Least Religiously Fractionalized Countries in the World
Rank
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
314
Most Fractionalized
Central African Republic
Estonia
Saint Vincent and the Grenadines
Suriname
New Zealand
South Korea
Malawi
Ghana
Kenya
Australia
Guyana
Lesotho
Trinidad and Tobago
Barbados
United States
Christmas Island
Netherlands
Comoros
Canada
Tanzania
Latvia
United Kingdom
Channel Islands
British Virgin islands
Falkland Islands
Least Fractionalized
Western Sahara
Martinique
Moldova
Tunisia
Yemen
Saint Pierre and Miquelon
Somalia
Cape Verde
Italy
Mayotte
Sweden
Spain
Colombia
Libya
Iraq
Wallis and Futuna Islands
Peru
Guadeloupe
Malta
Luxembourg
Venezuela
Norway
Djibouti
Ecuador
Mexico
Table Three: Index of Good Governance
Governance Indicator
Voice and Accountability
Political Stability and Absence of Violence
Government Effectiveness
Regulatory Quality
Rule of Law
Control of Corruption
Defined
The process by which those in authority are
selected and replaced
The likelihood that the government in power
will be destabilized or overthrown by
unconstitutional and/or violent means
Quality of public service provision, quality of
bureaucracy, competence of civil servants,
independence of the civil service from political
pressures, credibility of the government’s
commitment to policies
Incidence of market-unfriendly policies, as well
as burdens imposed by excessive regulation
Extent to which agents have confidence in and
abide by the rule of law
Perceptions of corruption, defined as the
exercise of public power for private gain
Kaufmann, Kraay and Mastruzzi (2003)
Table Four: 2004 United Nations Human Development Index
Index Component
Life Expectancy Index (33%)
Education Index (33%)
GDP Index (33%)
Basis of Measure
0-1 index, based on life expectancy at birth (in
years), 2002 estimates
0-1 index, based on adult literacy rate (% age
15 and above), 2002 estimates, and combined
gross enrollment ratio for primary, secondary
and tertiary schools, 2001/2002 estimates
0-1, based on GDP per capita (Purchasing
Power Parity of US$), 2002
United Nations (2004)
315
Table Five: OLS Regression Results
Standardized Beta Coefficients
DV
Good
Governance
Human
Development
Economic
Growth
’75 -‘02
Economic
Growth
’90 – ‘02
----
----
.209c
-.017
----
.686a
.025
-.206a
-.130b
-.058
-.064
-.116
.010
-.039
.073
.179c
-.084
.014
-.040
-.058
-.045
.073
-.003
Human
Development
Good
Governance
Religious
Fractionalization
Roman
Catholicism
Protestantism
NonDenominational
Christian
Anglican
Orthodox
Christian
Hindu
Jewish
Buddhist
Sunni Muslim
Shia Muslim
Chinese Folk
Indigenous
Other
Non-Religious
.140b
-.144c
.014
.128c
-.007
-.248b
.005
-.186c
-.051
.012
-.134c
-.416a
-.209a
.124c
-.152b
-.022
-.049
-.046
.052
.008
-.089
.045
-.002
-.147a
.033
.049
.079
-.024
.145
-.105
.001
.031
.022
-.054
.233a
.139c
.023
.248a
-.109
-.022
.094
-.042
-.008
.112
n
F
R2
Adj. R2
187
4.831a
.296
.235
170
15.865a
.622
.583
159
1.810b
.178
.080
145
3.793a
.335
.247
316
.294b
__________________________________________________
Women and Development in Uganda
Terri L. Rittenburg*
Abstract
This study examines development issues in Uganda, with a focus on the role of women in this African
nation. Uganda is a poor country facing many challenges; its situation parallels other war-torn
countries macromarketers have studied. Through observation, field notes, and interviews, data were
collected to identify emergent themes related to women and development, investigating these issues from
a macromarketing perspective. A number of positive factors favoring development as well as challenges
to development are identified. Specific macromarketing issues include a need for marketing training and
assistance and marketing channel structural issues. Further research is needed to more clearly
understand marketing problems in Uganda and identify possible solutions to these problems.
Once described by Winston Churchill as the ‘Pearl of Africa’, the east African country of Uganda is a
place of many contrasts. Though blessed with fertile lands, regular rainfall, a good growing climate, and
sizable mineral deposits (CIA 2004), Uganda’s historical political strife has slowed its economic
development. Recent developments are positive, however; Uganda’s prospects appear bright, with multiparty elections scheduled for March 2006, growth in foreign investments, AIDS cases down, and
education up (International Herald Tribune 2004). The nation faces many challenges as it strives to join
the global economy. The purpose of this paper is to examine the issues and challenges related to
Uganda’s development and further, to explore the role of women in this development. A macromarketing
perspective will be taken in identifying the systemic issues crucial to the success of Ugandan
development.
Background
Established as a British protectorate in 1894, Uganda gained independence in 1962. Despite an optimistic
beginning and an orderly and relatively harmonious transition to independence, within a few years
Uganda became notorious for its tyranny and oppression. Under the dictator Idi Amin, the government
and economy were in a state of siege. Under Amin, the economy crumbled and public services
disintegrated (Europa World Yearbook 2004; Leggett 2001).
Uganda’s situation began to turn around when the National Resistance Movement captured the capital
city of Kampala in 1986. A new government was installed, and an economic recovery program was
introduced in 1987. Inflation, over 200 percent at the program’s inception, had dropped to 4 percent by
2000 (Leggett 2001). Since 1986, dramatic strides have been made on several fronts. Public services
have been restored, infrastructure repaired, and the economy has undergone transformation. Despite
continued opposition, the government led by Yoweri Museveni has made progress in democratization,
particularly at local levels, through a local council structure. An ambitious program of universal primary
education (UPE) is underway, aimed at providing free education for four children from every family.
UPE has increased public spending on education to 4 percent of GDP; this includes construction of
classrooms, improvements to teacher training, and provision of free textbooks for children (Leggett
2001).
*
This study was made possible by funding from the U.S. Department of State and the League of Women Voters of
the U.S. The author also wishes to thank anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.
317
Despite the dazzling progress made in the last 18 years, Uganda’s problems still abound. Though
Ugandans have put the bloody dictatorship of Amin behind them, peace remains an elusive target. The
Lord’s Resistance Army (LRA), under the leadership of Joseph Kony, brought together a small, hard-core
group of rebels from different movements (Leggett 2001). Active in the northern part of Uganda for the
past 18 years, the LRA initially targeted only government troops, but began to engage civilians in 199192 when civilian militias were mobilized against it. Attempts at peace talks have failed, and the LRA
continues brutal tactics against civilians to maintain its resistance operation. At least 1.6 million people
have fled their homes in northern Uganda to escape the LRA, devastating the agricultural economic base
of this region. As of 2003 it was estimated that the LRA had snatched around 20,000 children from rural
villages (WPR Special Correspondent 2003). Although Sudan has scaled back its support of the LRA and
cooperated in a 2002 operation to allow Ugandan forces to enter its territory to attack LRA bases, the
group endures. The International Criminal Court began investigating the LRA for war crimes in July
2004 (Clark 2004).
On the economic front, Uganda’s picture has brightened, but many still live in poverty. Uganda faces the
same challenges as many other former colonies in transforming itself from an imperial trading system to
an independent player in the global market. Uganda remains primarily an agrarian economy, dominated
by small family farms. Over 80 percent of the working population is employed in agriculture (Europa
World Yearbook 2004). Most rural Ugandans still collect water by hand, gather firewood by hand, dig by
hand, and harvest by hand (Leggett 2001). A prime example of economic dualism, Uganda consists of
the almost bizarre juxtaposition of the modern, affluent capital city of Kampala with impoverished rural
villages comprised of mud-brick huts and little access to electricity or running water. There is a clear
geographic pattern to this imbalance. An artifact of colonial development, the ‘fertile crescent’ around
Lake Victoria drew infrastructure development, leading to economic disparities between this region and
the poorer, drier northern districts. Such industrial development as has taken place has reinforced this
geographic development pattern, from Jinja in the east to Masaka in the west, forming a corridor with
Kampala at its center. While uneven development has been an ongoing trend since pre-colonial times, the
regional imbalances have been exacerbated by further impoverishment and marginalization of the north,
coupled with the unrest created by the LRA. As Leggett (2001) notes, “Collectively, they have failed to
use economic development as a means of promoting equity and consolidating nationhood, as well as a
means of achieving sustainable growth” (p. 57). Furthermore, Uganda’s recovery has been achieved
without any fundamental improvement in the basic structure or capacity of the economy. “Uganda is
completely dependent on aid, and while dependency continues, so does poverty” (Leggett 2001, p. 60).
According to the World Bank (2003), 82 percent of Uganda’s population is living on less than $1 a day,
an indicator of the international poverty line. Clearly there is much to be done.
The specter of AIDS has devastated much of Africa, Uganda included. Yet here again, there is cause for
optimism of late. Spreading on the shores of Lake Victoria in the late 1970s, Uganda’s first AIDS case
was diagnosed in 1982. AIDS prevalence peaked at a little over 30 percent (measured among antenatal
clinic attendees in major urban areas) in 1990, and has declined steadily since 1992 to a current estimate
of 4.1 percent of adults with the virus (Avert.org 2004). In fact, Uganda’s public education campaign to
reduce casual sex is held up as a model for other countries in their AIDS-reduction efforts (CultureGrams
2004). Uganda has benefited from money channeled into Africa to assist with cutting HIV/AIDS, and is
showing positive results (Avert.org 2004). Nonetheless, the ravages of AIDS over a period of 20+ years
clearly have extracted a huge human cost. Life expectancy in Uganda hovers between 40-45 years (43
male, 45 female) (CultureGrams 2004). The social cost to families and the large number of orphans
whose parents have been lost to AIDS create major challenges.
318
Women and Development
As in any country, women have an integral role to play in Uganda’s development. “Since women form
the majority of the Ugandan poor, it means that they are more vulnerable to the effects of poverty than
their male counterparts” (Bainomugisha 1999). According to Bainomugisha (1999), women in Uganda
have always been the main actors in the development process, though their contributions have not always
been recognized. Snyder (2000) argues that women’s substantial cash and in-kind contributions to the
national and household economy remain inadequately recognized in such key documents as the national
budget.
From a historical perspective, some efforts have been made to increase the visibility of women’s roles in
Uganda. The Uganda Council of Women was formed in 1966 for the purpose of representing women’s
groups and clubs and articulating their rights and interests, but this group was not very effective.
Women’s political participation increased with the NRM government, which took power in 1986; more
women began to be selected for senior government positions, the first female judges were appointed, and
by 1988 the Directorate of Women’s Affairs was established to coordinate women’s issues. More women
were elected to office; as of 1999, 16 percent of parliamentarians were women and 16 percent of District
Resident Commissioners (RDCs) were women (Bainomugisha 1999).
Since so much of Uganda’s economy is concentrated in the agricultural sector, the contributions of
women and children may tend to be invisible, as they are workers on family farms. Snyder (2000)
characterizes women’s role as significant in agriculture and in micro and small enterprises. “Women
owners are more likely than men to be working at home or by the roadside; their enterprises survive
longer than those of men. Women’s tenure of premises is likely to be less secure than that of men, and
women are more often found in semi-permanent, temporary and roof-only structures. Men are more
likely than women to use banks . . . [and to] belong to formal business support groups, while women are
more prone to informal ones” (Snyder 2000, p. 9). She summarizes women’s critical role in Uganda’s
economic growth on the following bases:
•
Agriculture, the main source of GDP, is a female-intensive sector of production.
•
Women comprise ‘perhaps the majority’ of all entrepreneurs in the informal micro- and
small-enterprise economy—the fastest growing job-creation sector in Africa, accounting
for up to 40 percent of GDP.
•
Women reproduce the labor force both as mothers and increasingly as providers of a
significant portion of total family income, estimated at 50 percent in two-spouse
households and 100 percent in the 29 percent of households where women are sole
providers.
•
Allocations of income show clear gender differences. Men tend to first retain a share of
income for personal use, while women’s earnings are invested first of all in child and
family requirements, including schooling and health care, then in expanding their
businesses.
•
Uganda’s persistent poverty underlies the urgent need to broaden the economic base.
Women’s preferential investment in nutrition and schooling of the young confirm that
poverty cannot be eliminated without them.
•
Women entrepreneurs offer great potential for the simultaneous increase of people’s
well-being and growth of the country’s wealth (Snyder 2000, pp. 10-11).
While women’s political clout was slow in coming, Snyder (2000) found that women’s entrepreneurship
grew substantially during the Amin regime, when men either ran away or were killed. Some women
acquired a goat or cow to get milk or money to care for their children; others took over running large
businesses in their husband’s absence. Where whole families went into exile, women either found paid
employment or something to sell, many returning to Uganda with their experience and equipment in
319
which they’d invested. “They did not want to come back with just the Bible they went with” (Snyder
2000, p. 21).
Despite the great involvement of Ugandan women in development, disparities between men and women
remain. According to the United Nations Development Program’s 1997 report on human development in
Uganda, there is still a great dichotomy between women and men with regard to access to productive
resources, poverty levels, education, employment opportunities and participation in the political process.
While the preponderance of the agricultural labor force is women (70 percent) and the bulk of food
production (70-80 percent) is undertaken by women, only 7 percent of women own land. Gender
disparities cut across all social indicators, capability indices and indicators of political and economic
participation (United Nations 1998). So while Uganda’s economy overall is improving, women’s
economic empowerment lags behind. As Sen points out (in Agarwal 2003), “The poor do not have to
actually get poorer for the distribution system to be thoroughly unjust; the issue is the relative sharing of
gains, not the presence of an actual loss from the prevailing system of interrelations” (p. 327).
Based on her study of women in Uganda, Snyder (2000) presents a development model based on four
strategies: personal empowerment, factors of production, institutions, and national and local policies.
These strategies will be revisited later in this paper.
Marketing and Development
Issues related to development are integral to macromarketing. As Farmer (1987) demonstrates, marketing
has a critical role to play in a country’s social and economic development. Development is a crucial goal
for Uganda (Museveni 1999). With GDP per capita purchasing power parity estimated at U.S. $1,400
(CIA 2004) and a Human Development Index ranking of 146 among 177 countries (United Nations
Development Programme 2004), Uganda certainly is one of the poorest countries in the world. Based on
Shultz, Belk, and Ger’s (Shultz 2001) depiction of haves and have-nots in the global economy, clearly
Uganda is a country of have-nots; and its trend of uneven development discussed earlier characterizes
Uganda as a country of have-nots with a small elite of haves concentrated in the Kampala area.
In examining what role macromarketers can play in identifying the problems and (ideally) reducing the
suffering of poverty-stricken people in developing countries, research into the structural issues of
marketing systems in such countries is a beginning. Shultz (1997) advocates multiple-method field work
in war-torn transition economies as a methodology for gaining greater understanding of the problems and
the players involved. He demonstrated this approach as a means of enabling macromarketers to collect
data needed to effect policy change and enhance life quality for consumers in Vietnam, Cambodia,
Croatia, and Bosnia-Herzegovina. Ultimately, as noted by Fisk (1967), improving life quality and
standards of living is the goal of macromarketing research. Uganda’s situation seems comparable to these
countries, based on its history under the ruthless despot Amin, the continuing struggle for peace, and the
refugees absorbed from Rwanda’s civil war.
A further issue related to development is the role of women in a society. The United Nations
Development Programme (2004) measures the capabilities of women in a country by examining women’s
participation in political and economic decision making. In UNDP’s 2004 report, Uganda’s genderrelated development index (GDI) was ranked 113 out of 144 countries, placing it fairly low in gender
development. In terms of seats in parliament held by women, Uganda’s parliament is 24.7 percent
women (about the same as Switzerland) and ranked 28th, contrasted with Sweden with 45.3 percent and
ranked first. In estimated female-earned income (in PPP US$), Uganda ranks 124th with PPP US$1,088,
but 25th in its ratio of female-earned income to male-earned income at 66 percent. As discussed earlier,
women’s contributions to earning may be less visible than men’s (Snyder 2000).
320
METHODOLOGY
This study was conducted as an exploratory analysis to identify issues of women and development in
Uganda from a macromarketing perspective. The author, in Uganda to conduct workshops for women
interested in running for public office, capitalized on this opportunity by collecting data. Data were
collected through observation, field notes and interviews, as advocated by Shultz (1997). In addition,
feedback and comments from 47 workshop participants provided further insights.
Results
A number of themes emerge from this research related to women and development in Uganda. These
emergent themes are explicated below, grouped as positive factors contributing to development and as
challenges or needs that hamper development.
Factors Contributing to Development
Women’s support networks and organizations. Because of the nature of our project in Uganda, we
were in a unique position to observe a number of women’s organizations. It should be noted that, since
we were sponsored by the League of Women Voters of the U.S. and our workshops related to women’s
role in grassroots democracy, our hosts made a concerted effort to show us these types of groups. The
initiatives of several of these organizations were quite impressive. A few specific examples include:
•
Acfode (Action for Development). This organization, whose purpose is advocacy for
gender equality and equity, has been touted for its role in furthering women’s place in
Ugandan society (Snyder 2000). Acfode’s approach is to create an enabling
environment, identifying causes of gender inequality and inequity; build capacity for
women and girls by facilitating their acquisition of skills (such as through scholarship
and training programs); and empower women and girls to advocate for their rights
(Acfode 2002). Its publications provide practical assistance to women participating in
leadership positions in their communities.
•
FIDA, an organization for women attorneys. This group, having grown from five at its
inception to a membership of 240 at the time of our visit, provides professional
development for women lawyers, but it also provides free legal aid services to women
and children as a means of empowering vulnerable groups.
•
Women’s Education Centre, Kampala. Founded as a Christian Vocational School by the
Diocese of Kampala, the Women’s Education Centre offers vocational training in the
areas of: tailoring and designing, home economics, food science and technology,
computer basics, English language for beginners, and needlework, knitting and weaving.
It also provides a day care center and nursery school. More recently, a school for AIDS
orphans has been created there to help children who have been out of school catch up; the
children are mainstreamed into the public school system once they are working at their
appropriate grade level. Another program, “Feed My Lambs,” works with the World
Food Program to provide food for children.
•
Bulenga Women’s Family Promotion Association, Kampala. This women’s association
is representative of regional groups in Uganda. It provides networking and a women’s
support group; also, women members work together to manufacture craft products and
market them.
•
Family Health Improvement Program. A number of education and information initiatives
are developed to improve family health. Some we witnessed are the Moringa Tree
321
project, aimed at planting moringa trees (known as “miracle trees”), which provide many
benefits for multiple purposes through their bark, leaves, flowers, pods, and seeds; the
feast stove, a more fuel-efficient way of providing hot meals for the family; and the tippy
tap project, designed to teach families sanitation methods attainable without clean
running water. As the primary caregivers in their families, women are the main targets of
these programs.
Though we did not visit them, there are a number of other women’s support organizations in Uganda,
including: Women Engineers, Technicians and Scientists in Uganda (WETSU); Association of Uganda
Women Medical Doctors (AUWMD); Uganda Women’s Finance Trust (UWFT); Uganda Women’s
Media Association (UMWA); National Association of Women’s Organizations in Uganda (NAWOU);
and Uganda Women Parliamentarians Association (UWOPA). The Uganda Women’s Network
(UWONET) coordinates among these various groups to advocate and lobby for women’s issues
(UWONET 1999).
Women’s business initiatives. Besides the Bulenga Women’s Association mentioned in the previous
section, we observed a number of business initiatives led by women. A common theme was agricultural
projects. We visited heifer, goat, chicken and pig projects begun by women. Monica, our informant who
began the heifer project with her husband, said their motto is “send a cow”! The initiator gives a calf to
another, who raises it and gives its offspring to another, and so on, to provide meat, milk, and income
among an expanding network of families. Among workshop participants, a large number wanted to learn
about marketing so they could begin or expand similar home businesses; the motivation they most often
cited was to provide for their families, with particular emphasis on paying school fees for their children so
they can be educated. A very comprehensive women’s project we visited was the Komamboga Women’s
Project, wherein several types of animals and crops are grown, in addition to nutritional training, brick
making, and a model home demonstrating good sanitation methods.
Involvement in life improvement. Several of the foregoing examples relate to life improvement efforts.
Besides those with a particular women’s focus, there are others to which women contribute:
•
African Centre for the Treatment and Rehabilitation of Torture Victims (ACTV). The
ACTV, founded by a doctor and his wife, was created to ease the suffering of victims of
torture and to intervene on their behalf in obtaining legal recourse where possible. The
people they assist are primarily victims of police or military torture. The organization
relies on physicians and attorneys to donate their time to the cause of assisting torture
victims.
•
Kampala School for the Deaf. The founder of this school, who began on a shoestring
budget in a facility that is renovated one room at a time, told a moving story explaining
her motivations in starting the school. As in many cultures, there is a struggle for the
acceptance of disabled people. Further, because of the enormous task of trying to
implement universal primary education in a country where historically a large portion of
children did not receive education, the facilities and programs for those with special
needs are not nearly adequate for the demand. Based on the founder’s compassion for
children left out of traditional schools, the Kampala School for the Deaf was begun for
deaf children between the ages of 7 and 14.
Women’s political involvement and participation. As noted earlier, women comprise nearly 25
percent of Uganda’s parliament. In addition to seats gained in district elections, a few parliamentary seats
are set aside for representatives of women, youth, and disabled citizens—so women are guaranteed a
nominal representation.
322
Besides the national parliament, Uganda has a decentralized system of local councils to ensure local
representation and participation. The local council levels are labeled LC1 through LC5. LC1 councils are
for villages and cells, LC2 for parishes and wards, LC3 for sub-counties and divisions, LC4 for counties
and municipalities, and LC5 for districts and cities (Acfode 2000). Among our workshop participants,
virtually all were elected representatives on local councils, primarily in the LC1 to LC3 levels; and these
47 women represented just a small portion of the country, surrounding Jinja, east of Kampala, and the
Wakiso district, west of Kampala. A recurring theme among women in and out of our workshops was
the drive to make life better in Uganda. Many expressed interests in community projects targeting such
issues as literacy and education, health and sanitation, and hunger and food provision. Granted, the
women with whom we had contact were likely to be a more activist segment than the average Ugandan
woman. Given that caveat, however, there was a definite air of optimism and the feeling that Uganda is
on the move and that women in Uganda play a critical role in this movement.
Anti-corruption forces. One problem many developing countries face is corruption, which not only
creates inefficiencies in the economy but saps resources and keeps aid from reaching its intended
recipients. While Uganda is certainly not without corruption, we observed evidence of action against
corruption. In our visit to Acfode headquarters, a banner proclaimed it a “corruption-free zone”, bringing
this issue into the open. When we visited parliament, we had the opportunity to listen to two debates;
both centered on issues of accountability, and in both cases an individual was questioned about his use of
resources.
A third example related to corruption was our personal experience in Uganda in contrast to the
experiences of colleagues on the same project who visited other countries in Africa. We representatives
to Uganda were never once asked to pay any kind of bribe or grease money. We traveled freely in the
country (with a local driver) and never faced any problems that might illustrate corruption. In
comparison, other teams reported demands for bribes, some under rather terrifying circumstances (Nigeria
provided a particularly bad example). The contrast made Uganda appear indeed to be a corruption-free
zone!
The contributions of NGOs. While in Uganda, we crossed paths with a number of representatives of
nongovernmental organizations, there to provide assistance and aid to Ugandans. These included medical
professionals and representatives of micro-loan organizations, both providing much-needed services.
Snyder (2000) attests to the increasing number of micro-financing organizations operating in Uganda,
making capital more available to women. And, as Leggett (2001) points out, Uganda continues to depend
heavily on foreign aid, and this aid comes from foreign governments and from NGOs. An observer in
Kampala sees constant evidence of foreign NGOs on signs and offices.
Challenges to Development
Women’s struggle for equality and equity. The fact that organizations such as Acfode and FIDA have
missions of empowering women illustrates that women do not enjoy equal power yet in Ugandan society.
One poster which caught this observer’s eye may help to shed light on the context in which this struggle
takes place: “Say no to polygamy!”. Polygamy is practiced not only among the 16 percent of Ugandans
who are Muslim (CIA 2004), but also among Christians and those who practice traditional African
religion. Women in Uganda are faced not just with questions of self-fulfillment and economic equity
amidst abundance, as perhaps some of their sisters in affluent societies are, but also with issues of life and
death, survival and extreme poverty, abduction, rape and torture, rampant AIDS and HIV. This statement
is not made to trivialize the struggles of women in rich societies for full equality; it is intended to draw
attention to the dire circumstances under which many Ugandan women and girls live and the tenuous
grasp many have on any sort of equality. Until some of the structural issues in Ugandan society can be
addressed, the plight of women is not likely to improve substantially.
323
Conflict and terrorism. As noted earlier, no resolution has been made of the LRA insurgencies in the
northern part of Uganda. As visitors, we stayed in the southern part of the country near Kampala with
little risk of danger. But upon our arrival at the airport in Entebbe, we were warned by American military
representatives not to venture into the northern part of the country because of the unrest there. The
continuing conflict is a factor which hinders development and poses particular dangers for women and
children (see McKay and Mazurana 2004).
AIDS/HIV and other health issues. While Uganda’s battle with AIDS is touted as a relative success
story (CultureGrams 2004), AIDS and HIV have taken a huge toll on Ugandan lives. This observer’s
visit to a school for AIDS orphans in Kampala served as a stark reminder of the tragic costs being passed
to the next generation. The children’s recitation of a poem about AIDS provided heart-wrenching
evidence of the devastating human effects of this disease. The deaths of parents, displacement and, in
some cases, infection of children will have long-term effects on Ugandan society and extract both
economic and social costs.
With respect to health care in general, risks for typhoid fever, malaria, trypanosomiasis, and
schistosomiasis are rated very high (CIA 2004). Access to medical care is quite limited; Leggett (2001)
reports a doctor-to-patient ratio of 1:25,000. Coupled with the problem of poor sanitation and lack of
clean water supplies, it is no wonder that life expectancy is about 45 years (CIA 2004). Foreign aid and
NGO assistance help in these areas, but health care remains a major challenge.
Imbalances in development. Uganda exhibits imbalances in development in several ways. As discussed
earlier, women and children tend to be disproportionately affected by poverty (Bainomugisha 1999).
Uganda’s economic dualism is also quite evident in comparing south to north, the fertile crescent
surrounding Kampala with the more drought-ridden lands in the northern part of the country, and in
comparing the Kampala urban area with rural parts of the country (Leggett 2001). Kampala, a modern
city with world-class amenities, exists in sharp contrast to much of the country, where paved roads,
running water (or safe, clean water), basic sanitation and electricity are lacking.
Educational differences. Uganda is making strides in the area of education. According to CIA (2004)
estimates, 69.9 percent of Ugandan adults (ages 15+) can read and write; literacy among men is 79.5
percent and among women 60.4 percent. The Universal Primary Education law is an ambitious attempt to
make education available to all children. But it should be noted that this law provides free education (no
school fees) to four children per family (Leggett 2001). In a country where fertility rates average 6.64
children per woman (CIA 2004), this still leaves some children out. Traditionally, the trend has been to
educate males over females. The inroads of Acfode and other organizations may be changing this
situation somewhat, but clearly much progress is desired in the area of education to meet the needs of all
Ugandans.
We observed huge differences in the school facilities for wealthy students versus poor. Most schools
struggle for basic resources and equipment and usable classroom space; such was the case in most schools
we visited. One workshop participant set the goal of planting an acre of cassava at every school. Her
reasoning was that the children would learn a useful skill in growing the cassava, and that it would
provide food, because it’s hard for children to concentrate on their studies when they are hungry.
Mechanisms for training and assistance. One of the positive forces discussed earlier was the women’s
support networks and business initiatives. The flip side of this coin is that many women with home-based
business and/or agricultural operations need assistance in understanding how to market their products. In
fact, business assistance in general appears to be a major need for female entrepreneurs. As noted earlier,
men tend to have more access to capital (Snyder 2000), giving them an advantage in starting businesses;
even with increased availability of funding through micro-financing organizations, capitalization is a
324
barrier for many women. Moreover, the nature and size of women’s businesses may have further
disadvantages in identifying and reaching target markets. The women in our workshops overwhelmingly
wanted to learn basics about marketing, both to aid in marketing themselves as political candidates and to
help with their own businesses and community-based projects. Many of these women were subsidized
for the workshops, so higher education is a goal beyond their dreams. The major theme observed is a
need for practical training to be made available to women with families to assist them in their business
endeavors.
Marketing channels. In addition to the need for basic marketing skills, there appears to be a lack of
adequate marketing channels for Ugandan women’s products to reach target markets. Since so many of
these businesses are very low-budget operations, the focus tends to be on production and not on
distribution. As women make the transition from raising or making things for the subsistence of their
families to producing for external markets, a better understanding is needed of how to identify target
markets, assess their needs, and find ways of reaching those markets.
Discussion
The themes emerging from this research might be summarized through this characterization: women are
actively involved in development in Uganda, on economic, political and social fronts. Through the
leadership of women in organizations such as Acfode, women are gaining empowerment and leadership
skills to aid their causes. Moreover, leadership of both men and women at top levels of government is
assisting in creating opportunities for women. Even so, much of the Ugandan economy remains in
subsistence farming, and many women are struggling to eke out the financial means, not only to feed their
children, but to buy them school uniforms and books and to pay secondary school fees so that their
children can be educated.
In terms of challenges to development, women’s struggle for equality and equity is ongoing, but progress
is being made. The same might be said of the AIDS/HIV issue; the situation is improving, although the
effects will be felt for a long time. More troubling perhaps is the conflict and terrorism plaguing the
northern part of Uganda, with no solution in sight at this time. Imbalances in development call for greater
attention to the needs in rural areas. It is this observer’s impression that this problem is recognized, but
that there is so much to be done that the easier things, focusing in the region near Kampala, are being
done first. Educational differences remain, but here too, progress is being made. The purpose of
Universal Primary Education is to address these differences. UPE requires that at least two of the four
children per family must be girls (Snyder 2000). Families still must pay for school uniforms, books, and
other necessities, though the government is looking into providing primary school books. So progress is
being made through UPE, though poor families still face hardships, and there is no free secondary
education.
The last two challenges identified are most directly related to macromarketing: the need for mechanisms
for training and assistance, especially in the areas of marketing and entrepreneurship, and the need for
marketing channel development. In talking with elected officials and university representatives, it
appears that university resources in Uganda that might be suited for this training, particularly the business
school at Makerere University, are stretched thin in trying to meet burgeoning demand. Partnerships
between higher education institutions in Uganda and other countries may be one solution to providing the
needed expertise. In fact, structurally, the cooperative extension service in the U.S. might provide a
model for assisting people in both agricultural and other types of small businesses. A fruitful direction
for macromarketers might be in developing such a partnership and designing requisite training programs.
Further study is needed to develop a clearer understanding of the marketing channel issue. Informants
expressed a need for understanding how to sell their products; this need may well indicate a much broader
range of needs, relating to a host of marketing functions, such as product quality, identifying target
325
market segments and assessing their needs and wants, pricing, communication, and distribution issues.
Much of this is likely beyond the scope of the individual, home-based business, and may suggest a need
for cooperatives or other organizations that can take on these functions for groups of entrepreneurs. The
creation of intermediaries to provide expertise and service to small businesses in marketing their products
may be one approach. Perhaps the women’s associations provide a framework to explore as a means of
fulfilling this need. What is clear from these findings is that many small-business operators recognize
that they need help, and further study is required to clarify the nature of help that would be most
beneficial.
Conclusions
Uganda is a beautiful country of warm, gracious people. It is a country making strides forward despite
the hardships its people have endured and continue to endure. From the perspective of this author,
macromarketing has an important role to play in bringing Uganda out of the cycle of poverty that grips
much of the population.
This research, exploratory in nature, certainly has limitations, but is intended only as a first step in
identifying some of the issues related to women and development in Uganda and the role macromarketing
might play in addressing these issues. Based on the findings of this preliminary study, it appears that
macromarketing perspectives may be valuable in understanding structural problems faced by small
business owners in Uganda and in finding solutions to these problems. Further research is needed to
focus on these market structure issues, so that marketing may indeed fulfill its potential as a mechanism
for bringing development and a better standard of living to the poor in Uganda.
REFERENCES
Acfode. 2000. Leadership: guidelines for women local councillors. Kampala: Action for Development.
Acfode. 2002. Strategy 2002-2004. Brochure published by Action for Development.
Agarwal, B. 2003. Continuing the conversation: Amartya Sen Ttlks with Bina Agarwal, Jane
Humphries, and Ingrid Robeyns. Feminist Economics 9(2-3): 319-332.
Avert.org. 2004. HIV and AIDS in Uganda. (December 2): http://www.avert.org/aidsuganda.htm.
Bainomugisha, A. 1999. The empowerment of women. In Uganda’s age of reforms: a critical overview,
rev. ed., edited by J. Mugaju, 89-102. Kampala: Fountain Publishers Ltd.
CIA. 2004. The world factbook (December 16): http://www.cia.gov/cia/publications/factbook.
Clark, M. 2004. In the spotlight: the Lord’s Resistance Army (LRA). Center for defense information
(October 27): http://www.cdi.org.
CultureGrams. 2004. Republic of Uganda. Lindon, UT: Axiom Press, Inc., 165-168.
Europa World Yearbook. 2004. 45th ed. Uganda. London, England: Europa Publications Limited.
Farmer, R. N. 1987. Would you want your granddaughter to marry a Taiwanese marketing man? Journal
of Marketing 51(4) (October): 111-116.
Fisk, G. 1967. Marketing systems: an introductory analysis. New York: Harper and Row.
International Herald Tribune. 2004. Uganda: focusing on the future (sponsored section) (October 2-3).
326
Leggett, I. 2001. Uganda. Kampala: Oxfam GB.
McKay, S. and D. Mazurana. 2004. Where are the girls? Montreal: Rights and Democracy.
Museveni, Y. K. 1999. Foreword. In Uganda’s age of reforms: a critical overview, rev. ed., edited by J.
Mugaju, iv-v. Kampala: Fountain Publishers Ltd.
Shultz, C. J., II. 1997. Improving life quality for the destitute: contributions from multiple-method
fieldwork in war-ravaged transition economies. Journal of Macromarketing 17(1) (Spring): 5667.
Shultz, C. J., II. 2001. Have-not’s in a world of haves: disenfranchised nations and their consumers in an
increasingly affluent and global world. Advances in Consumer Research 28: 277-279.
Snyder, M. 2000. Women in African economies: from burning sun to boardroom. Kampala: Fountain
Publishers Ltd.
United Nations. 1998. Human development report. New York: Oxford University Press.
United Nations Development Programme. 2004. Human development report 2004.
http://www.undp.org/.
UWONET. 1999. Uganda women’s network annual report. Kampala: UWONET.
World Bank. 2003. World development indicators. Washington, DC: World Bank.
WPR Special Correspondent. 2003. My country’s anguish. World Press Review 50(11) (November): 20-22.
327
__________________________________________________
Relationship Marketing – Competitive Strategy or
Destruction of the Market?
Anthony Pecotich, University of Western Australia
Russel Kingshott, North Shore Mail Centre, New Zealand
Abstract
In this study we redefined the concept of relationship marketing, equated it to the Chinese cultural
framework of guanxi and attempt to conceptually relate it to the market system.
Introduction
The so called “relationship marketing” paradigm dominates modern marketing practice and scholarship.
Its novelty is emphasized despite the recognition that the ideas are not new and that society, as we know
it, has always consisted of a network of relationships (Berry 2002; Gummesson 2002; Palmer 2002; Sheth
and Parvatiyar 2002). Research has proliferated and many grandiose claims have been made for the
paradigm’s appearance as a new challenge to “traditional” marketing practice and scholarship (e.g.,
Brodie et al. 1997; Gummesson 2002). Indeed, Gummesson (2002, p. 38) using the sociology of science
arguments of Kuhn (1962) almost hints at a conspiracy to restrict the progress of this new “relationship
marketing” paradigm as follows:
As is characteristic of paradigm shifts, mainstream researchers, educators, and practitioners--who
are in majority--attempt to force those who deviate from standard thinking--who are in minority—
to adhere to the rules and regulations of ‘‘normal science’’ and to squeeze new thinking into the old
costume. This resistance slows down and partly distorts the introduction of new marketing
behavior.
He states this, disregarding Popper’s (1959) arguments for scientific progress by critical destruction, and
notwithstanding the evidence which suggests, for example, by the acknowledged proliferation of research,
the use of the term in practice and even the existence of a journal of its own, that the paradigm has been,
if anything, too readily and uncritically accepted in marketing (e.g., Berry 2002; Brodie et al. 1997;
Gummesson 2002; Hunt et al. 2002; Palmer 2002; Sheth and Parvatiyar 2002). Indeed the literature and
the research is plagued by imprecision, definitional problems (as recognized by Gummesson (2002, p.
39)), the general poor specification of the nature of the paradigm and its philosophical implications for the
economic functioning of industries or nations. Further, it appears that the psychological and sociological
literature as well as the historical basis of trade in networks of human relationships most recently
extensively emphasized in the examination of Chinese traditional values and labeled as Guanxi has been
largely overlooked (e.g., Berscheid 1994; Ellis and Pecotich 2001a, 2001b, 2002; Lovett et al. 1999;
Pecotich and Chia 2002; Seung and Luo 2001; Xin and Pearce 1996). It is our purpose to make a
preliminary attempt to clarify some of these issues and on this basis to question its philosophical value as
a strategy leading to the Macromarketing goal of human welfare.
Relationship Marketing Defined
In the western (particularly the US and European) literature there has been a explosion of definitions of
relationships marketing without clear agreement as to its nature and or specification of its context or level
328
of application (e.g., Grönroos 1997, 2000; Gummesson 2002; Morgan and Hunt 1994; Parvatiyar and
Sheth 2000). The term has been used to describe relationships both at the individual and at the
organizational level with terms such as: “affinity marketing, loyalty marketing, cross selling, up selling,
co-branding, co-marketing, customer-supplier partnering, personalized one-to-one relationship key
account management and solution selling” (Sheth and Parvatiyar 2002, p. 10), and there has been a failure
to clearly separate, enumerate and integrate the two elements (i.e. relationships and marketing). Recently
Gummesson (1999, p. 24; 2002, p. 39) has emphasized the long term nature of the relationship and taken
a very broad view by referring to:
Total relationship marketing is marketing based on relationships, networks and interaction,
recognizing that marketing is embedded in the total management of the networks of the selling
organization, the market and society. It is directed to long term win-win relationships with
individual customers, and value is jointly created between the parties involved. It transcends the
boundaries between specialist functions and disciplines. Total relationship marketing embraces
not just the supplier-customer dyad as does one-to-one marketing and CRM (customer
relationship management) but also relationships to a supplier’s own suppliers, to competitors and
to middlemen; these are all market relationships.
Sheth and Parvatiyar (2002, p. 10) elaborate further by suggesting that: (1) “it is a one-to-one relationship
between the marketer and the customer (cannot exist at an aggregate level); (2) it is an interactive process
and not a transaction exchange; and (3) it is a value-added activity through mutual interdependence and
collaboration, between suppliers and customers. The picture is somewhat completed when one notes the
emphasis on “mutual exchange and fulfillment of promises” by Grönroos, (1990, p. 7). Relationship
marketing may therefore be viewed “as a very broad type of relatively long term individual human
business relationship involving a degree of collaboration and interdependence, which is based on the
expectation of a fulfillment of promises in interactive marketing exchanges.”
It is noteworthy that this attempt at enumeration has not fully resolved the issues as the two key terms – marketing
and relationships – still appear on both sides of the definitional equation and the equation is consequently
tautological. It is, therefore, necessary to define both the key terms before proceeding further. Issues associated with
the “nature and scope of marketing” have undergone extensive debate and have to large extent been resolved (Hunt
1976b, 1976a, 1983) and it is not essential for us to revisit this matter but rather take it as given. The nature of
relationships, however, is more central to the purposes of our study and needs to be enumerated. The American
Heritage Dictionary (Anonymous 1992) defines a relationship as: (1) The condition or fact of being related connection or association; (2) connection by blood or marriage, kinship; (3) A particular type of connection existing
between people related to or having dealings with each other: has a close relationship with his sibling; and (4) A
romantic or sexual involvement. There are, therefore, many kinds of relationships but they all involve a connection
or association or to elaborate “a mental connection or relation between thoughts, feelings, ideas, or sensations - a
remembered or imagined feeling, emotion, idea, or sensation linked to a person, object, or idea”(Anonymous 1992).
Our interpretation of this and the related psychological literature (Berscheid 1994) is that a degree of emotional
commitment and intimacy is necessary, and that process and content issues are involved. In the relationship
marketing context the strategic prescriptions essentially specify the processes necessary to establish the relationship the
nature of which is the content. An interaction between persons can be conceptualized in terms of their needs, thoughts,
and motives in relation to one another in the context of the specific social (marketing) situation in which their
interaction transpires (Rusbult and Van Lange 2003, p. 352).
Thus, a major purpose of relationship marketing is to establish corresponding interests that minimize
information-seeking and that are unlikely to present moral dilemmas, but rather through coordinating
activities ensure mutual beneficial outcomes. To rephrase, relationship marketing is “a very broad type of
relatively long term, voluntary, individual, human business association involving a degree of emotional
commitment, trust, intimacy, collaboration and interdependence, which is based on the expectation of a
reciprocal, fulfillment of promises in interactive marketing exchanges for mutual benefit.”
329
Relationship Marketing and Chinese, Interpersonal Connections - Guanxi
Given this emphasis on relationship marketing in much of the western literature it is somewhat surprising
that very little attention has been paid to the Chinese variant of the concept. Labeled simply as guanxi it
basically means a relationship. However, its implications are broad, complex and may be culture bound.
The Chinese culture is deceptively varied and although built on a foundation of over 5,000 years it has
undergone unmatched social and cultural transformation (Bond and Hwang 1986; Chan 1963a, 1963b;
Chen 1973; Chen 2001; Cheng and Chan 2000; Wilson et al. 1979). Besides the historical transformations
(e.g., wars and political changes) three influences have shaped the Chinese culture, as it exists at the
beginning of this millennium: the traditional values, Marxism and Mao’s philosophy, and the pragmatic
approach of Deng (Pecotich and Yang 2001). The most enduring of these influences, and indeed the one
that has been integrated into the Chinese psyche is the complex amalgam labeled “traditional Chinese
values” (Pecotich and Chia 2002). The traditional values in turn have been shaped by Confucianist, Taoist,
Buddhist and Feudalistic philosophies (Zheng 1997). It is these ancient sources that form the basis of
personal associations referred to as a guanxi or networks of informal relationships and exchanges
of favors that dominate business (Yang 1994). The nature of these relationships although
postulated to have significant differences to western business practices have surprising similarities
to the notions advocated by the proponents of relationship marketing
Guanxi as practiced has both prescriptions for relationship formation and an understanding of the end
result (content) of such relationships. For example, although our concern is with “content” the
“process” prescription for network formation and expansion is to find an intermediary who
introduces a newcomer to a stranger, and who establishes the trustworthiness of the prospective
partner (Lovett et al. 1999). The relationship may be solidified by a skillful exchange of favors
and gift giving. Similarly to relationship marketing this is not a simple exchange, transaction
or barter but a longterm “relationship in which the participants quickly begin to cement their ties
through exchanges of respect and affection, as well as material objects or specific favors” (Lovett
et al. 1999, p. 233). Guanxi involves a subtle use of informal power within which a great emphasis is
placed on face (mianzi). Although associated with Western terms like "prestige" or "reputation" it is a
broader concept that can be defined as "the respectability and/or deference which a person can claim for
himself from others" (Ho 1976, p.883) or simply "a positive public image" (Hsu 1996, p. 71).
In China face is a social phenomenon which refers to two separate but related concepts: lian is the
confidence of society in a person's moral character, while mianzi represents social perceptions of a
person's personal prestige, reputation or esteem. As face translates into power and influence it is
important to avoid its loss. For example, loss of lian would result in a loss of trust, while a loss of mianzi
may result in a loss of authority (Lovett et al. 1999). Given that the Chinese value orientation is directed
at social harmony and work excellence respectively conflict avoidance is a common cultural characteristic
(Goffman 1955; Ho 1976; Hsu 1996). Face, therefore, is an important regulating force in social
interactions. Guanxi members are connected through an unwritten code of reciprocity and equity. Renqing
a form of social capital represents an informal expectation of reciprocity which if not met compromises
the guanxi network. A person who does not reciprocate is viewed as untrustworthy. Trust in turn is
postulated to consist of three entities; integrity, benevolence, and ability (Mayer et al. 1995). All
three must be present for a successful relationship.
In attempting to establish East-West differences writers have disregarded the relationship marketing
prescriptions, for example: (1) both are humanistic in their approach deemphasizing the impersonal
and universalistic quality of the Protestant ethic; (2) rather than focusing on any particular
transaction both are concerned with long-term pattern of behaviors grounded in ethical norms
rather then the discrete-transaction paradigm based on classical western contract law; and (3)
both believe that trust, commitment and loyalty and not a conflict oriented legal system will be
330
the basis of the “new” economy (Gummesson 2002; Lovett et al. 1999; Yang 1994; Yao 2000).
So to reiterate while some tend to emphasize subtle difference using the inappropriate broad
brush eastern-western dichotomy, the above discussion suggests that guanxi is also a very broad
type of relatively long term, voluntary, individual, human business association involving a degree of
emotional commitment, trust, intimacy, collaboration and interdependence, which is based on the
expectation of a reciprocal, fulfillment of promises in interactive marketing exchanges for a mutual
benefit. Hence, guanxi, an ancient system of relationship formation and management, may have universal
application and is simply a variation of modern marketing prescriptions labeled as relationship marketing.
The Market System, Competition, Guanxi and Relationship Marketing
Since the collapse of the Eastern block and the end of progressivism the transforming and developing
countries of the world have moved towards a market-based economies and the ideological debate between
socialism and capitalism seems to have paused (Nee 1989, 1992; Pecotich et al. 1994; Pecotich and Shultz
1998; Shultz and Pecotich 1997). Nonetheless, fundamental questions concerning the philosophical basis
of the market system, competition and social justice remain (Chua 2003; Easterly 2001; Krugman 1997).
With particular reference to this paper – in marketing we have the adoption of a “new” paradigm of
relationship marketing (e.g. as far back as 1990 Heide and John have suggested that relationship
marketing is a fait accompli (Heide and John 1990)) without any discussion or enumeration of its impact
on the competitively based market system. This is despite its growing importance as a western business
practices and its position as a virtual clone of the dominant Chinese cultural business practice guanxi. The advocates of relationship marketing and guanxi abound and arguments are presented for
their benefits in terms of social and economic efficiency, and this “cooperative” approach is presented as
superior to the “adversarial” market system (e.g. Berry 2002; Grönroos 1990, 1997, 2000; Gummesson
1999, 2002; Klein et al. 1994; Lovett et al. 1999; Sheth and Parvatiyar 2002). Lovett, Simmons and Kali
(1999, p. 243) in particular argue that “western businesses which are able to conduct transactions through
trust-based agreements and long-term net-works of relationships will increasingly out compete those
using traditional contracting methods.” The crux of their argument rests on the reduction of broadly
defined transactions costs that are postulated to consist of “the opportunity costs resulting from the loss
of flexibility” as well as “the legal costs of making and enforcing contracts.” Their position is conditional
on the extent to which the new system is able to “rapidly accept new exchange partners on the basis of
ability.” Their argument loose force when one notes that it is based on a future postulated change in the
relationship structure, and in that they seem to disregard the long standing argument for the advantages of
the market system and the basis of anti-trust (Hayek von 1960; Lindblom 1977, 2001; Porter 1980, 1985;
Scherer 1980; Von Mises 1963). Whatever the future may hold as Lovett, Simmons and Kali (1999, p.
243) later recognize the present emphasis is on loyalty and “(t)he result is that talented individuals are
either suffocated or squeezed out of the business and that the business itself is unable to achieve growth or
diversification.”
One does not have to take an extreme position to recognize the possibility that relationship marketing and
or guanxi may be anti-competition and corruptive of the market system. The market is a process, actuated
by the interplay of the actions of the various individuals. It involves cooperation in the sense that there is
acceptance as to how the system is to operate, but this can not be collusion that subverts the market (e.g.,
Hayek von 1960; Lane 1991; Lindblom 1977, 2001; Porter 1980, 1985; Scherer 1980; Von Mises 1963).
The market is postulated to be free and prices are set through a competitive interplay without interference
from institutional forces. The market determines nature of production as well as the price and quantity of
what is produced. According to Von Mises (1963, p. 274), “(i)n the market economy, competition
manifests itself in the fact that the sellers must outdo one another by offering better or cheaper goods and
services, and that the buyers must outdo one another by offering higher prices ... It means the opportunity
to serve the consumers in a better or cheaper way without being restrained by privileges granted to those
whose vested interests the innovation hurts.” Workable competition is therefore, a vital component of the
market system leading to greater efficiency and human welfare. At the layman level it may be described
331
“as the action of endeavoring to gain what another endeavors to gain at the same time.” More formally
Hunt (2000, p., 135) has defined it as “the constant struggle among firms for comparative advantage in
resources that will yield marketplace positions of competitive advantage for some market segments and,
thereby, superior financial performance.” It seems that competition is a kind of behavior involving
“structure” (the environment in which the firm must compete) and processes. It involves the conditions
prevailing in the market in which rivals sellers try to increase their profits at one another's expense. In the
economic sense it may range from perfect competition in which numerous firms produce or sell identical
goods or services to oligopoly in which a few large sellers with substantial influence on the market
compete for business (Hunt 2000). Although the market system may well be an unreachable ideal it is
clear that it is a goal aimed at by U.S. antitrust laws as well as the philosophical basis of the reforms in
much of the transforming and developing world (e.g., Dollar and Hallward-Driemeier 2000; Koves and
Marer 1991; Lane 1991; Lindblom 2001; Lovett et al. 1999; Pecotich and Shultz 1998).
The advocates of relationship marketing and guanxi appear to have overlooked the possibility that their
prescriptions a may be interpreted as anti-market and destructive of competition. The building of long
term relationships, as defined above and almost in any form as described in the literature, changes the
marketing game to a more complex form where the critical strategic competitive forces (such as price and
quality) are subverted by the quality of a personal relationship. To the extent that it is the manifest
intention of practitioners of relationship marketing (or guanxi) to grant and gain privileges from a
deliberately developed relationship whose purpose is to restrict competitive action (e.g., freedom of entry
and exit or information) and protect both parties from more efficient competitors the economic system is
damaged. Indeed it is not a far step to suggest that the whole purpose of relationship marketing is to
subvert the market system and to injure competition by reverting to an ancient system of crony capitalism.
The possibility of this becomes clear when one notes that, for example, a contract may be granted to a
supplier within the relationship network rather than to a low bidder all other things being equal.
This violates the structure of the market system and is readily seen as a violation of the principle
of "fairness" or "equal opportunity" and is injurious to competition.
Further, recognizing that the line may in some cases be thin, a marketing system that is based on an a
mutual exchange of relationship enhancing benefits (gifts in the guanxi case) may be construed as bribery
despite the ethical basis of guanxi in Confucianism (Yang 1994). It is asserted by some that bribery is
plainly an illicit action while relationship marketing and guanxi are concerned with relationship
building, and that this is not bribery because the gain-and-loss calculation does not dominate, the
obligation is long-term and diffuse, while bribery is for an immediate and specific purpose, and some
level of emotional content or affect is a part of guanxi, but not necessarily of bribery (Chan 1963a, 1963b;
Lovett et al. 1999; Yang 1994; Yao 2000). Nonetheless, until the definition and paradigmatic issues are
clarified as well as the nature of marketing practice, it is a distinct possibility that ethical problems
abound and that the fundamental effect of relationship marketing is to reduce competition as
formulated in marketing and economics (Cravens et al. 1980; Grewal and Tansuhay 2001; Hunt
2000; Porter 1980; Scherer 1980).
Discussion and Conclusion
The purpose of this study was to link the largely western concept of relationship marketing
to the similar Chinese cultural framework guanxi and to evaluate the impact of these ideas
on the competitive structure within a developing, transforming nation. Basing our
arguments on classic economic formulations of the market system, and the possibilities of
crony capitalism, corruption and bribery we suggested a negative relationship between the existence of
relationship marketing (guanxi) and the extent of competition. We have challenged a major
marketing dogma with we hope with a cogent and well reasoned argument. It is our hope that
future research will rise to this challenge and address this critical issue with new conceptual
structures and more fully developed instrumentation.
332
REFERENCES
Anonymous (1992), The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language (Third ed.). Boston:
Houghton Mifflin Company.
Berry, Leonard L. (2002), "Relationship Marketing of Services-- Perspectives from 1983 and 2000,"
Journal of Relationship Marketing, 1 (1), 59-78.
Berscheid, Ellen (1994), "Interpersonal Relationships," Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 79-129.
Bond, M.H. and K.K. Hwang (1986), "The Social Psychology of Chinese People," in The Psychology of
the Chinese People, M. Bond, Ed. Hong Kong: Oxford University Press.
Brodie, Roderick J., Nicole E. Covielo, Richard W. Brooks, et al. (1997), "Towards a Paradigm Shift in
Marketing? An Examination of Current Marketing Practices," Journal of Marketing
Management, 13, 383-406.
Chan, W. T. (1963a), Source Book in Chiense Philosophy. Princeton, N.J: University of Hawaii Press.
---- (1963b), The Way of Lao Tzu (Tao Te Ching). New York: Bobbs-Merrill.
Chen, T.S (1973), 1001 Chinese Sayings (in Chiense). Hong Kong: The Chinese University of Hong
Kong.
Chen, Y. (2001), "Chinese Values, Health and Nursing," Journal of Advanced Nursing, 36 (2), 270-73.
Cheng, H. and K. Chan (2000), "One Country, Two Systems: Cultural Values Reflected in Chinese and
Hong Kong Television Commercials," Proceedings of the Conference of the American Academy
of Advertising, 134.
Chua, Amy (2003), World on Fire: How Exporting Free Market Democracy Breeds Ethnic Hatred and
Global Instability. NY: Doubleday.
Cravens, D. W., G. E. Hills, and R. B. Woodruff (1980), Marketing Decision Making: Concepts and
Strategy (Rev ed.). Homewood: Richard D. Irwin.
Dollar, D. and M. Hallward-Driemeier (2000), "Crisis, Adjustment, and Reform in Thailand's Industrial
Firms," The World Bank Research Observer, 15 (1), 1-22.
Easterly, William (2001), The Elusive Quest for Growth: Ecomomists' Adventures and Misadventures in
the Tropics. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Ellis, Paul and Anthony Pecotich (2001a), "Finding International Exchange Partners: The Role of Social
Ties," Global Focus, 13 (2), 121-33.
---- (2001b), "Social Factors Influencing Export Initiation in Small and Medium-Sized Enterprises,"
Journal of Marketing Research, XXXVIII, (1), 119-30.
---- (2002), "Macromarketing and International Trade: Comparative Advantage Versus Cosmopolitan
Considerations," Journal of Macromarketing, 22 (1), 32-56.
333
Goffman, Erving (1955), "On Face Work. An Analysis of Ritual Elements in Social Interaction,"
Psychiatry (18), 213-31.
Grewal, Rajdeep and Patriya Tansuhay (2001), "Building Organizational Capabilities for Managing
Economic Crisis: The Role of Market Orientation and Strategic Flexibility," The Journal of
Marketing, 65 (April 2001), 67-80.
Grönroos, Christian (1990), "Relationship Approach to Marketing in Services Contexts: The Marketing
and Organizational Behaviour Interface," Journal of Business Research, 20, 3-11.
---- (1997), "Value-Driven Relationship Marketing: From Products to Resources to Values," Journal of
Marketing Management, 13 (5), 407-20.
---- (2000), Service Management and Marketing. Chichester: Wiley.
Gummesson, Evert (1999), Total Relationship Marketing. Oxford: Butterworth Heinemann.
---- (2002), "Relationship Marketing in the New Economy," Journal of Relationship Marketing, 1 (1), 3758.
Hayek von, Friedrich A. (1960), The Constitution of Liberty. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Heide, Jan B. and George John (1990), "Alliances in Industrial Purchasing: The Determinants of Joint
Action in Buyer-Supplier Relationships," Journal of Marketing Research, XXVII (February), 2436.
Ho, D. Y. F. (1976), "On the Concept of Face," American Journal of Sociology, 81 (4), 867-84.
Hsu, Chuansi Stephen (1996), "Face": An Ethnographic Study of Chinese Social Behavior. Ann Arbor:
UMI.
Hunt, Shelby D. (1976a), "The Nature and Scope of Marketing," Journal of Marketing, 40 (July), 17-28.
---- (1976b), Marketing Theory: Conceptual Foundations of Research in Marketing. Columbus: Grid, Inc.
---- (1983), "General Theories and the Fundamental Explanada of Marketing," Journal of Marketing, 47
(Fall), 9-17.
---- (2000), A General Theory of Competition: Resources, Competencies, Productivity, Economic Growth.
Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc.
Hunt, Shelby D., C. Jay Lambe, and C. Michael Wittmann (2002), "A Theory and Model of Business
Alliance Success," Journal of Relationship Marketing, 1 (1), 17-36.
Klein, J. Katherine, Fred Dansereau, and Rosalie J. Hall (1994), "Levels Issues in Theory Development,
Data Collection and Analysis," Academy of Management Review, 19 (2), 195_229.
Koves, Andras and Paul Marer (1991), "Economic Liberalization in Eastern Europe and in Market
Economies," in Foreign Economic Liberalization: Transformations in Socialist and Market
Economies, Andras Koves and Paul Marer, Eds. Boulder: Westview Press.
334
Krugman, P. (1997), "Whatever Happened to the Asian Miracle?" Vol. 2002.
Kuhn, T. (1962), The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lane, Robert (1991), The Market Experience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lindblom, Charles E. (1977), Politics and Markets. N.Y.: Basic Books.
---- (2001), The Market System. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Lovett, Steve, Lee C Simmons, and Raja Kali (1999), "Guanxi Versus the Market: Ethics and
Efficiency," Journal of International Business Studies, 30 (2), 231-47.
Mayer, Roger C., James H. Davis, and David F. Schoorman (1995), "An Integrative Model of
Organizational Trust," The Academy of Management Review, 20 (3).
Morgan, Robert M. and Shelby D. Hunt (1994), "The Commitment Trust Theory of Relationship
Marketing," Journal of Marketing, 58 (July), 20-38.
Nee, V. (1989), "A Theory of Market Transition: From Redistribution to Markets in State Socialism,"
American Sociological Review, 54 (663-681).
---- (1992), "Organizational Dynamics of Market Transition: Hybrid Firms, Property Rights, and Mixed
Economy in China," Administrative Science Quarterly, 31, 1-27.
Palmer, Adrian (2002), "The Evolution of an Idea: An Environmental Explanation of Relationship
Marketing," Journal of Relationship Marketing, 1 (1), 79-97.
Parvatiyar, Atul and Jagdish N. Sheth (2000), "The Domain and Conceptual Foundations of Relationship
Marketing," in Handbook of Relationship Marketing, Jagdish N. Sheth and Atul Parvatiyar, Eds.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Pecotich, A., N. Renko, and C. J. Shultz (1994), "Yugoslav Disintegration, War and Consumption in
Croatia," in Research in Consumer Behavior, C. J. Shultz and R. Belk and G. Ger, Eds. Vol. 7.
Greenwich: JAI Press.
Pecotich, Anthony and Song Yang (2001), "Traditional Chinese Culture and Socialist Ideology in
Personal Selling: An Exploratory Evaluation," in Bridging Marketing Theory and Practice:
Proceedings of the Australian and New Zealand Marketing Academy Conference, Sylvie Chetty
and Brett Collins (Eds.). College of Business: Massey University,Albany, New Zealand.
Pecotich, Anthony and C. Shultz Eds. (1998), Marketing and Consumer Behaviour in East and SouthEast Asia. Sydney: McGraw-Hill Book Company Pty. Ltd.
Pecotich, Anthony and Hock Hwa Chia (2002), "Macromarketing and Traditional Chinese Wisdom for
Modern Business," in Macromarketing in the Asia Pacific Century: The Proceedings of the 27th
Annual Macromarkeiting Conference, Jack Cadeaux and Anthony Pecotich (Eds.). Sydney: the
University of New South Wales.
Popper, K. R. (1959), The Logic of Scientific Discovery. New York, NY: Basic Books.
335
Porter, Michael E. (1980), Competitive Strategy: Techniques for Analyzing Industries and Competitors.
New York: The Free Press.
---- (1985), Competitive Advantage. New York: The Free Press.
Rusbult, Caryl E. and Paul A. M. Van Lange (2003), "Interdependence, Interaction, and Relationships,"
Annual Review Psychology, 54, 351-75.
Scherer, F.M. (1980), Industrial Market Structure and Economic Performance. Chicago: Rand McNally
College Publishing Company.
Seung, Ho Park and Yadong Luo (2001), "Guanxi and Organizational Dynamics: Organizational
Networking in Chinese Firms," Strategic Management Journal, 22, 455–77.
Sheth, Jagdish N. and Atul Parvatiyar (2002), "Evolving Relationship Marketing into a Discipline,"
Journal of Relationship Marketing, 1 (1), 3-36.
Shultz, C. J. I. and A. Pecotich (1997), "Marketing and Development in the Transition Economies of
Southeast Asia: Policy Explication Assessment and Implications," Journal of Public Policy and
Marketing, 16 (1), 55-68.
Von Mises, Ludwig (1963), Human Action: A Treatise on Economics (Fourth Revised ed.). San
Francisco: Fox & Wilkes.
Wilson, Richard W., Amy Aurebacher Wilson, and Sidney L. Greenblatt Eds. (1979), Value Change in
Chinese Society. New York: Praeger Publishers.
Xin, Katherine R and Jone L Pearce (1996), "Guanxi: Connections as Substitutes for Formal Institutional
Support," Academy of Management Journa, 39 (6), 1641-58.
Yang, M. M. (1994), Gifts, Favors and Banquets: The Art of Social Relationships in China. Ithaca, NY:
Cornell University Press.
Yao, X. (2000), An Introduction to Confucianism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zheng, Yishan Ed. (1997), The Compilation of Three Religions: Confucianism, Taoism and Buddhism (in
Chinese). Taibei, Taiwan: Zheng Yishan Press.
336
Macromarketing and the Concept of Openness:
Toward a Research Agenda
Anthony Pecotich, University of Split, Croatia
Kenneth Beng Yap, University of Western Australia
Abstract
We attempt to develop a conceptual basis addressing a critical Macromarketing issue – the relationship
between openness and human welfare. In doing so we extend the economic literature to Macromarketing
and present a model that may serve as a basis for research.
Introduction
Socialism is evil because it disguises theft and makes robbery seem legitimate by passing
money through government hands. Tax money passed through the government is like money
laundered by the mob. Once tax money is collected by a government, it loses its identity, its
origins and, therefore, its true owners.
Capitalism is evil because it disguises theft and makes robbery seem legitimate by
passing money through government hands. Tax money passed through the government is like
money laundered by the mob. Once tax money is collected by a government, it loses its identity,
its origins and, therefore, its true owners.
Anonymous
Fiske (2001) captured the essential nature of macromarketing when he suggested that it was “marketing
with a heart.” In a similar vein although in a more general context Bartels (1976, p. 8) stated that:
“marketing thought can be regarded not less than as a portion of human intelligence and social
development at this time. It is more than a business technique, more than an economic function, more
than a social institution. Our way of thinking about marketing reflects our way of thinking about life.”
More formally macromarketing as a discipline had from its inception two defining elements macro
referring to “aggregations;” and “social welfare” (Bartels 1976; Fiske 1981; Hunt 1976b; Hunt 1976a;
Hunt 1981; Hunt and Barnett 1982; Meade and Nason 1982; Moyer and Hutt 1978; Shawer and Nickels
1981). By implication, therefore, two questions that are central to macromarketing are: (1) what
constitutes human welfare and (2) what is the best economic and social system for achieving it? It is our
purpose to take some basic steps towards developing a research agenda to explore these issues.
Background and Conceptual Development
Despite major changes in economic philosophy in the eastern block, the almost universal “move to the
market” (Asian Business 1992), globalization and economic integration, a small number of countries, and
a small proportion of people within them, still hold a disproportionate amount of economic wealth and
power, and the world continues to faces unprecedented conflict and disaster (Alba and Wesley 1987;
Mehanna 2004; Shenggen et al. 2004; World Health Organization 2004). The majority of the world’s
population still lives in poverty as many states continue to struggle out of economic destitution.
Governments of poorer nations claim that to their detriment, globalization has brought about a further
imbalance in economic equity (e.g., Economist 2004). Economists from patron institutions the World
337
Bank, International Monetary Fund and World Trade Organization have argued that for poor nations,
economic openness and liberalization are the necessary underpinnings of any successful reform agenda
and an increase in human welfare (e.g., World Trade Organization 2004). They go further to suggest that
the concept of openness entails transparency and receptivity to change or reform.
Openness towards trade has always been the focal point of the economic rhetoric propagated by these
international agencies. The argument presented is that since there are gains to be maximized from trade, a
country can improve its long-term economic standing by establishing or maintaining an open market that
is unencumbered by trade barriers (Economist 2003; Johansson 2002; Krugman and Obstfeld 2003b;
Terpstra and Sarathy 2000; World Trade Organization 2004). However, evidence of the causal link
between openness towards trade and economic development is sparse and hardly a complete or accurate
description of the state of affairs in many countries that continue to struggle economically. While this
contradiction warrants further discussion, there are other areas of investigation that would complement
and accompany the trade openness-development claim and this is the more general thesis of the all
encompassing benefits of intra/inter-national openness. It is our intention to attempt to sketch some of the
major positions and attempt to develop a preliminary basis for a possible research agenda.
A Brief Historical Overview
While the full historical sources may be found in ancient history (mostly perhaps of Greece and Rome),
and the nuances and controversy are both subtle and deep, strangely, among more modern economists,
from Marx to Freedman there seems a degree of agreement concerning the desirability of openness with
its sources in democratic principles and the right to dissent (e.g. Friedman 1962; Hayek von 1944, 1960;
Kuznets 1955; Lewis 1955; Mao 1967; Marx 1844/1969, 1967; Myrdal 1957; Popper 1943, a, 1943, b;
Ricardo 1911; Robertson 1935; Schumpeter 1942; Smith 1776; Von Mises 1963; Weber 1958;
Williamson and Buttrick 1954). Popper (1943, a; 1943, b) from a philosophers point of view presents a
strong argument for the development of a free society (“open society”) the key to which is the ability and
right of all to criticize those in authority without fear of punishment. His utopian vision saw the rise of an
open social and political organization which allows individuals to rise according to merit. Hayek and
other libertarian economists (e.g., Bartley 1988; Hayek von 1944, 1960; Von Mises 1963) provided an
economic view of this open society relying on freedom, liberty and the market system. Opposed to
equalitarianism that they believed unrealistic, the undefined metaphysical notion of competition and the
“invisible hand” play a critical role in their economic system. Hayek (Bartley 1988; Hayek von 1944,
1960) in particular relies on the market system, democratic processes including the constitution, division
of power and in particularly on the autonomy of the judiciary. The system, therefore, to function relies on
openness, integrity and a set of principles which the legislature can not subvert10. These historical
positions form the point of departure for our further development.
THE CONCEPT OF OPENNESS
Extant literature investigating openness has focused primarily on the economic dimension by way of
trade. However, the concept of openness is much broader and encompasses at least two of the following
attributes. Firstly, openness should infer mobility and freedom of entry and exit of material, organizations
and human resources. This attribute implies that public policies that govern industries and markets should
encourage competition and decentralization (or fragmentation) and discourage protectionism or
centralization. Secondly, openness should also infer an open exchange of ideas, skills and information,
which in turn includes a favorable disposition towards modernization and the adoption of new technology
and practices. This attribute implies that an “open society” would be one that is both dynamic and diverse,
notwithstanding the conventions of cultural traditions and practices. In the process of operationalizing the
10
It is perhaps of value to note the implied distrust of all governments in most of this literature - see the quote
above.
338
concept of openness of particular interest from the macromarketing point of view are any policies,
infrastructure, and cultural factors that instigate, propagate, and facilitate these two attributes of openness.
These factors could be used to measure the degree of openness within a country.
FIGURE 1
OPENNESS AND HUMAN WELFARE
External
economy
Power
dependency
Internal
economy
Objectiv e
indicators
Political
openness
Cultural
openness
Openness
(the open
society)
Human welfare
Subjective
indicators
Marketing
openess
EXTERNAL ECONOMIC OPENNESS
The issue of economic openness has been well researched by economists, but has been confined to two
constructs: the extent of trade and the extent of capital mobility (Dollar 1992; Feldstein and Horioka
1980; Grassman 1980; Obstfeld 1995; Vamvakidis 2002). Several proxy measures have been developed
for these constructs but no consensus has beeb reached on the best way to measure economic openness
(Krugman and Obstfeld 2003a; O'Rourke 2000). In our conceptual diagram (see figure 1) this variable is
shown under the broad label of “external economy” and is postulated to be an indicator of the broad
“openness” construct. We do this fully aware that the evidence of the link between economic openness
and economic development is inconclusive at best. While Frankel and Romer (1999) found that countries
that traded more freely had relatively higher growth rates, Vamvakidis (2002) did not find any support for
the relationship between openness of trade and growth. Similarly, scholars are divided on the relationship
between capital mobility and economic growth (Edwards 2002; Quinn 1997; Rodrik 1998). Anecdotal
observations serve to fuel these debates. Countries like Malaysia continue to experience economic growth
despite the imposition of capital restrictions. And even the world’s most economically developed nations
like the United States and countries in the European Union have often been criticized for exercising
protectionism while they advocate free trade in the spirit of neo-classical economics (Oxfam 2002).
The link between external economic openness and economic development forms the theoretical rhetoric
of many neo-classical reformists. However, there exists an alternate point of view that may provide a
basis for a criti
Download